Political crime and corruption take center stage: introductory notes
- Published: 17 March 2021
- Volume 75 , pages 195–200, ( 2021 )
Cite this article
- Mary Dodge ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-2662-8301 1 &
- Henry N. Pontell 2
5550 Accesses
3 Citations
1 Altmetric
Explore all metrics
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
The incidence of political crime, both known and unknown, is likely to exceed even the most unhinged estimates of what actually occurs in the elite world of white-collar criminality. In short, data on the exact number of crimes and unethical acts committed globally by politicians and corporations are unattainable. This problem exists due to the structurally veiled nature of political criminal behavior more generally. White-collar crime conducted by high-level officials often is hidden from public view, though, as one article documents in this issue, offenders may now more openly commit unspeakable acts that become normalized, by airing dirty laundry and situating crimes in the public domain as acceptable or inevitable. This special issue offers current perspectives on political corruption that represent singularly distinct but overlapping research and commentary on the insidious and global nature of white-collar crime.
Definitions of what political crime entails are controversial, which can make it difficult to pinpoint what happened, who was responsible, and why it occurred. Offenders and offenses present a wide array of wrongdoing and opportunities for analysis. Hence, white-collar crime researchers face perplexing issues when dealing with the complexities relating to both definitions and data access. These obstacles need to be overcome in order to provide better understandings of political corruption and the necessary means to combat it.
Political crime easily fits within the parameters of white-collar crime, though some offenses against the state may represent outliers. For example, in one major analysis Ross ( 2003 :2) emphasized that there remained “considerable confusion about what constitutes a political offense.” Conceptual issues in white-collar crime, as noted by scholars Shover and Wright [ 12 ], result in skirmishes over offender/offense distinctions and definitions that may make even the most experienced researchers squeamish. Political offenses include crimes against the state (e.g., insurrection, terrorism, sedition) and by the state (i.e., illegal or unethical acts by political figures). More common forms include bribery, ethical misconduct, election manipulations, graft, nepotism, looting, and misuse of financial resources. Political offending can also include war crimes, genocide and ethnic cleansing, espionage, assassinations, illegal experiments, terrorism, human rights violations, money laundering, theft, and wrongful incarceration [ 2 , 7 , 9 ]. In this issue, Joel Martinsson notes that discovering an “all-encompassing corruption definition would for corruption researchers be like finding the Holy Grail.”
Definitions of political crime are considered “broad and ill defined” and accusations of corruption can be all-encompassing and relatively common [ 1 ]: 201,[ 8 ]. Many scholars have adopted a state crime approach, though they readily concede that expansion of this term results in a definition that might include any “undesirable activities,” which, according to Sharkansky [ 11 ] represents an “intellectual transgression.” Friedrichs [ 5 ] also acknowledged the confusion surrounding state and governmental crimes with the latter representing a wide-range of misdeeds committed in a governmental context, often by an individual, and dependent on occupational status. The former, as defined by Friedrichs, represents actions carried out by the state itself. Geis and Meier [ 6 ] propose a rather straightforward definition of political crime that fits well-within traditional notions of white-collar criminality more generally,illegal actions committed by political officeholders in the course of their duties. An exhaustive exploration of definitional issues of political crime is beyond the scope of this introduction, but it can be emphasized that the concept continues to defy clear demarcations, and purposed definitions may result in a quagmire of incongruous offenses that are difficult to sort out accordingly.
Global jurisdictional issues, cover-ups, financial complexities, legal technicalities, political structures, and economic considerations also make attempts to research political crime difficult. The generally hidden nature of political crime involves acts by ruling elites that are clearly corrupt, yet fully ingrained in existing societal arrangements that act to thwart enforcement efforts, regulatory action, and judicial involvement. Most recently, the normalization of political malfeasance has resulted from the repeated “look at me” attitude and displays of Donald Trump as U.S. president.
Public complacency has developed from the number of overt illegal and immoral acts that are ignored, accepted, or unpunished. In this issue, Pontell, Tillman, and Ghazi-Tehrani offer a new perspective on the criminal debacle of Donald Trump’s presidency. The authors frame the newest “gate” as reminiscent of a publicly-aired Watergate scandal, and examine how the alleged offenses are flagrant acts enabled by the proliferation of a new lexicon of “fake news,” “cancel culture,” “cult of the personality,” “whataboutism,” and “not me” that assisted in rationalizing wrongdoing. Pontell, Tillman and Ghazi-Tehrani explore the Trump administration’s maneuvers to neutralize illegal and unethical acts in a public forum, making significant segments of the population more accepting of a wide range of political crimes. They also consider these moves as part of a larger existential war against white-collar and corporate crime in the United States, that not only sought to reduce regulatory capacity and enforcement efforts, but to normalize such offending itself.
Corruption in government is often explored using indices such as the World Bank’s Control of Corruption, and Transparency International’s Corruption Perception, and Global Integrity, which allow for quantitative comparisons across countries. Transparency International, established in mid-1990s, compiles information on corruption scandals involving “bribes and money laundering of epic proportions” (see Table 1 ). While particular cases are anecdotal, overall the criminal enterprises note the need for additional prosecution and preventive policies.
In many cases, political figures who reap the benefits of their ill-gotten gains have seized opportunities afforded to them by positions that feed their greed and inhumanity. In Peru, former president Alberto Fujimori, maintained a high public approval rating, despite bribing politicians and judges, employing death squads to kill guerillas, and embezzling at least US$600 million. Similarly, Ramzen Kadyrov, the head of Chechnya, used public funds to throw birthday parties and buy a US $2-million-dollar private boxing session with former world heavyweight boxing champion, Mike Tyson. While living in a palace with his pet tiger, Kadyrov ordered the killing of numerous rivals and committed a host of human rights violations [ 13 ].
Perhaps the most heinous political crimes documented by Transparency International are those involving collusion between governmental officials and companies [ 14 ]. Siemens Corporation, a German company, provoked a worldwide scandal by engaging in a bribery scheme with government officials and civil servants that involved approximately US$1.4 billion.
The extensive networks of political crime immersed in alliance with legitimate businesses is sometimes discovered only after a massive depletion of private and public funds. Research studies that focus on specific aspects of political crime continue to add impressive perspectives and endeavors to expand the extant literature. In this issue, Fiona Chan, Carole Gibbs, Rachel Boratto, and Mark Speers offer an innovative examination of transnational corporate bribery. The authors acknowledge the importance of employing a white-collar crime framework to research corruption and corporate bribery. Chan and colleagues’ work utilizes violation data of the Foreign Corrupt Practices Act from the US Department of Justice’s Fraud Section. The article offers a valuable and insightful analysis that establishes a theoretical and practical framework for exploring and researching corporate bribery.
Political crime is highly problematic in that the extent and nature of offenses act to destroy public trust and create feelings of helplessness in the populace. Indeed, identified corruption by a political leader may result in outrage and protests but the elite offender can create a safe haven for their actions by virtue of their position. In 2014, The Pew Research Center released the findings of a global study in which it found that a median of 83% of people across 34 emerging and developing economies responded that crime in their countries was a major problem, and that 76% saw corrupt political leaders in the same way. Additionally, corruption was perceived to be highest in Africa, followed by Ukraine, Russia, and China. Respondents from Latin America, Africa, Asia and the Middle East saw crime and corruption as the greatest problems in their countries. [ 10 ]. Despite such findings of citizen recognition of the problem, Ross [ 8 ] noted that the proliferation of corruption also “hampers public understanding” and results in “cynicism, skepticism, and complacency (p. 2).” Martinsson (this issue) cites a 2019 Pew Research Center survey finding that 81 percent of Americans distrust congress and 68 percent of the population in the European Union view corruption as a major problem.
Through detailed historical analysis, Børge Bakken and Jasmine Wang detail the growth of corruption in modern China. Their narrative of the influence of culturally embedded views identifies the changing forms of corruption in China and the connections between political elites and entrepreneurs. The authors adopt a critical perspective on white-collar crime in explaining how corruption and oppression is essential to exploitation that erodes public trust. The article places corruption in the contextual experiences of a single party autocracy that saw practices evolve that emphasized “decentralization without accountability,” resulting in increased wrongdoing at local levels.
Joel Martinsson’s article acknowledges that preventing corruption is problematic and that current approaches have created a false dichotomy. Martinsson emphasizes the need to look beyond the two popular frameworks—principle-agent and collective-action—in order to effectively prevent institutional corruption. He adopts a policy-centered approach toward systemic corruption that can address “honest graft” in established democracies. His portrayal of George Washington Plunkitt’s case ironically mirrors current events in the United States, as well as global problems that systematically reproduce political crimes.
Efforts to research sanctions and prevention efforts directed at political crime are challenging, especially across local and global jurisdictions. Carlos Novella-García and Alexis Cloquell-Lozano’s article addresses corruption in Spain. They employ a sophisticated analysis with a framework of Hermeneutic definitions of maxima and minima and social justice and offer prevention suggestions that stress the importance of public ethics as a mechanism for reducing corruption. They explore various theoretical approaches and provide a historical narrative for a case study of public corruption in Spain. When corruption is perceived as widespread, according to the authors, political and public ethics are needed to ensure that “the human condition is not easily corrupted by the act of wanting greater prestige, more power, and more money.”
Despite progress in the study of political crime and corruption, they still rage on today throughout the world and result in major societal losses. Moreover, like white-collar crime more generally, their social costs are physical as well as financial, or, as William Black has put it in challenging common business narratives, “corruption kills” [ 3 ]. Future research is warranted for both theory development, and prevention and control efforts.
Several major themes throughout the articles that follow are summarily captured by Ferrari who, a century ago, wrote:
This absolute contrast in the treatment of political criminals is due to a change of interpretation of their character and of their role from the point of view of the evolution of humanity. Whereas formerly the political criminal was treated as a public enemy, he is today considered as a friend of the public good, as a man of progress, desirous of bettering the political institutions of his country, having laudable intentions, hastening the onward march of humanity, his only fault being that he wishes to go too fast, and that he employs, in attempting to realize the progress which he desires, means irregular, illegal and violent (1920: 312).
Ferrari, despite the dearth of literature on white-collar crime at the time, further concluded that the “political criminal is reprehensible and ought to be punished in the interest of the established order, his criminality can- not be compared with that of the ordinary malefactor, with the murderer, the thief, etc.”
We extend our appreciation and thanks to all the authors who contributed to this valuable and timely issue.
Mary Dodge and Henry N. Pontell.
Allen, H. E., Friday, P. C., Roebuck, J. B., & Sagarin, E. (1981). Crime and punishment: An introduction to criminology (pp. 4–8). Free Press.
Google Scholar
Barak, G. (1991). Crimes by the capitalist state: An introduction to state criminality . State University of New York Press.
Black, W. K. (2007). Corruption kills. In H. N. Pontell & G. Geis (Eds.), International Handbook of White-Collar and Corporate Crime (pp. 439–455). Springer.
Chapter Google Scholar
Ferrari, R. (1920). Political crime. Columbia Law Review, 20, 308.
Article Google Scholar
Friedrichs, D. O. (2000). State crime or governmental crimes: Making sense of the conceptual confusion. In J. A. Ross (Ed.), Controlling state crime (pp. 53–80). Transaction Publishers.
Geis, G., & Meier, R. F. (1977). White-collar crime: Offenses in business, politics, and the professions (rev). Free Press.
Hagan, F. E. (1997). Political crime: Ideology and criminality . Allyn and Bacon.
Ross, J. A. (2003). The dynamics of political crime . Sage Publications.
Ross, J. A. (2000). Controlling state crime (2nd ed.). Transaction Publishers.
Pew Research Center (2014). Crime and corruption top problems in emerging and developing countries. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2014/11/06/crime-and-corruption-top-problems-in-emerging-and-developing-countries . Accessed 20 Feb 2021.
Sharkansky, I. (2000). A state action may be nasty but is not likely to be a crime. In J. A. Ross (Ed.), Controlling state crime (pp. 35–52). Transaction Publishers.
Shover, N., & Wright, J. P. (Eds.). (2001). Crimes of privilege: Readings in white-collar crime . Oxford University Press.
The Guardian (2015). Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/world/2015/sep/23/putins-closest-ally-and-his-biggest-liability2015/sep/23/putins-closest-ally-and-his-biggest-liability . Accessed 20 Feb 2021.
Transparency International (2019). Retrieved from https://www.transparency.org/en/news/25-corruption-scandals#Siemens . Accessed 20 Feb 2021.
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
University of Colorado Denver, Campus Box 142, P.O Box 173364, Denver, CO, 802017, USA
John Jay College of Criminal Justice, New York, NY, 10019, USA
Henry N. Pontell
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Mary Dodge .
Additional information
Publisher's note.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Dodge, M., Pontell, H.N. Political crime and corruption take center stage: introductory notes. Crime Law Soc Change 75 , 195–200 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10611-021-09950-5
Download citation
Accepted : 28 February 2021
Published : 17 March 2021
Issue Date : April 2021
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10611-021-09950-5
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- Find a journal
- Publish with us
- Track your research
The Rise of Political Violence in the United States
- Rachel Kleinfeld
Select your citation format:
Recent alterations to violent groups in the United States and to the composition of the two main political parties have created a latent force for violence that can be 1) triggered by a variety of social events that touch on a number of interrelated identities; or 2) purposefully ignited for partisan political purposes. This essay describes the history of such forces in the U.S., shares the risk factors for election violence globally and how they are trending in the U.S., and concludes with some potential paths to mitigate the problem.
O ne week after the 2020 U.S. presidential election, Eric Coomer, an executive at Dominion Voting Systems, was forced into hiding. Angry supporters of then-president Donald Trump, believing false accusations that Dominion had switched votes in favor of Joe Biden, published Coomer’s home address and phone number and put a million-dollar bounty on his head. Coomer was one of many people in the crosshairs. An unprecedented number of elections administrators received threats in 2020—so much so that a third of poll workers surveyed by the Brennan Center for Justice in April 2021 said that they felt unsafe and 79 percent wanted government-provided security. In July, the Department of Justice set up a special task force specifically to combat threats against election administrators. 1
From death threats against previously anonymous bureaucrats and public-health officials to a plot to kidnap Michigan’s governor and the 6 January 2021 attack on the U.S. Capitol, acts of political violence in the United States have skyrocketed in the last five years. 2 The nature of political violence has also changed. The media’s focus on groups such as the Proud Boys, Oath Keepers, and Boogaloo Bois has obscured a deeper trend: the “ungrouping” of political violence as people self-radicalize via online engagement. According to the National Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism (START), which maintains the Global Terrorism Database, most political violence in the United States is committed by people who do not belong to any formal organization.
About the Author
Rachel Kleinfeld is senior fellow in the Democracy, Conflict, and Governance Program at the Carnegie Endowment for International Peace. She was the founding CEO of the Truman National Security Project and serves on the National Task Force on Election Crises.
View all work by Rachel Kleinfeld
Instead, ideas that were once confined to fringe groups now appear in the mainstream media. White-supremacist ideas, militia fashion, and conspiracy theories spread via gaming websites, YouTube channels, and blogs, while a slippery language of memes, slang, and jokes blurs the line between posturing and provoking violence, normalizing radical ideologies and activities.
These shifts have created a new reality: millions of Americans willing to undertake, support, or excuse political violence, defined here (following the violence-prevention organization Over Zero) as physical harm or intimidation that affects who benefits from or can participate fully in political, economic, or sociocultural life. Violence may be catalyzed by predictable social events such as Black Lives Matter protests or mask mandates that trigger a sense of threat to a common shared identity. Violence can also be intentionally wielded as a partisan tool to affect elections and democracy itself. This organizational pattern makes stopping political violence more difficult, and also more crucial, than ever before.
Political Violence in the United States Historically
Political violence has a long history in the United States. Since the late 1960s, it was carried out by intensely ideological groups that pulled adherents out of the mainstream into clandestine cells, such as the anti-imperialist Weather Underground Organization or the anti-abortion Operation Rescue. In the late 1960s and 1970s, these violent fringes were mostly on the far left. They committed extensive violence, largely against property (with notable exceptions), in the name of social, environmental, and animal-rights causes. Starting in the late 1970s, political violence shifted rightward with the rise of white supremacist, anti-abortion, and militia groups. The number of violent events declined, but targets shifted from property to people—minorities, abortion providers, and federal agents.
What is occurring today does not resemble this recent past. Although incidents from the left are on the rise, political violence still comes overwhelmingly from the right, whether one looks at the Global Terrorism Database, FBI statistics, or other government or independent counts. 3 Yet people committing far-right violence—particularly planned violence rather than spontaneous hate crimes—are older and more established than typical terrorists and violent criminals. They often hold jobs, are married, and have children. Those who attend church or belong to community groups are more likely to hold violent, conspiratorial beliefs. 4 These are not isolated “lone wolves”; they are part of a broad community that echoes their ideas.
Two subgroups appear most prone to violence. The January 2021 American Perspectives Survey found that white Christian evangelical Republicans were outsized supporters of both political violence and the Q-Anon conspiracy, which claims that Democratic politicians and Hollywood elites are pedophiles who (aided by mask mandates that hinder identification) traffic children and harvest their blood; separate polls by evangelical political scientists found that in October 2020 approximately 47 percent of white evangelical Christians believed in the tenets of Q-Anon, as did 59 percent of Republicans. 5 Many evangelical pastors are working to turn their flocks away from this heresy. The details appear outlandish, but stripped to its core, the broad appeal becomes clearer: Democrats and cultural elites are often portrayed as Satanic forces arrayed against Christianity and seeking to harm Christian children.
The other subgroup prone to violence comprises those who feel threatened by either women or minorities. The polling on them is not clear. Separate surveys conducted by the American Enterprise Institute and academics in 2020 and 2021 found a majority of Republicans agreeing that “the traditional American way of life is disappearing so fast” that they “may have to use force to save it.” Respondents who believed that whites faced greater discrimination than minorities were more likely to agree. 6 Scholars Nathan Kalmoe and Lilliana Mason found that white Republicans with higher levels of minority resentment were more likely to see Democrats as evil or subhuman (beliefs thought to reduce inhibitions to violence). However, despite these feelings, the racially resentful did not stand out for endorsing violence against Democrats. Instead, the people most likely to support political violence were both Democrats and Republicans who espoused hostility toward women. 7 A sense of racial threat may be priming more conservatives to express greater resentment in ways that normalize violence and create a more permissive atmosphere, while men in both parties who feel particularly aggrieved toward women may be most willing to act on those feelings.
The bedrock idea uniting right-wing communities who condone violence is that white Christian men in the United States are under cultural and demographic threat and require defending—and that it is the Republican Party and Donald Trump, in particular, who will safeguard their way of life. 8 This pattern is similar to that of political violence in the nineteenth-century United States, where partisan identity was conflated with race, ethnicity, religion, and immigration status; many U.S.-born citizens felt they were losing cultural power and status to other social groups; and the violence was committed not by a few deviant outliers, but by many otherwise ordinary citizens engaged in normal civic life.
Changing social dynamics were the obvious spur for this violence, but it often yielded political outcomes. The ambiguity incentivized and enabled politicians to play with fire, deliberately provoking violence while claiming plausible deniability. In the 1840s and 1850s, from Maine and Maryland to Kentucky and Louisiana, the Know-Nothing party incited white Protestants to riot against mostly Catholic Irish and Italian immigrants (seen as both nonwhite and Democratic Party voters). When the Know-Nothings collapsed in 1855 in the North and 1860 in the South, anti-Catholic violence suddenly plummeted, despite continued bigotry. In the South, white supremacist violence was blamed on racism, but the timing was linked to elections. After the Supreme Court ruled in 1883 that the federal government lacked jurisdiction over racist terror, overturning the 1875 Civil Rights Act, violence became an open campaign strategy for the Democratic Party in multiple states. Lynchings were used in a similar manner. While proximate causes were social and economic, their time and place were primed by politics: Lynchings increased prior to elections in competitive counties. 9 Democratic Party politicians used racial rhetoric to amplify anger, then allowed violence to occur, to convince poor whites that they shared more in common with wealthy whites than with poor blacks, preventing the Populist and Progressive Parties from uniting poor whites and blacks into a single voting base. As Jim Crow laws enshrined Democratic one-party control, lynchings were not needed by politicians. Their numbers fell swiftly; they were no longer linked to elections. 10
Risk Factors for Election Violence
Globally, four factors elevate the risk of election-related violence, whether carried out directly by a political party through state security or armed party youth wings, outsourced to militias and gangs, or perpetrated by ordinary citizens: 1) a highly competitive election that could shift the balance of power; 2) partisan division based on identity; 3) electoral rules that enable winning by exploiting identity cleavages; and 4) weak institutional constraints on violence, particularly security-sector bias toward one group, leading perpetrators to believe they will not be held accountable for violence. 11
The rise of the Hindu-nationalist Bharatiya Janata Party (BJP) illustrates this dynamic. In 2002, a train fire killed Hindu pilgrims returning to Gujarat, India, from a contested site in Ayodhya. An anti-Muslim pogrom erupted. India’s current prime minister, the BJP’s Narendra Modi, was then chief minister of Gujarat. During three days of violence directed almost entirely against Muslims, he allowed the police to stand by and afterward refused to prosecute the rioters. The party won state legislative elections later that year by exploiting Hindu-Muslim tensions to pry Hindu voters from the Congress Party. The party has since stoked ethnic riots to win in contested areas across the country, and Modi reprised the strategy as prime minister. 12
In India’s winner-take-all electoral system, mob violence can potentially swing elections. Though fueled by social grievance, mob violence is susceptible to political manipulation. This is the form of electoral violence most like what the United States is experiencing, and it is particularly dangerous. Social movements have goals of their own. Though they may also serve partisan purposes, they can move in unintended directions and are hard to control.
Today, the risk factors for electoral violence are elevated in the United States, putting greater pressure on institutional constraints.
Highly competitive elections that could shift the balance of power: Heightened political competition is strongly associated with electoral violence. Only when outcomes are uncertain but close is there a reason to resort to violence. For much of U.S. history, one party held legislative power for decades. Yet since 1980, a shift in control of at least one house of Congress was possible—and since 2010, elections have seen a level of competition not seen since Reconstruction (1865–77). 13
Partisan division based on identity: Up to the 1990s, many Americans belonged to multiple identity groups—for example, a union member might have been a conservative, religious, Southern man who nevertheless voted Democratic. Today, Americans have sorted themselves into two broad identity groups: Democrats tend to live in cities, are more likely to be minorities, women, and religiously unaffiliated, and are trending liberal. Republicans generally live in rural areas or exurbs and are more likely to be white, male, Christian, and conservative. 14 Those who hold a cross-cutting identity (such as black Christians or female Republicans) generally cleave to the other identities that align with their partisan “tribe.”
As political psychologist Lilliana Mason has shown, greater homogeneity within groups with fewer cross-cutting ties allows people to form clearer in- and out-groups, priming them for conflict. When many identities align, belittling any one of them can trigger humiliation and anger. Such feelings are heightened by policy differences but are not about policy; they are personal, and thus are more powerful. These real cultural and belief differences are at the heart of the cultural conflicts in the United States.
U.S. party and electoral institutions are intensifying rather than reducing these identity cleavages. The alignment of racial and religious identity with political party is not random. Sorting began after the passage of the Civil Rights Act in 1965 as whites who disagreed with racial equality fled the Democratic Party. A second wave—the so-called Reagan Democrats, who had varied ideological motivations, followed in 1980 and 1984. A third wave, pushed away from the Democratic Party by the election of Barack Obama and attracted by Trump’s 2016 presidential campaign, drew previous swing voters who were particularly likely to define “Americanness” as white and Christian into the Republican Party. 15
A 2016 Pew Research Center poll found that 32 percent of U.S. citizens believed that to be a “real American,” one must be a U.S.-born Christian. But among Trump’s primary voters, according to a 2017 Voter Study Group analysis, 86 percent thought it was “very important” to have been born in the United States; 77 percent believed that one must be Christian; and 47 percent thought one must also be “of European descent.” 16 According to Democracy Fund voter surveys, during the 2016 primaries, many economic conservatives, libertarians, and other traditional Republican groups did not share these views on citizenship. By 2020, however, white identity voters made up an even larger share of the Republican base. Moreover, their influence is greater than their numbers because in the current U.S. context—where identities are so fixed and political polarization is so intense—swing voters are rare, so it is more cost-effective for campaigns to focus on turning out reliable voters. The easiest way to do this is with emotional appeals to shared identities rather than to policies on which groups may disagree. 17 This is true for both Republicans and Democrats.
The Democratic Party’s base, however, is extremely heterogeneous. The party must therefore balance competing demands—for example, those of less reliable young “woke” voters with those of highly reliable African American churchgoers, or those of more-conservative Mexican American men with those of progressive activists. In contrast, the Republican Party is increasingly homogenous, which allows campaigns to target appeals to white, Christian, male identities and the traditional social hierarchy.
The emergence of large numbers of Americans who can be prompted to commit political violence by a variety of social events is thus partially an accidental byproduct of normal politics in highly politically sorted, psychologically abnormal times. Even in normal times, people more readily rally to their group’s defense when it is under attack, which is why “ they are out to take your x” is such a time-honored fundraising and get-out-the-vote message. Usually, such tactics merely heighten polarization. But when individuals and societies are highly sorted and stressed, the effects can be much worse. Inequality and loneliness, which were endemic in the United States even before the covid-19 pandemic and have only gotten worse since, are factors highly correlated with violence and aggression. Contagious disease, meanwhile, has led to xenophobic violence historically.
The confluence of these factors with sudden social-distancing requirements, closures of businesses and public spaces, and unusually intrusive pandemic-related government measures during an election year may have pushed the more psychologically fragile over the edge. Psychologists have found that when more homogenous groups with significant overlap in their identities face a sense of group threat, they respond with deep anger. Acting on that anger can restore a sense of agency and self-esteem and, in an environment in which violence is justified and normalized, perhaps even win social approval. 18
The sorts of racially coded political messages that have been in use for decades will be received differently in a political party whose composition has altered to include a greater percentage of white identity voters. Those who feel that their dominant status in the social hierarchy is under attack may respond violently to perceived racial or other threats to their status at the top. But those lower on the social ladder may also resort to violence to assert dominance over (and thus psychological separation from) those at the bottom—for example, minority men over women or other minorities, one religious minority over another, or white women over minority women. Antisemitism is growing among the young, and exists on the left, but is far stronger on the right, and is particularly salient among racial minorities who lean right. 19 On the far-left, violent feelings are emerging from the same sense of group threat and defense, but in mirror-image: Those most willing to dehumanize the right are people who see themselves as defending racial minorities.
Republicans and Democrats have been espousing similar views on the acceptability of violence since 2017, when Kalmoe and Mason began collecting monthly data.
Between 2017 and 2020, Democrats and Republicans were extremely close in justifying violence, with Democrats slightly more prone to condone violence—except in November 2019, the month before Trump’s first impeachment, when Republican support for violence spiked. Both sides also expressed similarly high levels of dehumanizing thought: 39 percent of Democrats and 41 percent of Republicans saw the other side as “downright evil,” and 16 percent of Democrats and 20 percent of Republicans said that their opponents were “like animals.” Such feelings can point to psychological readiness for violence. Separate polling found lower but still comparable levels: 4 percent of Democrats and 3 percent of Republicans believed in October 2020 that attacks on their political opponents would be justified if their party leader alleged the election was stolen; 6 percent of Democrats and 4 percent of Republicans believed property damage to be acceptable in such a case. 20
The parallel attitudes suggest that partisan sorting and social pressures were working equally on all Americans, although Republicans may have greater tolerance for online threats and harassment of opponents and opposition leaders. 21 Yet actual incidents of political violence, while rising on both sides, have been vastly more prevalent on the right. Why has the right been more willing to act on violent feelings?
The clue lies in the sudden shift in attitudes in October 2020, when after maintaining similarity for years, Republicans’ endorsements of violence suddenly leapt across every one of Kalmoe and Mason’s questions regarding the acceptability of violence; findings that were repeated in other polling. 22 In January 2020, 41 percent of Republicans agreed that “a time will come when patriotic Americans have to take the law into their own hands”; a year later, after the January 6 insurrection, 56 percent of Republicans agreed that “if elected leaders will not protect America, the people must do it themselves even if it requires taking violent action.” 23 Moral disengagement also spiked: By February 2021, more than two-thirds of Republicans (and half of Democrats) saw the other party as “downright evil,”; while 12 percent more Republicans believed Democrats were less than human than the other way around. 24
The false narrative of a stolen 2020 election clearly increased support for political violence. Those who believed the election was fraudulent were far more likely to endorse coups and armed citizen rebellion; by February 2021, a quarter of Republicans felt that it was at least “a little” justified to take over state government buildings with violence to advance their political goals. 25 This politically driven false narrative points to the role of politicians since 2016 in fueling the difference in violence between right and left. As has been found in Israel and Germany, domestic terrorists are emboldened by the belief that politicians encourage violence or that authorities will tolerate it. 26
It is not uncommon for politicians to incite communal violence to affect electoral outcomes. In northern Kenya, voters call this “war by remote control.” Incumbent leaders who fear losing are particularly prone to using electoral violence to intimidate potential opponents, build their base, affect voting behavior and election-day vote counts, and, failing all that, to keep themselves relevant or at least out of jail. 27 Communal violence can clear opposition voters from contested areas, altering the demographics of electoral districts, as happened in Kenya’s Rift Valley in 2007 and the U.S. South during Reconstruction. Violent intimidation can keep voters away from the polls, as has occurred since the 1990s in Bangladesh; from the 1990s through 2013 in Pakistan; and in the U.S. South in the 1960s.
Communal violence often flares in contested districts where it is politically expedient, as in Kenya and India. Likewise, political violence in the United States has been greatest in suburbs where Asian American and Hispanic American immigration has been growing fastest, particularly in heavily Democratic metropoles surrounded by Republican-dominated rural areas. These areas, where white flight from the 1960s is meeting demographic change, are areas of social contestation. They are also politically contested swing districts. Most of the arrested January 6 insurrectionists hailed from these areas rather than from Trump strongholds. 28 Postelection violence can also be useful to politicians. They can manipulate angry voters who believe their votes were stolen into using violence to influence or block final counts or gain leverage in power-sharing negotiations, as occurred in Kenya in 2007 and Afghanistan in 2019.
Not all political violence directly serves an electoral purpose. Using violence to defend a group bonds members to the group. Thus violence is a particularly effective way to build voter “intensity.” In 1932, young black-clad militants of the British Union of Fascists roamed England’s streets, picking fights and harassing Jews. The leadership of the nascent party realized that its profile grew whenever the “blackshirts” got into violent confrontations. Two years later, the party held a rally of nearly fifteen-thousand people that became a brutal melee between blackshirts and antifascist protestors. After the clash (which was not fully spontaneous), people queued to join the party for the next two days and nights and membership soared. 29 As every organizer knows, effective mobilization requires keeping supporters engaged. Given the role of gun rights to Republican identity, armed rallies can mobilize supporters and expand fundraising. Yet even peaceful rallies of crowds carrying automatic weapons can intimidate people who hold opposing views.
Finally, politicians may personally benefit from violent mobilization that is not election-related. In South Africa, former president Jacob Zuma spent years cultivating ties with violent criminal groups in his home state of Kwa-Zulu Natal. 30 When he was out of office and on trial for corruption and facing jail time for contempt of court, he activated those connections to spur a round of violence and looting on a scale not seen in South Africa since apartheid. Vast inequality, unemployment, and other social causes allowed for plausible deniability—many looters with no political ties were just joining in the fracas. Zuma has, as of this writing, avoided imprisonment due to undisclosed “medical reasons.”
Electoral rules enable winning by exploiting identity cleavages: The fissures in divided societies such as the United States can be either mitigated or enhanced by electoral systems. The U.S. electoral system comprises features that are correlated with greater violence globally. Winner-take-all elections are particularly prone to violence, possibly because small numbers of voters can shift outcomes. Two-party systems are also more correlated with violence than are multiparty systems, perhaps because they create us-them dynamics that deepen polarization. 31 Although multiparty systems allow more-extreme parties to gain representation, such as Alternative for Germany or Golden Dawn in Greece, they also enable other parties to work together against a common threat. The U.S. system is more brittle. A two-party system can prevent the representation of fringe views, as occurred for years in the United States—for example, American Independent Party candidate George Wallace won 14 percent of the popular vote in 1968 but no representation. Yet because party primaries tend to be low-turnout contests with highly partisan voters, small factions can gain outsized influence over a mainstream party. If that happens, extreme politicians can gain control over half of the political spectrum—leaving that party’s voters nowhere to turn.
Weak institutional constraints on violence: The United States suffers from three particularly concerning institutional weaknesses today—the challenge of adjudicating disputes between the executive and legislative branches inherent in presidential majoritarian systems, recent legal decisions enhancing the electoral power of state legislatures, and the politicization of law enforcement and the courts.
Juan Linz famously noted that apart from the United States, few presidential majoritarian systems had survived as continuous democracies. One key reason was the problem of resolving disputes between the executive and legislative branches. Because both are popularly elected, when they are held by different parties stalemates between the two invite resolution through violence. Such a dynamic drove state-level electoral violence throughout the nineteenth century, not only in the Reconstruction South, but also in Pennsylvania, Maine, Rhode Island, and Colorado. It is thus particularly concerning that in the last year, nine states have passed laws to give greater power to partisan bodies, particularly state legislatures. 32 The U.S. Supreme Court has also made several recent decisions vesting greater power over elections in state legislatures. These trends are weakening institutional guardrails against future political violence.
When law and justice institutions are believed to lean toward one party or side of an identity cleavage, political violence becomes more likely. International cases reveal that groups that believe they can use violence without consequences are more likely to do so. The U.S. justice system, police, and military are far more professional and less politicized than those of most developing democracies that face widespread electoral violence. Longstanding perceptions that police favor one side are supported by Armed Conflict Location and Event Data Project (ACLED) data showing that police used far greater force at left-wing protests than at right-wing protests throughout 2020. Despite this conservative ideological tilt, party affiliation and feelings were more complicated: Law enforcement was also a target of right-wing militias, and partisan affiliation (based on donations) had previously been mixed due to union membership and other cross-cutting identities that connected police to the Democratic Party. In 2020, however, donations from individual law enforcement officers to political parties increased, and they tilted far toward the Republican Party, suggesting that the polarizing events of 2020 have led them to sort themselves to the right and deepen their partisanship. 33
How to Counter the Trends
Interventions in five key areas could help defuse the threat of political violence in the United States: 1) election credibility, 2) electoral rules, 3) policing, 4) prevention and redirection, and 5) political speech. The steps best taken depend on who is in power and who is committing the violence. Technical measures to enhance election credibility and train police can reduce inadvertent violence from the state. But such technical solutions will fail if the party in power is inciting violence, as happens more often than not. In that case, behind-the-scenes efforts to help parties and leaders strike deals or mediate grievances can sometimes keep violence at bay. In Kenya, for instance, two opposing politicians accused of leading election violence in 2007 joined forces to run as president and vice-president; their alliance enabled a peaceful election in 2013. Ironically, strong institutions, low levels of corruption, and reductions in institutionalized methods of elite deal-making (such as Congressional earmarks) make such bargains more difficult in the United States. However, the United States is helped by its unusually high level of federalism in terms of elections and law enforcement, because if one part of “the state” is acting against reform, it may still be possible at another level.
More credible elections: While there was no widespread fraud in the 2020 U.S. elections, international election experts agree that the U.S. electoral system is antiquated and prone to failure. The proposed Freedom to Vote Act, which enhances cybersecurity, protects election officers, provides a paper trail for ballots, and provides proper training and funding for election administration, among other measures, could offer the sort of bipartisan compromise that favors neither side and would shore up a problematic system. But if it is turned into a political cudgel, as is likely, it will fail to reassure voters, despite its excellent provisions.
Changing the electoral rules: Whether politicians use violence as a campaign strategy is shaped by the nature of the electoral system. A seminal study on India by Steven Wilkinson suggests that where politicians need minority votes to win, they protect minorities; where they do not, they are more likely to incite violence. 34 By this logic, Section 2 of the U.S. Voting Rights Act of 1965, which allows for gerrymandering majority-minority districts to ensure African American representation in Congress, may inadvertently incentivize violence by making minority votes unnecessary for Republican wins in the remaining districts . While minority representation is its own valuable democratic goal, creating districts where Republicans need minority votes to win—and where Democrats need white votes to win—might reduce the likelihood of violence.
Whether extremists get elected and whether voters feel represented or become disillusioned with the peaceful process of democracy can also be affected by electoral-system design. Thus postconflict countries often redesign electoral institutions. For example, a major plank of the 1998 Good Friday Agreement that ended the Troubles in Northern Ireland involved introducing a type of ranked-choice voting with multimember districts to increase a sense of representation. There are organizations in the United States today that are advocating various reform measures—for example, eliminating primaries and introducing forms of ranked-choice voting or requiring lawmakers to win a majority of votes to be elected (currently the case in only a handful of states)—that could result in fewer extremists gaining power while increasing voter satisfaction and representation.
Fairer policing and accountability: Even in contexts of high polarization, external deterrence and societal norms generally keep people from resorting to political violence. Partisans who are tempted to act violently should know that they will be held accountable, even if their party is in power. Minority communities, meanwhile, need assurance that the state will defend them.
A number of police-reform measures could help. Police training in de-escalation techniques and nonviolent protest and crowd control, support for officers under psychological strain, improved intelligence collection and sharing regarding domestic threats, and more-representative police forces would all help deter both political violence and police brutality. Publicizing such efforts would demonstrate to society that the government will not tolerate political violence.
Meanwhile, swift justice for violence, incitement, and credible threats against officials—speedy jail sentences, for instance, even if short—is also crucial for its signaling and deterrent value. So are laws that criminalize harassment, intimidation, and political violence.
Prevention and redirection: Lab experiments have found that internal norms can be reinforced by “inoculating” individuals with warnings that people may one day try to indoctrinate them to extremist beliefs or recruit them to participate in acts of political violence. Because no one likes to be manipulated, the forewarned organize their mental defenses against it. The technique seems promising for preventing younger people from radicalization, though it requires more testing among older partisans whose beliefs are strongly set. 35
A significant portion of those engaged in far-right violence are also under mental distress. People searching online for far-right violent extremist content are 115 percent more likely to click on mental-health ads; those undertaking planned hate crimes show greater signs of mental illness than does the general offender population. 36 Groups such as Moonshot CVE are experimenting with targeted ads that can redirect people searching for extremist content toward hotlines for depression and loneliness and help for leaving violent groups.
Political speech: When political leaders denounce violence from their own side, partisans listen. Experiments using quotes from Biden and Trump show that leaders’ rhetoric has the power to de-escalate and deter violence—if they are willing to speak against their own side. 37
Long-term trends in social and political-party organization, isolation, distrust, and inequality, capped by a pandemic, have placed individual psychological health and social cohesion under immense strain. Kalmoe and Mason’s surveys found that in February 2021, a fifth of Republicans and 13 percent of Democrats—or more than 65 million people—believed immediate violence was justified. Even if only a tiny portion are serious, such large numbers put a country at risk of stochastic terrorism—that is, it becomes statistically near certain that someone (though it is impossible to predict who) somewhere will act if a public figure incites violence.
Thus while social factors may have created the conditions, politicians have the match to light the tinder. In recent years, some candidates on the right have been particularly willing to use violent speech and engage with groups that spread hate. Yet Democrats are not immune to these trends. Far-left violence is far lower than on the right, but rising. The firearm industry’s trade association found that, in 2020, 40 percent of all legal gun sales were to first-time buyers, and 58 percent of those five-million new owners were women and African Americans. 38 Kalmoe and Mason’s February 2020 polling found that 11 percent of Democrats and 12 percent of Republicans agreed that it was at least “a little” justified to kill opposing political leaders to advance their own political goals. With both the left and the right increasingly armed, viewing the other side as evil or subhuman, and believing political violence to be justified, the possibility grows of tit-for-tat street warfare, like the clashes between antifascist protesters and Proud Boys in Portland, Oregon, from 2020 through this writing. If Democrats have been less likely to act on these beliefs, it is likely because Democratic politicians have largely and vocally spoken out against violence.
Although political violence in the United States is on the rise, it is still lower than in many other countries. Once violence begins, however, it fuels itself. Far from making people turn away in horror, political violence in the present is the greatest factor normalizing it for the future. Preventing a downward spiral is therefore imperative.
1. “Election Officials Under Attack: How to Protect Administrators and Safeguard Democracy,” Brennan Center for Justice, 16 June 2021, www.brennancenter.org/sites/default/files/2021-06/BCJ-129%20ElectionOfficials_v7.pdf ; “Documenting and Addressing Harassment of Election Officials,” California Voter Foundation, June 2021, www.calvoter.org/sites/default/files/cvf_addressing_harassment_of_election_officials_report.pdf ; Zach Montellaro, “‘Center of the Maelstrom’: Election Officials Grapple with 2020’s Long Shadow,” Politico, 18 August 2021.
2. See the Global Terrorism Database maintained by the National Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism at the University of Maryland, the dataset of extremist far-right violent incidents maintained by Arie Perliger at the University of Massachusetts, Lowell, and FBI data on hate crimes.
3. Robert O’Harrow, Jr., Andrew Ba Tran, and Derek Hawkins, “The Rise of Domestic Extremism in America,” Washington Post, 12 April 2021.
4. Robert Pape and the Chicago Project on Security and Threats, “Understanding American Domestic Terrorism: Mobilization Potential and Risk Factors of a New Threat Trajectory” (presentation, University of Chicago, 6 April 2021), https://d3qi0qp55mx5f5.cloudfront.net/cpost/i/docs/americas_insurrectionists_online_2021_04_06.pdf?mtime=1617807009.
5. Daniel A. Cox, “Social Isolation and Community Disconnection Are Not Spurring Conspiracy Theories,” Survey Center on American Life, 4 March 2021, www.americansurveycenter.org/research/social-isolation-and-community-disconnection-are-not-spurring-conspiracy-theories ; Paul A. Djupe and Ryan P. Burge, “A Conspiracy at the Heart of It: Religion and Q,” Religion in Public blog, 6 November 2020, https://religioninpublic.blog/2020/11/06/a-conspiracy-at-the-heart-of-it-religion-and-q .
6. Daniel A. Cox, “After the Ballots Are Counted: Conspiracies, Political Violence, and American Exceptionalism,” Survey Center on American Life, 11 February 2021, www.americansurveycenter.org/research/after-the-ballots-are-counted-conspiracies-political-violence-and-american-exceptionalism ; Larry M. Bartels, “Ethnic Antagonism Erodes Republicans’ Commitment to Democracy,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117 (September 2020): 22752–59.
7. Nathan P. Kalmoe and Lilliana Mason, Radical American Partisanship: Mapping Violent Hostility, Its Causes, and the Consequences for Democracy (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, forthcoming [2022]), 105, 109.
8. Daniel A. Cox, “Support for Political Violence Among Americans Is on the Rise. It’s a Grim Warning About America’s Political Future,” American Enterprise Institute, 26 March 2021, www.aei.org/op-eds/support-for-political-violence-among-americans-is-on-the-rise-its-a-grim-warning-about-americas-political-future ; Bartels, “Ethnic Antagonism Erodes Republicans’ Commitment to Democracy”; Sonia Roccas and Marilynn B. Brewer, “Social Identity Complexity,” Personality and Social Psychology Review 6 (May 2002): 86–102.
9. Susan Olzak, “The Political Context of Competition: Lynching and Urban Racial Violence, 1882–1914,” Social Forces 69 (December 2020): 395–421; Ryan Hagen, Kinga Makovi, and Peter Bearman, “The Influence of Political Dynamics on Southern Lynch Mob Formation and Lethality,” Social Forces 92 (December 2013): 757-87.
10. Brad Epperly, Christopher Witko, Ryan Strickler, and Paul White, “Rule by Violence, Rule by Law: Lynching, Jim Crow, and the Continuing Evolution of Voter Suppression in the U.S.,” Perspectives on Politics 18 (September 2020): 756-69.
11. Sarah Birch, Ursula Daxecker, and Kristine Hӧglund, “Electoral Violence: An Introduction,” Journal of Peace Research 57 (January 2020): 3–14.
12. Steven I. Wilkinson, Votes and Violence: Electoral Competition and Ethnic Riots in India (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2004).
13. Frances E. Lee, Insecure Majorities : Congress and the Perpetual Campaign (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2016).
14. Lilliana Mason, Uncivil Agreement: How Politics Became Our Identity (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2018).
15. Tyler T. Reny, Loren Collingwood, and Ali A. Valenzuela, “Vote Switching in the 2016 Election: How Racial and Immigration Attitudes, Not Economics, Explain Shifts in White Voting,” Public Opinion Quarterly 83 (Spring 2019): 91–113; John Sides, Michael Tesler, and Lynn Vavreck, Identity Crisis: The 2016 Presidential Campaign and the Battle for the Meaning of America (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2018).
16. Bruce Stokes, “What It Takes to Truly Be ‘One of Us,’” Pew Research Center, 1 February 2017, www.pewresearch.org/global/2017/02/01/what-it-takes-to-truly-be-one-of-us ; Emily Ekins, “The Five Types of Trump Voters: Who They Are and What They Believe,” Democracy Fund Voter Study Group, June 2017, www.voterstudygroup.org/publication/the-five-types-trump-voters .
17. Costas Panagopoulos, “All About That Base: Changing Campaign Strategies in U.S. Presidential Elections,” Party Politics 22 (March 2016): 179–90.
18. Pablo Fajnzylber, Daniel Lederman, and Norman Loayza, “Inequality and Violent Crime,” Journal of Law and Economics 45 (April 2002): 1–39; James V. P. Check, Daniel Perlman, and Neil M. Malamuth, “Loneliness and Aggressive Behavior,” Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 2 (September 1985): 243–52; Mark Schaller and Justin H. Park, “The Behavioral Immune System (and Why It Matters),” Current Directions in Psychological Science 20 (April 2011): 99–103.
19. Eitan Hersh and Laura Royden, “Antisemitic Attitudes Across the Ideological Spectrum,” 9 April 2021, unpubl ms., www.eitanhersh.com/uploads/7/9/7/5/7975685/hersh_royden_antisemitism_040921.pdf .
20. Kalmoe and Mason, Radical American Partisanship ; Noelle Malvar et al., “Democracy for President: A Guide to How Americans Can Strengthen Democracy During a Divisive Election,” More in Common, October 2020, https://democracyforpresident.com/topics/election-violence.
21. The Democracy Fund’s 2019 VOTER Survey shows 10-point gaps for each in December 2019; however, monthly Kalmoe and Mason polling shows no gap, and Bright Line Watch polling in 2020 shows splits of less than 6 and 3 percent for identically worded questions.
22. Kalmoe and Mason, Radical American Partisanship, 83–90.
23. Bartels, “Ethnic Antagonism Erodes Republicans’ Commitment to Democracy”; Cox, “Support for Political Violence.”
24. Kalmoe and Mason, Radical American Partisanship, 86.
25. Kalmoe and Mason, Radical American Partisanship, 164, 90.
26. Arie Perliger, “Terror Isn’t Always a Weapon of the Weak—It Can Also Support the Powerful,” The Conversation, 28 October 2018, https://theconversation.com/terror-isnt-always-a-weapon-of-the-weak-it-can-also-support-the-powerful-82626 .
27. Ken Menkhaus, Conflict Assessment: Northern Kenya and Somaliland (Copenhagen: Danish Deming Group, 2015), 42; Thad Dunning, “Fighting and Voting: Violent Conflict and Electoral Politics,” Journal of Conflict Resolution 55 (June 2011): 327–39.
28. Arie Perliger, “Why Do Hate Crimes Proliferate in Progressive Blue States?” Medium, 20 August 2020, https://medium.com/3streams/why-hate-crimes-proliferate-in-progressive-blue-state-72483b2d72a7 ; Pape and Chicago Project, “Understanding American Domestic Terrorism.”
29. Martin Pugh, “The British Union of Fascists and the Olympia Debate,” Historical Journal 41 (June 1998): 529–42.
30. Gavin Evans, “Why Political Killings Have Taken Hold—Again—In South Africa’s KwaZulu-Natal,” The Conversation, 10 August 2020, https://theconversation.com/why-political-killings-have-taken-hold-again-in-south-africas-kwazulu-natal-143908 .
31. G. Bingham Powell, Jr., “Party Systems and Political System Performance: Voting Participation, Government Stability and Mass Violence in Contemporary Democracies,” American Political Science Review 75, no. 4 (1981): 861–79; Hanne Fjelde and Kristine Höglund, “Electoral Institutions and Electoral Violence in Sub-Saharan Africa,” British Journal of Political Science 46 (April 2016): 297–320.
32. Quinn Scanlan, “10 New State Laws Shift Power Over Elections to Partisan Entities,” ABC News, 16 August 2021, https://abcnews.go.com/Politics/dozen-state-laws-shift-power-elections-partisan-entities/story?id=79408455 ; “Democracy Crisis Report Update: New Data and Trends Show the Warning Signs Have Intensified in the Last Two Months,” States United Democracy Center, Protect Democracy, and Law Forward, 10 June 2021, https://statesuniteddemocracy.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/06/Democracy-Crisis-Part-II_June-10_Final_v7.pdf .
33. Phillip Bump, “Police Made a Lot More Contributions in 2020 Than Normal—Mostly to Republicans,” Washington Post, 25 February 2021.
34. Wilkinson, Votes and Violence.
35. Kurt Braddock, “Vaccinating Against Hate: Using Inoculation to Confer Resistance to Persuasion by Extremist Propaganda,” Terrorism and Political Violence (2019), 1–23.
36. “Mental Health and Violent Extremism,” Moonshot CVE, 28 June 2018, https://moonshotteam.com/mental-health-violent-extremism ; Profiles of Individual Radicalization in the United States (PIRUS) dataset, Global Terrorism Database.
37. Kalmoe and Mason, Radical American Partisanship, 180-87; Matthew A. Baum and Tim Groeling, “Shot by the Messenger: Partisan Cues and Public Opinion Regarding National Security and War,” Political Behavior 31 (June 2009): 157–86; Susan Birch and David Muchlinski, “Electoral Violence Prevention: What Works?” Democratization 25 (April 2018): 385–403.
38. “First-Time Gun Buyers Grow to Nearly 5 Million in 2020,” Fire Arms Industry Trade Association, 24 August 2020, www.nssf.org/articles/first-time-gun-buyers-grow-to-nearly-5-million-in-2020 .
Copyright © 2021 National Endowment for Democracy and Johns Hopkins University Press
Image Credit: lev radin/Shutterstock.com.
Further Reading
Volume 35, Issue 2
America’s Crisis of Civic Virtue
- Arthur C. Brooks
The problem for democracy today is not capitalism; it is a decline in public honesty and civility. But there is an opportunity to revive our sense of national community, if…
Volume 35, Issue 4
Liberal Democracy in an Age of Immigration
- Rafaela Dancygier
Immigration threatens to erode liberalism, as far-right parties and migrant communities with illiberal views gain power. Mass publics have shouldered the blame. But should political elites be held responsible?
The Liberalism of Refuge
- Bryan Garsten
Liberal societies are those which offer refuge from the very people they empower—through individual choice, mobility, and the possibility of exit. This is the form of liberty that most clearly…
- Tools and Resources
- Customer Services
- Corrections
- Crime, Media, and Popular Culture
- Criminal Behavior
- Criminological Theory
- Critical Criminology
- Geography of Crime
- International Crime
- Juvenile Justice
- Prevention/Public Policy
- Race, Ethnicity, and Crime
- Research Methods
- Victimology/Criminal Victimization
- White Collar Crime
- Women, Crime, and Justice
- Share Facebook LinkedIn Twitter
Article contents
Political corruption and state crime.
- Clayton Peoples Clayton Peoples Department of Social Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno
- , and James E. Sutton James E. Sutton Department of Anthropology and Sociology, Hobart and William Smith Colleges
- https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190264079.013.274
- Published online: 26 April 2017
The state is responsible for maintaining law and order in society and protecting the people. Sometimes it fails to fulfill these responsibilities; in other cases, it actively harms people. There have been many instances of political corruption and state crime throughout history, with impacts that range from economic damage to physical injury to death—sometimes on a massive scale (e.g., economic recession, pollution/poisoning, genocide). The challenge for criminologists, however, is that defining political corruption and state crime can be thorny, as can identifying their perpetrators—who can often be collectives of individuals such as organizations and governments—and their victims. In turn, pinpointing appropriate avenues of controlling these crimes can be difficult. These challenges are exacerbated by power issues and the associated reality that the state is in a position to write or change laws and, in essence, regulate itself. One possible solution is to define political corruption and state crime—as well as their perpetrators and victims—as broadly as possible to include a variety of scenarios that may or may not exhibit violations of criminal law. Likewise, a resolution to the issue of social control would be to move beyond strictly institutional mechanisms of control. Criminological research should further elucidate these issues; it should also, however, move beyond conceptual dilemmas toward (a) better understanding the processes underlying political corruption/state crime and (b) illustrating the broader ramifications of these crimes.
- political corruption
- state crime
- state-corporate crime
- white collar crime
- corporate crime
- official deviance
- organizational deviance
You do not currently have access to this article
Please login to access the full content.
Access to the full content requires a subscription
Printed from Oxford Research Encyclopedias, Criminology and Criminal Justice. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a single article for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).
date: 14 November 2024
- Cookie Policy
- Privacy Policy
- Legal Notice
- Accessibility
- [66.249.64.20|185.80.150.64]
- 185.80.150.64
Character limit 500 /500
An official website of the United States government
Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.
- Publications
- Account settings
- Advanced Search
- Journal List
Current research overstates American support for political violence
Sean j westwood, justin grimmer, matthew tyler, clayton nall.
- Author information
- Article notes
- Copyright and License information
To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: [email protected] .
Edited by Anthony Fowler, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL; received September 15, 2021; accepted February 7, 2022 by Editorial Board Member Margaret Levi
Author contributions: S.J.W. designed research; S.J.W., J.G., and M.T. performed research; M.T. and C.N. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; S.J.W. analyzed data; and S.J.W. and J.G. wrote the paper.
Accepted 2022 Feb 7; Issue date 2022 Mar 22.
This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND) .
Significance
Recent political events show that members of extreme political groups support partisan violence, and survey evidence supposedly shows widespread public support. We show, however, that, after accounting for survey-based measurement error, support for partisan violence is far more limited. Prior estimates overstate support for political violence because of random responding by disengaged respondents and because of a reliance on hypothetical questions about violence in general instead of questions on specific acts of political violence. These same issues also cause the magnitude of the relationship between previously identified correlates and partisan violence to be overstated. As policy makers consider interventions designed to dampen support for violence, our results provide critical information about the magnitude of the problem.
Keywords: political violence, affective polarization, democratic norms
Political scientists, pundits, and citizens worry that America is entering a new period of violent partisan conflict. Provocative survey data show that a large share of Americans (between 8% and 40%) support politically motivated violence. Yet, despite media attention, political violence is rare, amounting to a little more than 1% of violent hate crimes in the United States. We reconcile these seemingly conflicting facts with four large survey experiments ( n = 4,904), demonstrating that self-reported attitudes on political violence are biased upward because of respondent disengagement and survey questions that allow multiple interpretations of political violence. Addressing question wording and respondent disengagement, we find that the median of existing estimates of support for partisan violence is nearly 6 times larger than the median of our estimates (18.5% versus 2.9%). Critically, we show the prior estimates overstate support for political violence because of random responding by disengaged respondents. Respondent disengagement also inflates the relationship between support for violence and previously identified correlates by a factor of 4. Partial identification bounds imply that, under generous assumptions, support for violence among engaged and disengaged respondents is, at most, 6.86%. Finally, nearly all respondents support criminally charging suspects who commit acts of political violence. These findings suggest that, although recent acts of political violence dominate the news, they do not portend a new era of violent conflict.
Provocative recent work ( 1 – 3 )—cited in PNAS ( 4 , 5 ), The American Journal of Political Science ( 6 ), 60 other articles and books, and 40 news articles that, together, have garnered over 2,281,133 Twitter engagements—asserts that large segments of the American population now support politically motivated violence. These studies report that up to 44% of Americans would endorse hypothetical violence in some undetermined future event ( 1 – 3 , 7 ). This survey work fits within a media landscape that regularly raises the specter of political violence. Since 2016, we counted 2,863 mentions of political violence on news television, more than 630 news stories about political violence, and over 10 million tweets on the topic of the January 6 riot alone (see SI Appendix , section 1 for details for all counts in this paragraph). Political violence, however, remains exceedingly rare in the United States, amounting to 48 incidents ( 8 ) in 2019 (the most recent year for which data are available) compared to 4,526 incidents of nonpolitical violent hate crimes ( 9 ) and 1,203,808 total violent crimes ( 10 ) documented by the Department of Justice.
In this paper, we reconcile supposedly significant public support for political violence with minimal actual instances of violent political action. To do this, we use four survey experiments that assess respondents’ reactions to specific acts of violence, where we experimentally manipulate whether partisanship motivated the activity and the severity of the violence. Using these studies, we identify two reasons why current survey data overestimate support for political violence in the United States.
First, ambiguous survey questions cause overestimates of support for violence. Prior studies ask about general support for violence without offering context, leaving the respondent to infer what “violence” means. Using detailed treatments and precisely worded survey questions, we resolve this ambiguity and reveal that support for violence varies substantially depending on the severity of the specific violent act. With our measures, assault and murder attract minimal support, while low-level property crimes gain higher (although still low) support. Moreover, even though segments of the public may support violence or report that it is justified in the abstract, nearly all respondents still believe that perpetrators of well-defined instances of severe political violence should be criminally charged.
Second, disengaged survey respondents cause an upward bias in reported support for violence. Prior survey questions force respondents to select a response without providing a neutral midpoint or a “don’t know” option. This causes disengaged respondents—satisficers ( 11 )—to select an arbitrary or random response ( 12 ). Current violence support scales are coded such that four of five choices indicate acceptance of violence. In the presence of arbitrary responding, such a scale will overstate support for violence. Across all four studies, we show that disengaged respondents report higher support for violence.
Accounting for these sources of error, our four studies show that American support for political violence is less intense than prior work asserts ( Fig. 1 ) and is contingent on the severity of the violent act. Depending on how the question is asked, we show that the median of existing estimates of the public’s support for partisan violence is nearly 6 times larger than the median of our estimates (18.5% versus 2.9%). While recent political events show that extreme political groups are willing to engage in violence, these groups are likely to overlap with the narrow segment of the population who already support political violence. As policy makers consider interventions designed to dampen support for violence, our results demonstrate that support for violence is not a mass phenomenon, indicating that antiviolence measures should be appropriately tailored to match the scale of the problem.
We created a census of all reported estimates of support for violence using the Kalmoe–Mason measure in the media (this includes work done by authors other than Kalmoe and Mason). This figure shows their distribution. We report this in the full sample ( A ), for Republicans ( B ), and for Democrats ( C ). To contextualize the problems, in these estimates, we overlay the largest estimates (orange line) and smallest estimates (blue line) from the studies that follow. There is large variation in the reported values, but all are significantly larger than our preferred estimate. See SI Appendix for additional details.
Support for Partisan Violence Is Lower than Previously Reported
Partisan animosity, often referred to as affective polarization ( 13 ), has increased significantly over the last 30 y. While Americans are arguably no more ideologically polarized than in the recent past, they hold more-negative views toward the political opposition and more-positive views toward members of their own party. This pattern has been documented across several measures of animosity and has raised alarm among scholars across disciplines about the potential consequences of growing partisan discord (e.g., ref. 14 ). Numerous studies have documented the negative interpersonal, “apolitical” ( 15 ) consequences of affective polarization, including politically based discrimination against job applicants ( 16 ), prospective romantic partners ( 17 ), workers ( 18 ), and even scholarship recipients (for a review, see ref. 13 ). These findings have created substantial concerns over partisan animosity’s pervasive effects on American social life ( 19 ).
Yet, evidence suggests that affective polarization is not related to and does not cause increases in support for political violence ( 20 , 21 ) and is generally unrelated to political outcomes ( 21 , 22 ). Moreover, partisan violence appears to be unrelated to many other political variables ( 2 ). We are therefore left with a phenomenon that is not explained by the current literature on partisan animosity, that is rarely observed in the world, but that is based on prior work supported by a near majority of the American population ( 1 – 3 ).
We show that documented support for political violence is illusory, a product of ambiguous questions and disengaged respondents. We now explain how each causes political violence to appear more popular than it is in the public.
Ambiguous Questions Create Upward Bias in Estimates of Support for Violence.
Even if respondents truthfully report their views on political violence, vague questions make it impossible to compare responses across individuals, and render sample averages uninterpretable. For example, a measure from Kalmoe and Mason (hereafter, Kalmoe–Mason) ( 2 , 3 ) asks about perceived justification for partisan violence generally: “How much do you feel it is justified for [respondent’s own party] to use violence in advancing their political goals these days?” But the estimand measured by this survey item is unclear because it leaves ambiguous what “violence” refers to. Another question from Robert Pape ( 23 ), “The use of force is justified to restore Donald Trump to the presidency,” offers a specific motivation, but, like the Kalmoe–Mason measures, leaves definition of “violence” to the respondent to fill in. As a simplistic example, suppose that respondents interpret the question as asking about either partisan-motivated assault or partisan-motivated murder (both acts of violence). If one individual interprets violence as “assault” while another interprets violence as “murder,” then these responses are not comparable, and therefore we cannot make an inference about which respondent expresses more support for political violence ( 24 ). This also affects mean expressed support for violence. The quantity P ( support partisan violence ) is an average of respondents who interpret the question as asking about assault and others interpreting the question as asking about murder. The conditional average support for partisan violence and the relative prevalence of the components of the mixture are unknown, P ( support partisan violence ) = P ( support partisan vio-lence | assault ) P ( assault ) + P ( support partisan violence | murder ) P ( murder ) .
It is impossible to know, from existing responses to vague questions, whether respondents support severe, moderate, or minor forms of violence, which could range from support for violent overthrow of the government to minor supporting assault at a local protest. We address this concern in two ways across our four survey experiments. First, we use two different levels of violence for study 1, study 2, and study 3: assault and murder. Second, in study 4, we vary the underlying violent act along a taxonomy of severity.
Disengaged Respondents Upwardly Bias Measures of Support for Political Violence.
The goal of all surveys is to capture genuine opinions from a sample. However, it is well known that not all respondents engage in the thought, consideration, and reflection necessary to provide reasoned responses to all questions ( 25 ), and some may even overreport rare and negative traits/opinions to troll researchers ( 26 ). As the complexity of the work needed to answer a question increases (i.e., thinking about meaning, filling in details in ambiguous questions, forming opinions on a question a respondent has never previously considered, etc.) and motivation to deeply engage decreases, respondents are more likely to satisfice ( 12 ). When satisficing, respondents may simply select a neutral midpoint ( 11 ), randomly select a response ( 27 ), or even leave a survey ( 25 ). We suspect that the vague and ambiguous nature of current survey measures of political violence are especially likely to cause respondents to satisfice.
Two features of the current survey designs cause the problem. First, existing measures of support for partisan violence collapse response categories to indicate support ( 1 , 2 ). For example, one survey question asks respondents, “How much do you feel it is justified for Democrats to use violence in advancing their political goals these days?” and uses a five-point Likert-like scale with options “Not at all,” “A little,” “A moderate amount,” “A lot,” and “A great deal.” Ref. 2 then recodes the responses “A little” to “A great deal” as indicating support for partisan violence and “Not at all” as opposing partisan violence. Second, such survey questions fail to offer a neutral midpoint or a “don’t know” option. If these imperfect options or frustration from the ambiguous nature of the actual question cause a respondent to disengage from the survey task and satisfice ( 11 ), they are likely to arbitrarily pick from the set of imperfect options. But, in this example, satisficers picking a random response would end up indicating support for violence four times out of five.
To formalize this example, the goal is to measure the true preferences for partisan violence in the population, which we will call E [ Y ] . This quantity is estimated from a representative survey of the population by taking a mean of a survey question, E [ Y survey ] . If some disengaged respondents satisfice, then the estimated support for partisan violence will be
where reported support when satisficing, Y satisfice , might be different from the true support Y depending on the survey respondent’s behavior when satisficing. If E [ Y satisfice | Disengaged ] > E [ Y | Disengaged ] , then the survey-based estimate will be larger than the true level of support for violence. This condition is likely to hold under current survey-based approaches to measuring preferences for partisan violence where four of five response options indicate support for violence (80% of possible responses). If respondents choose their response at random with a uniform probability, then the chance that they would appear to support partisan violence is 0.8. If true E [ Y | Disengaged ] < 0.8 , then the presence of disengaged respondents will cause an upward bias. In an extreme example, if no one actually supports partisan violence, but 31% of respondents—the proportion who fail our engagement test in study 1—in a survey answer at random, a survey would find that 0.31 × 0.8 = 24.8% of respondents support partisan violence. This is very close to the amount of inflation we see in partisan violence in our following studies. *
We take explicit steps to address disengaged respondents who satisfice. We offer satisficers response options that are less likely to upwardly bias estimates: a balanced five-point scale with a neutral midpoint. This brings the measure in line with standard and methodologically robust approaches to measurement, and reduces the chances that a satisficer will randomly select a response indicating support for violence. We also report our estimates based on individuals who are engaged—passing a comprehension check—and individuals who are disengaged, or fail a comprehension check.
Assessing Partisan Differences in Who Commits Political Violence.
Concern about political violence in the United States is often associated with increasing levels of affective polarization between Democrats and Republicans ( 14 ). But existing measures of support for partisan violence tend to not assess whether providing information about the partisanship of who committed the act of violence affects support or opposition for the act of violence. Providing this information is important, because there are two potential interpretations of a positive effect. If the response is sincere, it could be that copartisans give additional leeway for acts committed by copartisans. But, if the response is insincere, it could be that partisans, in general, are merely offering support for their party—a version of partisan cheerleading. While randomizing information about partisanship alone is insufficient to distinguish between these two possibilities, failure to find a difference in a well-powered study provides strong evidence that neither leeway nor cheerleading occur.
To assess how partisanship affects support for violence, in our study 1 and study 2, we explicitly vary information about the partisanship of who committed the acts of violence. As we show below, we fail to find a consistent partisan difference—implying that there is little evidence for a general leeway or cheerleading effect.
While we find little evidence of partisan cheerleading among all partisans, we might worry that a specific subset of partisans engage in explicit partisan cheerleading. To make this assessment, in study 3, we use existing survey questions to identify partisan cheerleading ( 28 ) and find that partisan cheerleaders inflate support for violence, but those cheerleaders comprise only a small share of respondents and therefore do not appear to meaningfully affect results.
To uncover how these sources of error affect perceptions of partisan violence, we conducted four survey experiments. We fielded our first survey (which contained study 1 and study 4) via Qualtrics panels in January 2021—starting 2 d after the violence of January 6. This allows us to test our predictions during a period when partisan discord and violence dominated news coverage. Our second survey (study 2) was fielded in April 2021, also on Qualtrics panels. Our final survey (study 3) was fielded in November of 2021 on the YouGov panel. This allows us to verify that our results are not dependent on proximity to the Capitol riots or on a specific survey panel.
The Qualtrics data were collected from Qualtrics panels and utilized quota sampling. Respondents were recruited from panel members by email. All surveys were restricted to Democrats and Republicans. Leaners were coded as partisans. For Qualtrics data, we quota sampled on age, sex, and race/ethnicity to match Census targets. The sample is generally very representative of the population ( SI Appendix , Tables S1, S19, S30, and S39 ). These data were analyzed without survey weights per our preanalysis plan. The YouGov data were sampled with the standard YouGov matching procedure. YouGov data were analyzed with provided weights. †
The survey flow was as follows: consent, attention check, demographics, covariates (including the measure from refs. 1, 2), randomized treatment, engagement test, and then outcome questions. Our experiments were approved by institutional review boards at Stanford, Dartmouth, and University of California, Santa Barbara. Participants were asked to give consent after reading an information sheet.
All four experiments were preregistered. For our Qualtrics data, following our preanalysis plan, we excluded participants who failed a prerandomization attention check (a question asking respondents to make two specific response choices) and those who completed the survey in less than one-third of the median complete time. Neither of these choices altered the demographic composition of the sample, as purged respondents were not counted toward quotas and were replaced. Both of these choices work against us by removing disengaged subjects, which means that our estimates are conservative, as these design choices remove respondents who are most likely to respond to all survey questions at random. Those who remain and satisfice are likely doing so because of flawed, ambiguous, or insufficiently contextualized questions and not because of general inattention.
Measuring Engagement.
To assess how satisficing affects responses, we group participants based on their cognitive engagement with our survey, measured as the retention of information from vignettes. Respondents who can identify the state where the event in the vignette they just read occurred (information that was repeated in the headline and up to two times in the text) are coded as engaged, and those who cannot are coded as disengaged. Reading short passages is not a cognitively intense task, but committing content to memory is ( 29 ), and reveals engagement with cognitive work. Across our three studies, we group our sample into “engaged” respondents—those who are sufficiently motivated to carefully read and retain information—and “disengaged” respondents—those who can follow instructions but are less likely to retain facts or carefully evaluate questions. ‡
Outcome Questions.
We use three core questions for study 1, study 2, and study 3 and deploy two strategies to identify satisficing’s upward bias on estimates of support for violence. First, in our justification outcome question, we use a dichotomous scale which should prompt disengaged respondents to randomly respond, which we can detect by observing the response distribution. Second, we offer a balanced scale with a neutral midpoint when measuring support, building on prior work that suggests disengaged respondents will respond with a “don’t know” or neutral midpoint response ( 11 ). Our three questions are as follows:
“Was the suspect justified or unjustified?” using a dichotomous outcome scale. This differs from refs. 1–3, where the authors ask a hypothetical question with an unbalanced five-point Likert scale that is then recoded in their analysis to a dichotomous measure.
“Do you support or oppose the actions of [suspect]?” This is measured with a balanced Likert with a neutral midpoint and is intended to separately capture support for a suspect when a violent act is committed.
“Should the suspect face criminal charges?” We capture responses with a dichotomous yes/no scale, and the question is intended to determine whether the respondent thinks that someone who commits an act of violence should or should not face charges.
Respondents Reject Extreme Violence, Whether It Is Political or Not
Study 1, study 2, and study 3 show that, as preregistered, respondents overwhelmingly reject both political and nonpolitical violence, and disengaged survey respondents show higher measured support for political violence. We find no evidence of partisan effects, as partisans from both sides express similar tolerance for political violence. We also find higher (although still low) levels of support for the less violent act in study 1 relative to the more violent act in study 2 and study 3.
To avoid the problem of ambiguous question wording, our design presents a detailed act of violence, which prevents respondents from substituting their own definition of “violence” when answering our outcome questions.
In study 1 ( n = 1,002), we randomly assigned participants to read one of two stories based on real acts of political violence. In the first story, a Democratic driver was charged with hitting a group of Republicans in Florida who were registering citizens to vote. In the second story, a Republican driver was charged with assault for driving his car through Democratic protesters in Oregon. Respondents were also randomized to see the original version of the story that included partisan details or a version of the story that was altered to remove any reference to partisan motivation.
In this study, we focused on reporting details from real events. This means that, while comparable, the Democratic and Republican stories varied in several ways. To ensure that any effects we identify are not the result of those differences, we conducted a second version of this experiment. Study 2 ( n = 1,023) used a single contrived story of violence in Iowa. To test the bounds of support for political violence, this story reported an extreme form of violence: murder. Similar to study 1, participants were randomly assigned to see a story with a Republican or Democratic shooter engaging in politically motivated violence or an apolitical act of murder. This story was necessarily fabricated to limit the differences across treatment conditions.
Study 3 ( n = 1,863) is a replication of study 2 using the YouGov panel with the following alterations: 1) We removed the apolitical condition to focus on attitudes toward partisan violence, and 2) we removed covariate questions to reduce survey time.
Disengaged Responses Lead to Higher Estimates of Support for Political Violence.
At first glance, the results of this experiment appear to align with prior surveys. Across conditions where the driver’s actions are presented as political violence, we find that 21.07% of respondents in study 1 say the attack was justified. We find a similarly high level of support for the apolitical versions, where 20.56% of respondents in study 1 say the driver’s action is justified. The overall support for violence is lower in study 2 and study 3, reflecting the greater severity of the violence, with 10.02% of respondents in study 2 describing the political homicide as justified and 6.70% of the respondents describing the apolitical homicide as justified. In study 3, 10.11% described the homicide as justified.
But this is biased upward by respondents who fail the engagement test (∼31% of respondents in study 1, 19% of respondents in study 2, and 19% of weighted respondents in study 3). For the political treatments, 37.87% of respondents who fail the engagement test say the driver’s actions were justified, while only 12.06% of respondents who passed the engagement test agree that the driver’s actions are justified. For the nonpolitical treatment, we find that 44.06% of respondents who failed the engagement test say the driver’s actions were justified, but only 10.93% of respondents who passed the engagement test say the driver’s actions are justified. Similarly, for study 2, in the political treatments, we find that 33.82% of the respondents who fail the engagement test say the shooter’s actions were justified, but only 4.37% of individuals who passed the engagement test say the action was justified. In the nonpolitical treatments, we find a similar large gap: 25.93% of respondents who fail the engagement test say the action was justified, but 2.68% of those who passed say the action was justified. The same pattern is found in study 3 (YouGov data), with 28.25% of disengaged respondents saying the shooting was justified, while only 5.86% of engaged respondents say the shooting was justified.
Fig. 2 shows that this overall pattern is found across all treatment conditions in both studies. The red circles and lines in Fig. 2 show disengaged respondents, while teal squares and lines show engaged respondents. In all cases, disengaged responses indicate significantly greater justification and support for political violence relative to engaged responses.
This figure shows attitudes toward violence for each of our three measures: justification ( A ), support ( B ), and should the subject be charged ( C ). We plot group means and 95% CIs. For the YouGov data (study 3), we utilize survey weights. Providing partisan motivations has no effect on support for violence relative to identical, but apolitical, violence.
When it comes to our third outcome question, support for charging the accused, we see a different pattern. Unlike the first two outcome questions, which are abstract moral judgments, this question is concrete: Should those who commit a crime face legal consequences? Consistent with the specificity of this question, we find much higher overall agreement. In study 1, pooling across our conditions, 92.70% of respondents who passed the engagement test want the suspect in the politically motivated violent crime charged, while 79.88% of disengaged respondents want the suspect in the politically motivated violent crime charged. For study 2, 99.30% of engaged respondents and 86.76% of disengaged respondents want the suspect charged. Similarly, in study 3, 97.30% of engaged respondents and 85.48% of disengaged respondents want the suspect charged
Abstract Questions and Disengaged Respondents Inflate Support for Violence.
Respondents who fail our engagement test express much higher rates of support for the hypothetical political violence measure used in extant observational studies (which we included in all our studies pretreatment). We show problems with disengaged respondents with two sets of analyses. First, we show, in Table 1 , that the current hypothetical question developed by refs. 1 and 2 (measured here with a balanced Likert with a neutral midpoint) generates overestimates of public support for partisan violence, because of disengaged respondents. Across our three studies, we find that support for violence on this measure is nearly twice as large in the disengaged group as in the engaged group.
Kalmoe–Mason support for violence measure by engagement
Second, we look for evidence of satisficing on our three outcome measures. Our preregistered expectation is that disengaged respondents provide upwardly biased responses to abstract questions. We find substantial support for this hypothesis in the data. As detailed earlier, our questions vary in the extent to which they demand a well-considered response. Questions of justification and support require reflection on the criminal act, a personal moral code, and social norms, whereas asking if a person who committed a violent act should be charged requires no such introspection. Assuming respondents are cognitive misers who satisfice to escape considered thought where possible, we should then expect more satisficing on the first two questions than on the third ( 11 ).
This is borne out in our data. Fig. 3 A shows that, when presented with a dichotomous question and no “don’t know” option, disengaged respondents essentially randomly split their responses between the two choices, while engaged respondents overwhelmingly report that the driver is not justified. Fig. 3 B shows that, when disengaged respondents are presented with five choices that include a neutral midpoint, the modal response is the midpoint, with the remaining respondents splitting their responses between the remaining four categories. Both response strategies are consistent with satisficing. A plurality of engaged respondents report strongly opposing violence.
The response distribution for justification ( A ), support ( B ), and charging preferences ( C ) by engagement for study 1. High levels of support for political violence can be partially attributed to random responding by disengaged respondents, especially when questions are vague.
Fig. 3 C shows that, when answering a simpler question with clear normative expectations—charging criminals for crimes—disengaged and engaged respondents are much more comparable. It is also possible that respondents deemed the information in the newspaper articles we provided insufficient to establish moral justification but sufficient to determine a preference for criminal charges.
Results from study 2, where the reported crime was murder, show a more dramatic difference between the engaged and the disengaged. For engaged respondents, justification peaks at 6.81%, support peaks at 2.15%, and willingness to excuse the suspect from criminal charges peaks at 1.08%. This compares to disengaged respondents where justification peaks at 35.50%, support peaks at 17.10%, and willingness to excuse the suspect from criminal charges peaks at 15.8%. Disengaged respondents report support that is ∼8 times greater than engaged respondents.
Study 3, our YouGov replication of study 2, produces very similar results. Justification is ∼5.5 times larger for disengaged (32.60%) versus engaged (5.98%) respondents, support is ∼9 times larger for disengaged (26.10%) versus engaged (2.89%) respondents, and willingness to excuse the suspect from criminal charges is ∼4 times larger for disengaged (15.10%) versus engaged (3.98%) respondents.
These results suggest that overestimates of support for political violence on surveys are partially explained by satisficing and random responses because of flawed questions.
Support for Political Violence Is Lowest for the Most Severe Crimes
We have, so far, demonstrated that disengaged respondents create upward bias in support for political violence and that this is a function of the amount of thought questions require of respondents. Our expectation is that offering additional information—that a suspect has been convicted of a specific crime—reduces question ambiguity enough to attenuate differences between disengaged and engaged respondents. By reporting an exact crime, we are also able to bound what support for political violence exists by crime severity.
Study 4 ( n = 1,009) captures support for nullifying convictions for a set of politically motivated crimes (some violent and some not) that vary in severity from protesting without a permit to murder. To administer the survey, we first asked standard demographic and covariate batteries and administered a neutral vignette that mentioned a state. We coded engagement by asking respondents to identify the state where a news event occurred in a pretreatment and unrelated vignette ( 31 ). Each respondent then read a short prompt informing them that a man, “Jon James Fishnick,” had been convicted of a crime and faces sentencing in the coming week. We then randomly selected a single crime (protesting without a permit, vandalism, petty assault, arson, assault with a deadly weapon, and murder) along with details specifying that the crime was partisan and committed against a member of the opposing party. Participants were then asked to suggest a sentence for Fishnick that ranged from community service to more than 20 y in prison.
Fig. 4 shows the frequency of each suggested sentence by crime and by respondent engagement. When the crime is nonviolent (protesting without a permit, vandalism) a near majority of both engaged and disengaged respondents support the minimal penalty of community service. A minimally violent crime (assault—throwing rocks leading to an injury) sees most respondents suggest a term in jail, although about 20 to 25% of respondents still support community service. However, a clear inflection point arrives when the crimes become violent and serious. For the remaining three crimes, respondents overwhelmingly support lengthy prison terms. Almost no engaged respondents favor community service as punishment for severe crimes: arson (3.8% of engaged respondents), assault with a deadly weapon (4.6%), and murder (2.6%). Indeed, the majority of engaged respondents believe more than 20 y in prison is the appropriate punishment for murder.
In this study, we remove as much ambiguity as possible by identifying a specific crime for which someone has been convicted. This additional context makes differences between engaged and disengaged respondents largely vanish. Furthermore, respondents, especially engaged ones, punish more-severe violent crimes with longer prison sentences. This suggests that, although support for political violence exists in the electorate, it is primarily constrained to support for minor crimes.
In addition to asking about the appropriate punishment, we asked whether the governor should pardon Fishnick. SI Appendix , Fig. S2 shows that, on average, respondents only support a pardon for minor crimes. Engaged respondents are, however, much more likely than disengaged respondents to oppose a pardon for serious acts of violence.
Disengaged Respondents Bias Estimates of the Correlates of Political Violence
Our primary goal thus far has been to precisely estimate the levels of support for partisan violence in the public. However, others focus on a second goal: finding the characteristics of individuals that predict support for violence ( 2 , 32 , 33 ). But the same issues that create bias in estimates of support for violence also cause bias in estimates of the relationship between supporting violence and other variables. This is because the usual rules of vanilla measurement error are not applicable with disengaged survey respondents, who are likely to remain disengaged across several questions and therefore cause nonrandom measurement error. The consequence is that disengaged survey respondents can create measurement error that causes bias in an unknown direction and, in some cases, can make the relationships between variables appear stronger rather than weaker.
To get intuition for how this can occur, consider a simple example. Suppose our goal is to measure how much support for violence differs across a dichotomous attribute, X . As in our analyses above, we suppose that our respondents are divided into engaged and disengaged individuals. We will further suppose that being disengaged affects both the reported support for violence and the measured value of X , biasing both upward. As a hypothetical example, suppose that P(Violence | Engaged, X = 1) = 0.15, P(Violence | Engaged, X = 0) = 0.05, that P(X = 1 | Engaged) = P(X = 0 | Engaged) = 0.5, and that P(Engaged) = 0.8. But, for disengaged respondents, we suppose that P(Violence | Disengaged, X = 1) = P(Violence | Disengaged, X = 0) = 0.8, and that P(X = 1 | Disengaged) = 0.8. The true difference among the engaged respondents is P(Violence | Engaged, X = 1) –P(Violence | Engaged, X = 0) = 0.1. But, because of the nonrandom measurement error among the disengaged respondents, the estimated difference using the overall data is P(Violence | X = 1) –P(Violence | X = 0) = 0.217. Nonrandom measurement error from disengaged respondents causes the relationship between X and support for violence (measured as the difference in average support for violence at levels of X ) to be more than twice as large as the true relationship.
We find evidence that this bias occurs when assessing predictors of political violence. The literature has identified three significant predictors of support for violence: partisan social identity, aggression, and hostile sexism ( 2 , 32 , 33 ). Here we focus on the largest predictor: aggression [as measured in our work with the Buss–Perry Short Form ( 34 ) from study 2]. As we show in Fig. 5 A , the proportion of respondents who are engaged decreases rapidly at high levels of reported aggressive personality. Fig. 5 B shows that, as a result, disengaged respondents are disproportionately represented among those with the highest levels of reported aggressive personality.
This figure shows the problems with estimating correlates of support for violence when measures are biased. ( A ) The proportion of respondents who are disengaged by scored level of aggression on the Buss–Perry scale. ( B ) The distribution of aggression by engagement.
The higher reported levels of aggressive personality are coupled with the higher levels of support for violence among disengaged respondents that we documented above, resulting in disengaged respondents creating a stronger relationship between aggressive personality and support for violence. Fig. 6 shows that, if we use all respondents and the original measure of violence support from ref. 2 , moving from the least to most aggressive personalities is associated with an 82 percentage point increase in support for violence. That same shift goes down to 67 percentage points among just the engaged respondents with the original measure. But, if we focus on only the engaged respondents, using our more precise measure, that same large shift from least to most aggressive is associated with a 20 percentage point increase in support for violence. Taken together, using imprecise survey questions and failing to account for disengaged respondents produces a relationship between aggressive personality and support for violence that is ∼4 times too large.
This plot shows that the relationship between aggression and support for political violence—as measured as the regression coefficient from a linear regression of support for violence on aggressive personality—is biased upward by disengaged respondents. Moreover, the relationship is much smaller when using a more precise measure of support for political violence.
Finally, Fig. 5 A suggests that the assumption of a linear relationship obscures a nonlinear relationship ( 35 ). We continue to assume a linear relationship to provide an apples-to-apples comparison to the analysis in ref. 2 . In SI Appendix , Table S66 , we provide binned estimates of the relationship between aggression and violence.
Recommendations
Our goal is not to argue that there is no support for political violence in America. Recent events demonstrate that groups of American extremists will violate the law and engage in violence to advance their political goals. Instead, our purpose is to show that, when attempting to estimate support for political violence among the public, care and precision is required. Generic and hypothetical questions offer respondents too many degrees of freedom and require greater cognition than a sizable portion of the population will engage in. We suggest that future attempts to measure support for political violence 1) utilize specific examples with sufficient details to remove the need for respondents to speculate, 2) benchmark results against general support for all violence, and 3) capture support for crimes that vary in severity.
Conclusion: Limited Support for Political Violence
Our results show support for political violence is not broad based and is, on average, ∼13 times lower than the average estimate previously reported by Kalmoe–Mason ( 1 , 2 ) and 6 times lower than the estimate provided by Pape ( 23 ). The public overwhelmingly rejects acts of violence, whether they are political or not. Our evidence suggests that extant studies have reached a different conclusion because of design and measurement flaws. When disengaged respondents are not excluded from analysis, measured support for violence is biased upward. Our evidence suggests that this is because disengaged respondents are satisficing in response to ambiguous questions. Vague questions about acceptance of partisan violence demand too much interpretation from respondents, yielding incorrect inferences about support for severe political violence. Not only is support for violence low overall, but support drops considerably as political violence becomes more severe. The most serious form of political violence—murder in service of a political cause—is widely condemned.
Importantly, our results are not conditional on partisanship ( SI Appendix , Tables S2, S20, and S33 ). Our results are robust to several other predicted causes of political violence. We find that several standard political measures (i.e., affective polarization and political engagement) are less predictive of support for political violence than are general measures of aggression [measured using the Buss–Perry scale ( 34 ); SI Appendix , Tables S10 and S26 ], suggesting that tolerance for violence is a general human preference and not a specifically political preference. § We also find that social desirability [measured with the Marlowe–Crowne scale ( 36 )] does not temper support for political violence on surveys, suggesting that social desirability is not responsible for our lower estimates of support.
In study 3, we address two alternative mechanisms: partisan cheerleading and respondent trolling. We find that both significantly inflate support for violence, but do so for both engaged and disengaged respondents, suggesting that these mechanisms offer additional reasons to be skeptical of prior estimates. To test for partisan cheerleading ( 37 ), we use the design from ref. 28 . Partisan cheerleaders are significantly more likely to support partisan violence across all three of our measures ( SI Appendix , Table S34 ), but this is unlikely to drive our results, as this represents 3.6% of the sample, and cheerleaders are nearly evenly split between disengaged respondents ( n = 33) and engaged respondents ( n = 38). Secondly, we test for trolling using a shark bite question ( 26 ) as deployed on the American National Election Studies (the expectation is that responses above the known rate indicate trolling behavior). Trolling respondents inflate support for violence on two of our three measures ( SI Appendix , Table S33 ), but, again, they represent a small portion of the sample (2.7%) and are split between engaged ( n = 17) and disengaged respondents ( n = 34). Removing cheerleaders and trolls decreases mean support for political violence from 1.42 to 1.39 (a change of 0.03 points).
Another concern is that focusing on engaged respondents is misleading because true support for violence might be correlated (positively or negatively) with disengaged survey responding. To address this, we derive partial identification bounds assuming that the true support for violence among disengaged respondents is not observable from the survey question (see SI Appendix , section S9 for details on the methods used below). For example, in the study 3 outcomes asking about murder, if we assume that true support for violence among disengaged respondents is anywhere between 0% and 100%, then the 95% CI expands from [1.76%, 4.22%] to [0.94%, 24.95%]. However, if we cap true support among the disengaged at a more plausible yet still alarming number, such as 20% (approximately the median value reported in prior work), then the partial identification CI shrinks considerably to [0.94%, 6.86%]. ¶ We note that 6.3% support is less than the minimum support for violence reported in Fig. 1 . Overall, these bounds suggest that, unless disengaged respondents are orders of magnitude more proviolence than engaged respondents, the population average support for violence is still much lower than previous estimates have implied.
Of course, it is important to understand that, while we show that support for political violence is lower than expected, it is not precisely measured as zero. An important next step is identifying why remaining support exists and where, specifically, violent political action is likely to emerge. Future work could randomize attention and identify what crimes people default to when asked generic violence questions.
Our results offer critical context to stakeholders, citizens, and politicians on the nation’s response to political protests in Portland and the events following the 2020 presidential election. A small share of Americans support political violence, but most of this support comes from a troubling segment of the public who support violence in general. Even among this group, support is further contingent on the severity of the violent act and is generally limited to relatively minor crimes. Political violence is a problem in every public, but, as our results show, it is important to carefully and accurately measure such support before raising alarm that might not be warranted. This is especially true when these alarms direct attention, funding, and concern away from other critical policy debates ( 38 ).
Violence of the sort seen on January 6 is, at most, concentrated at the extremes of the parties, and, despite the massive news coverage of political violence, the underlying acts are very rare in comparison to general crime trends. Nevertheless, any amount of support for political violence is troubling and worthy of exploration. Researchers should set their sights on these pockets of extremism and organized violent activity—not the casual and frequently underconsidered opinions of everyday voters. Mainstream Americans of both parties have little appetite for violence—political or not.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments.
For helpful comments, we thank Alex Coppock, Jamie Druckman, Matt Gentzkow, Andrew Hall, Shanto Iyengar, Jennifer Jerrit, Samara Klar, Yphtach Lelkes, Neil Malhotra, Jonathan Mummolo, Erik Peterson, and Brandon Stewart.
The authors declare no competing interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. A.F. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.
*We note that, while not observed here, if true support for violence were above 0.8, the bias would be negative. Also, if the true prevalence rate among the disengaged were 0.8, then the bias for the population parameter would be zero.
† By necessity, weights were not used when estimating partial bounds.
‡ SI Appendix , Table S64 shows that removing disengaged respondents does not meaningfully change the demographics of our sample (age, gender, race, partisanship, income, and education). Another concern is that we are conditioning on a posttreatment outcome. However, our goal is not to measure the causal effect of engagement ( 30 ), but to merely show that responses differ based on engagement.
§ We do, however, find that strong partisans are more likely to support violence.
¶ For completeness, we note the other outcomes from study 3. For the justification outcome, the average support among just engaged respondents is [3.83%, 7.11%], with 0 to 100 disengaged support the full population average is [2.63%, 26.97%], and with 0 to 20 disengaged support the full population average is [2.63%, 10.14%]. For charging the attacker, the average charge rate among just engaged respondents is [95.00%, 97.71%], with 0 to 100 disengaged support the full population charge rate is [74.24%, 98.36%], and with 0 to 20 disengaged support the full population charge rate is [92.29%, 98.36%]. Note that all of these estimates are for respondents assigned to the in-party shooter condition, and no survey weights were used.
This article contains supporting information online at https://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.2116870119/-/DCSupplemental .
Data Availability
Experimental data and all replication code have been deposited in Harvard Dataverse ( https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/ZEHO8E ). We have also prepared a Code Ocean replication capsule ( https://doi.org/10.24433/CO.4651754.v1 ).
- 1. Diamond L., Drutman L., Lindberg T., Kalmoe N. P., Mason L., Americans increasingly believe violence is justified if the other side wins. Politico , 1 October 2020. https://www.politico.com/news/magazine/2020/10/01/political-violence-424157 . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 2. Kalmoe N. P., Mason L., Radical American Partisanship: Mapping Violent Hostility, Its Causes, & What It Means for Democracy (University of Chicago Press, 2022). [ Google Scholar ]
- 3. Carey J. M., Helmke G., Nyhan B., Stokes S. C., American. democracy on the eve of the 2020 election. Bright Line Watch (2020). https://brightlinewatch.org/american-democracy-on-the-eve-of-the-2020-election/ . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 4. Bartels L. M., Ethnic antagonism erodes Republicans’ commitment to democracy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 22752–22759 (2020). [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 5. Clayton K., et al., Elite rhetoric can undermine democratic norms. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2024125118 (2021). [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 6. Uscinski J. E., et al., American politics in two dimensions: Partisan and ideological identities versus anti-establishment orientations. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 65, 877–895 (2021). [ Google Scholar ]
- 7. Cox D. A., Support for political violence among Americans is on the rise. It’s a grim warning about America’s political future. Business Insider , 26 March 2021. https://www.aei.org/op-eds/support-for-political-violence-among-americans-is-on-the-rise-its-a-grim-warning-about-americas-political-future/ . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 8. Jones S. G., Doxsee C., Harrington N., The escalating terrorism problem in the United States. https://www.csis.org/analysis/escalating-terrorism-problem-united-states . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 9. Federal Bureau of Investigation, Hate crime statistics, 2019. https://ucr.fbi.gov/hat-crime/2019 . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 10. Federal Bureau of Investigation, FBI releases 2019 crime statistics. https://www.fbi.gov/news/pressrel/press-releases/fbi-releases-2019-hate-crime-statistics . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 11. Krosnick J. A., Narayan S., Smith W. R., Satisficing in surveys: Initial evidence. New Dir. Eval. 1996, 29–44 (1996). [ Google Scholar ]
- 12. Krosnick J. A., Response strategies for coping with the cognitive demands of attitude measures in surveys. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 5, 213–236 (1991). [ Google Scholar ]
- 13. Iyengar S., Lelkes Y., Levendusky M., Malhotra N., Westwood S. J., The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 22, 129–146 (2019). [ Google Scholar ]
- 14. Finkel E. J., et al., Political sectarianism in America. Science 370, 533–536 (2020). [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 15. Druckman J. N., Klar S., Krupnikov Y., Levendusky M., Ryan J. B., (Mis)estimating affective polarization J. Polit., 10.1086/715603 (2020). [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 16. Gift K., Gift T., Does politics influence hiring? Evidence from a randomized experiment. Polit. Behav. 37, 653–675 (2015). [ Google Scholar ]
- 17. Huber G. A., Malhotra N., Political homophily in social relationships: Evidence from online dating behavior. J. Polit. 79, 269–283 (2017). [ Google Scholar ]
- 18. McConnell C., Margalit Y., Malhotra N., Levendusky M., The economic consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. Am. J. Pol. Sci. 62, 5–18 (2018). [ Google Scholar ]
- 19. Druckman J. N., Klar S., Krupnikov Y., Levendusky M., Ryan J. B., How affective polarization shapes Americans’ political beliefs: A study of response to the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Exp. Polit. Sci. 8, 223–234 (2020). [ Google Scholar ]
- 20. Lelkes Y., Westwood S. J., The limits of partisan prejudice. J. Polit. 79, 485–501 (2017). [ Google Scholar ]
- 21. Broockman D., Kalla J., Westwood S., Does affective polarization undermine democratic norms or accountability? Maybe not. Am. J. Polit. Sci. , in press.
- 22. Voelkel J. G., et al., Interventions reducing affective polarization do not improve anti-democratic attitudes. OSF Preprints [Preprint] (2021). 10.31219/osf.io/7evmp (Accessed 2 March 2022). [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 23. Pape R., Understanding the American insurrectionist movement: A nationally representative survey. Chicago Project on Security and Threats (2021). https://d3qi0qp55mx5f5.cloudfront.net/cpost/i/docs/CPOST-NORC_UnderstandingInsurrectionSurvey_JUN2021_Topline.pdf . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 24. King G., Murray C. J., Salomon J. A., Tandon A., Enhancing the validity and cross-cultural comparability of measurement in survey research. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 98, 191–207 (2004). [ Google Scholar ]
- 25. Tourangeau R., Rips L. J., Rasinski K., The Psychology of Survey Response (Cambridge University Press, 2000). [ Google Scholar ]
- 26. Lopez J., Hillygus D. S., Why so serious?: Survey trolls and misinformation. SSRN [Preprint] (2018). https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3131087 (Accessed 2 March 2022).
- 27. Krosnick J. A., Survey research. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50, 537–567 (1999). [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 28. Schaffner B. F., Luks S., Misinformation or expressive responding? What an inauguration crowd can tell us about the source of political misinformation in surveys. Public Opin. Q. 82, 135–147 (2018). [ Google Scholar ]
- 29. Marello C. C., “The effects of an integrated reading and writing curriculum on academic performance, motivation, and retention rates of underprepared college students,” PhD thesis, University of Maryland, College Park, MD: (1999). [ Google Scholar ]
- 30. Peyton K., Huber G. A., Coppock A., The generalizability of online experiments conducted during the covid-19 pandemic. J. Exp. Polit. Sci., 10.1017/XPS.2021.17 (2021). [ Google Scholar ]
- 31. Kane J. V., Velez Y. R., Barabas J., Analyze the attentive & bypass bias: Mock vignette checks in survey experiments. APSA Preprints [Preprint] (2020). https://preprints.apsanet.org/engage/apsa/article-details/5f825740efc0c2001974f0f7 (Accessed 2 March 2022).
- 32. Edsall T., No hate left behind. NY Times , 13 March 2019. https://www.nytimes.com/2019.03.13/opinion/hate-politics.html . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 33. Mason L., Mason K., What you need to know about how many Americans condone political violence–and why. Washington Post , 11 January 2021. https://www.washingtonpost.com/politics/2021/01/11/what-you-need-know-about-how-many-americans-condone-political-violence-why/ . Accessed 2 March 2022.
- 34. Diamond P. M., Magaletta P. R., The short-form Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ-SF): A validation study with federal offenders. Assessment 13, 227–240 (2006). [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- 35. Hainmueller J., Mummolo J., Xu Y., How much should we trust estimates from multiplicative interaction models? Simple tools to improve empirical practice. Polit. Anal. 27, 163–192 (2019). [ Google Scholar ]
- 36. Reynolds W. M., Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 38, 119–125 (1982). [ Google Scholar ]
- 37. Bullock J. G., Lenz G., Partisan bias in surveys. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 22, 325–342 (2019). [ Google Scholar ]
- 38. Boydstun A. E., Making the News: Politics, the Media, and Agenda Setting (University of Chicago Press, 2013). [ Google Scholar ]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data availability statement.
- View on publisher site
- PDF (1.0 MB)
- Collections
Similar articles
Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.
- Download .nbib .nbib
- Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Add to Collections
- Search Menu
- Sign in through your institution
- Author Guidelines
- Submission Site
- Open Access Options
- Why Submit?
- About International Journal of Public Opinion Research
- About the World Association for Public Opinion Research
- Editorial Board
- Advertising and Corporate Services
- Journals Career Network
- Self-Archiving Policy
- Journals on Oxford Academic
- Books on Oxford Academic
Article Contents
- Crime, Violence, and Participation: Debates and Conflicting Evidence
- Meta-analysis
- Personality, Political Engagement, and Willingness to Answer Sensitive Questions
- Research Strategy and Findings
- Estimation Strategy
- Conclusions
- Supplementary Data
- < Previous
Crime, Violence, and Political Participation
- Article contents
- Figures & tables
Carew Boulding, Shawnna Mullenax, Kathryn Schauer, Crime, Violence, and Political Participation, International Journal of Public Opinion Research , Volume 34, Issue 1, Spring 2022, edab032, https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edab032
- Permissions Icon Permissions
The paper explores the relationship between crime victimization and political participation, making the argument that the observed (and oft-cited) relationship in surveys is the result of response bias. We show that people who are more extroverted, efficacious, and opinionated are more likely to respond positively to a question about victimization, and they are more likely to participate in politics. The implications of this finding are important and widespread—both for survey researchers interested in sensitive questions and for research on the relationship between crime and political engagement.
Email alerts
Citing articles via.
- X (formerly Twitter)
- Recommend to Your Librarian
Affiliations
- Online ISSN 1471-6909
- Copyright © 2024 World Association for Public Opinion Research
- About Oxford Academic
- Publish journals with us
- University press partners
- What we publish
- New features
- Open access
- Institutional account management
- Rights and permissions
- Get help with access
- Accessibility
- Advertising
- Media enquiries
- Oxford University Press
- Oxford Languages
- University of Oxford
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide
- Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
- Cookie settings
- Cookie policy
- Privacy policy
- Legal notice
This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only
Sign In or Create an Account
This PDF is available to Subscribers Only
For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.
Political Violence and the Law
Past offerings, 2023-2024 spring, useful links.
- Course Evaluations
- Approved Non-Law Courses
Political Violence and the Law (7122): Twenty years after September 11, 2001, and the onset of the U.S.-led global war on terror, fears of political violence within the United States are rising and shifting. Between the assault on the Capitol on January 6, 2021, record numbers of hate crimes targeting identity groups, and growing proportions of the American public expressing support for violence against the state, the specter of political violence has generated new legal questions and rekindled old ones. How does the law conceptualize "political" crimes or "crimes against the state"? What are the legal, political, and symbolic differences between categories such as treason, terrorism, insurrection, sedition, and hate crimes? How does the law conceptualize such violence in comparison with violence by the state? How should government institutions, including executive agencies and courts, respond to the threat of political violence while protecting rights, supporting equality, and aspiring to democratic accountability? This seminar explores contemporary policy questions in light of a broader theoretical, historical, and legal context. This course has two grading options. You may either choose to complete four response papers (4-5 pages each) responding to the week's readings or write a 18-20 page research paper for R credit. Elements used in grading: Attendance, class participation, and written assignments or a final research paper.
Political Violence and the Law | LAW 7122 Section 01 Class #1120
- Grading: Law Honors/Pass/Restrd Cr/Fail
- 2023-2024 Spring Schedule No Longer Available
- Enrollment Limitations: Consent 16
- LO1 - Substantive and Procedural Law
- LO2 - Legal Analysis and Reasoning
- LO3 - Ability to Conduct Legal Research
Notes: This class section has been cancelled!
- 2023-2024 Spring Schedule No Longer Available This class section has been cancelled!
Political Violence and the Law | LAW 7122 Section 02 Class #32077
- Enrollment Limitations: Consent
- R -Research Requirement for Law Degree
- LO4 - Ability to Communicate Effectively in Writing
COMMENTS
Research studies that focus on specific aspects of political crime continue to add impressive perspectives and endeavors to expand the extant literature. In this issue, Fiona Chan, Carole Gibbs, Rachel Boratto, and Mark Speers offer an innovative examination of transnational corporate bribery.
This review focuses on terrorism and extremist crimes, including ideologically motivated hate crimes. Research on these topics has become more rigorous in recent decades, and more scholars have engaged in original data collection.
From death threats against previously anonymous bureaucrats and public-health officials to a plot to kidnap Michigan’s governor and the 6 January 2021 attack on the U.S. Capitol, acts of political violence in the United States have skyrocketed in the last five years. 2 The nature of political violence has also changed.
Until recently, the majority of research on crime has focused on the illegal actions of individuals, gangs, syndicates, and corporations. Less attention has been given to state crimes (Ross, 1995/2000; Ross et al., 1999).
A growing number of criminologists have started to focus their attentions on the relationships between crime (broadly understood) and politics (understood as processes, partisanship and institutions).
Criminological work on political corruption and state crime has roots in research on white collar and organized offending (Nelken & Levi, 1996). As such, this section begins with an overview of selected key works on white collar crime to provide a starting point for understanding political corruption and crimes of the state.
Using an original survey of more than 1300 subjects in the United States, I evaluate four elements that theoretically mediate the relationship between populism and support for political violence: economic grievances; distrust of political institutions; perception that social and demographic changes in the United States are threatening; and prefe...
Provocative survey data show that a large share of Americans (between 8% and 40%) support politically motivated violence. Yet, despite media attention, political violence is rare, amounting to a little more than 1% of violent hate crimes in the United States.
The paper explores the relationship between crime victimization and political participation, making the argument that the observed (and oft-cited) relationship in surveys is the result of response bias. We show that people who are more extroverted, efficacious, and opinionated are more likely to respond positively to a question about ...
What are the legal, political, and symbolic differences between categories such as treason, terrorism, insurrection, sedition, and hate crimes? How does the law conceptualize such violence in comparison with violence by the state?