Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • My Account Login
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 29 July 2021

The evolution of our understanding of human development over the last 10 years

  • Ali H. Brivanlou   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1761-280X 1 &
  • Norbert Gleicher   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-0202-4167 2 , 3 , 4  

Nature Communications volume  12 , Article number:  4615 ( 2021 ) Cite this article

5760 Accesses

4 Citations

1 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Developmental biology
  • Embryogenesis

As it fulfills an irresistible need to understand our own origins, research on human development occupies a unique niche in scientific and medical research. In this Comment, we explore the progress in our understanding of human development over the past 10 years. The focus is on basic research, clinical applications, and ethical considerations.

What basic research has taught us about human development

Over the last decade, progress in understanding our own development was mostly driven by the emergence and combination of remarkable new technologies. New molecular biology tools such as single-cell RNA-sequencing (sc-RNA-seq) unveiled the earliest genetic signature of the three cell lineages of the human blastocyst and allowed for the discovery of human-specific signatures 1 , 2 , 3 . CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing has offered further access to in vitro functional studies in human blastocysts 4 . However, as we discuss below, an ethical line was crossed when a group claimed that genetically modified human embryos had been transferred, leading to births 5 when neither public opinion nor a consensus within the scientific community had been reached regarding whether crossing the germline in in vitro fertilization (IVF) was safe and ethically acceptable.

On the embryology side, the development of an in vitro attachment platform for human blastocysts offered a first glance into post-implantation events up to 12 days 1 , 3 , 5 , 6 . This paved the way for several important discoveries, including the observation that the human embryo can self-organize to generate embryonic and extraembryonic germ layers, yolk sac, and amniotic cavities in the absence of maternal influences 5 , 6 ; and the presence of a transient embryonic tissue of trophectodermal lineage, adjacent to the yolk sac, therefore named, yolk-sac trophectoderm ( ysTE ) 5 . The presence of these seemingly human-specific populations was independently confirmed by sc-RNA-seq 1 .

The marriage of stem cell biology with bioengineering gave birth to the field of synthetic embryology 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 . This technology uses human embryonic stem cells (hESCs) cultured on geometrically confined micropatterned substrates to generate 2D in vitro models of human conceptuses, such as models of the gastrula ( gastruloids ) 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , or parts of the embryo, such as cerebroids and neuruloids 14 . Thousands of nearly identical self-organizing human embryonic structures allow for standardization and reproducibility, which cannot be achieved in standard organoid structures 15 . Cells within these structures can be tracked and quantified in real time with sub-cellular resolution, using sophisticated quantification code, including artificial intelligence 14 .

Human gastruloids induce formation of the primitive streak and have enabled the deciphering of the molecular network underlying gastrulation—the most crucial moment of our lives 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 . 3D models of human epiblasts can spontaneously break axial symmetry, thus providing an assay for the elucidation of molecular events underlying the emergence of antero–posterior polarity 11 , 16 . A highly homogenous population of self-organizing 3D models of amniotic ectoderm-like cells can be obtained by combining microfluidic and microculture approaches 17 .

Finally, the development of interspecies chimeras provided the most stringent in vivo validation of human embryo models 9 , 10 , 18 . Unimaginable in human models, inter-species chimeras have become the next best choice to test whether hESC behavior in self-organizing gastruloids , as observed on microchips, would also occur in an embryonic environment 10 , 18 , 19 . Human/bird chimeras generated from transplanting human gastruloids into early chick embryos in ovo unexpectedly proved more efficient than previous methods 9 , 19 . They allowed for the observation of an entire self-organizing embryonic axis in bird eggs 9 . As birds are closer to dinosaurs than to humans, this high rate of success with these chimeras further suggested that these early patterning events must be highly conserved.

Translational clinical applications that arose from basic research

The past 10 years bore witness to significant clinical progress in reproductive medicine, often translated from basic research. Successful human uterus transplantation and the subsequent birth of healthy offspring was, for example, only achieved after years of meticulous laboratory work in animals 10 . Significant improvements in cryopreservation technology for human eggs and ovarian tissue were also preceded by research in model systems 10 , 20 . Practical clinical applications have been developed for women in need of cancer treatment that are toxic to ovaries. In these cases, oocytes and/or ovarian tissue can be cryopreserved for later use in fertility treatments once the patient is cured of her cancer 21 . This ever-evolving technology has already proven to result in live births, and has also become an integral part of routine infertility treatments with IVF, giving rise to the brand-new concept of fertility extension through egg-freezing.

Diagnostic technologies to assess retrieved eggs and preimplantation-stage embryos in the IVF process have been disappointing. For example, tracking extended embryo culture to blastocyst-stage with time-lapse imaging failed to improve embryo selection 22 . That chromosomal-abnormal embryos increase with maternal (but not paternal) age has been interpreted to mean that chromosomal abnormalities were a principal cause for lower implantation chances and higher miscarriage risks among older women. This assumption led to the rapidly growing utilization of chromosomal testing of human embryos prior to embryo transfer in a procedure recently renamed preimplantation genetic testing for aneuploidy (PGT-A) 23 . The hypothesis behind PGT-A is to exclude chromosomal-abnormal embryos from the transfer, thereby improving implantation potentials of remaining euploid embryos.

Here too, clinical evidence was unable to confirm the hypothesis 24 . Moreover, basic research demonstrated a self-correction mechanism in mouse 25 and human embryos 26 , 27 , 28 , 29 that arose during embryogenesis that was cell lineage-specific to the embryonic cell lineage. In contrast, PGT-A biopsies are obtained from the extraembryonic-derived trophectoderm, rendering any diagnostic procedure at the blastocyst stage ineffective. In addition, mathematical modeling demonstrated that results from a single trophectoderm biopsy could not be extrapolated to the whole embryo 30 . Transfer of PGT-A “chromosomal-abnormal diagnosed embryos” has resulted in the births of over 400 chromosomal-normal offspring 20 , 21 .

In recent years, increasing attention has also been given to the quickly evolving understanding of how interdependent lifestyle and human fertility are 31 , 32 , 33 , including the influence of diet on the microbiome, as in many other areas of medicine.

The ethical significance of understanding human development

Whether in clinical medicine or in the research laboratory, human embryology has remained an ethical minefield, strongly influenced by socio-political and religious considerations. At the core of the controversy resides the special moral value of the human embryo, a subject that has come to the forefront again with the ascent of human embryonic stem cell research 34 . There is, however, little consensus as to how to answer a previously raised question: “ what is an embryo ?” 35 . The term pre-embryo, first introduced in 1986, was defined as the interval up to the appearance of the primitive streak, which marks biological individuation at ~14 days post-fertilization. This definition designated the period beyond 14 days as the time when a pre-embryo attains special moral status 36 , 37 . Paradoxically, the term pre-embryo has been replaced by the indiscriminate use of the term embryo, whether at preimplantation cleavage or blastocyst-stages or post-implantation before day 14. It was suggested that the distinction was important for ethical, moral, and biological relevance. The principal reason is simple: Until a pre-embryo becomes an embryo, there is no way of knowing whether implantation has taken place, whether a pregnancy is developing, whether there is a single pregnancy or twinning, or whether fertilization ended up in a benign (hydatidiform mole) or even in a malignant tumor (choriocarcinoma) 35 . Assigning advanced moral value to embryos at those early stages is, therefore, difficult to defend.

The past 10 years have witnessed innumerous ethical debates related to this subject, each with its own social, historical, and religious justifications, reflecting cultural diversities in human populations. Most are triggered by scientific breakthroughs. We summarize here the major ethical challenges preoccupying reproductive research and clinical practice.

We have already briefly referred to CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing. While the use of sc-RNA-seq to identify the molecular blueprint of human development has not elicited significant controversy, CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing of human embryos has been a topic of intense discussions and is currently permissible only in vitro 38 . An alleged attempt in China of implanting human genome-edited embryos into the uterus supposedly led to two births (one a twin birth). Though widely discussed in the media, neither attempt was published in the medical literature, and therefore cannot be verified 5 , 38 .

The ethical debates surrounding the 14-day rule, quiescent since the early IVF days, experienced a rebirth that was prompted by in vitro human embryo attachment studies and the emergence of synthetic human embryos. Within this context, we note that self-organizing embryo models are nothing more than cells in culture and are certainly not embryos. Regardless of scientific merits, in the U.S., the National Institutes of Health (NIH) currently prohibits the use of public funds for the study of synthetic embryos “for ethical reasons”. After being under an NIH moratorium for more than a year, research on chimeras is now, however, again permitted, though human/non-human primate chimeras remain prohibited.

These ongoing ethical debates mostly also mirror those surrounding the lack of U.S. federal funding for clinical IVF and related research, as well as hESCs-derived model embryos. In this context, the American Society for Reproductive Medicine (ASRM)’s Ethics in Embryo Research Task Force recently made an important statement: “ Scientific research using human embryos advances human health and provides vital insights into reproduction and disease ” 39 .

Provided certain guidelines and safeguards are followed, research with already existing embryos or embryos specifically produced for research should be ethically acceptable as a means of obtaining new knowledge that may benefit human health. ASRM also pointed out that scientists and society must understand which research questions necessitate the use of human embryos.

It is gratifying to acknowledge the history and vitality of ongoing debates, especially since they increasingly mimic decision-making processes in the medical field. These debates are meant to be based on cost-benefit and/or risk-benefit assessments. These debates will, unquestionably, continue and, indeed, considering that every intervention has consequences, must be decided based on careful considerations, including all relevant stakeholders and all parts of society.

Zhou, F. et al. Reconstituting the transcriptome and DNA methylome landscapes of human implantation. Nature 572 , 660–664 (2019).

Article   ADS   CAS   Google Scholar  

Petropoulos, S. et al. Single-cell RNA-Seq reveals lineage and X chromosome dynamics in human preimplantation embryos. Cell 167 , 285 (2016).

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Xiang, L. et al. A developmental landscape of 3D-cultured human pre-gastrulation embryos. Nature 577 , 537–542 (2020).

Ma, H. et al. Correction of a pathogenic gene mutation in human embryos. Nature 548 , 413–419 (2017).

Deglincerti, A. et al. Self-organization of the in vitro attached human embryo. Nature 533 , 251–254 (2016).

Shahbazi, M. N. et al. Self-organization of the human embryo in the absence of maternal tissues. Nat. Cell Biol. 18 , 700–708 (2016).

Warmflash, A., Sorre, B., Etoc, F., Siggia, E. D. & Brivanlou, A. H. A method to recapitulate early embryonic spatial patterning in human embryonic stem cells. Nat. Methods 11 , 847–854 (2014).

Etoc, F. et al. A balance between secreted inhibitors and edge sensing controls gastruloid self-organization. Developmental Cell 39 , 302–315 (2016).

Martyn, I., Kanno, T. Y., Ruzo, A., Siggia, E. D. & Brivanlou, A. H. Self-organization of a human organizer by combined Wnt and Nodal signalling. Nature 558 , 132–135 (2018).

Yoney, A. et al. WNT signaling memory is required for ACTIVIN to function as a morphogen in human gastruloids. Elife 7 , e38279 (2018).

Simunovic, M. & Brivanlou, A. H. Embryoids, organoids and gastruloids: new approaches to understanding embryogenesis. Development 144 , 976–985 (2017).

Martyn, I., Brivanlou, A. H. & Siggia, E. D. A wave of WNT signaling balanced by secreted inhibitors controls primitive streak formation in micropattern colonies of human embryonic stem cells. Development 146 , dev172791 (2019).

Deglincerti, A. et al. Self-organization of human embryonic stem cells on micropatterns. Nat. Protoc. 11 , 2223–2232 (2016).

Haremaki, T. et al. Self-organizing neuruloids model developmental aspects of Huntington’s disease in the ectodermal compartment. Nat. Biotechnol. 37 , 1198–1208 (2019).

Lancaster, M. A. et al. Cerebral organoids model human brain development and microcephaly. Nature 501 , 373–379 (2013).

Simunovic, M. et al. A 3D model of a human epiblast reveals BMP4-driven symmetry breaking. Nat. Cell Biol. 21 , 900–910 (2019).

Zheng, Y. et al. Controlled modelling of human epiblast and amnion development using stem cells. Nature 573 , 421–425 (2019).

Levine, S. & Grabel, L. The contribution of human/non-human animal chimeras to stem cell research. Stem. Cell Res. 24 , 128–134 (2017).

Article   Google Scholar  

James, D., Noggle, S. A., Swigut, T. & Brivanlou, A. H. Contribution of human embryonic stem cells to mouse blastocysts. Dev. Biol. 295 , 90–102 (2006).

ESHRE Working Group on Time-Lapse Technology, et al. Good practice recommendations for the use of time-lapse technology. Hum. Reprod. Open 2020 , hoaa008 (2020).

Homer, H. A. Preimplantation genetic testing for aneuploidy (PGT-A): the biology, the technology and the clinical outcomes. Aust. N. Z. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 59 , 317–324 (2019).

Castellón, L. A. R. et al. The history behind successful uterine transplantation in humans. JBRA Assist. Reprod. 21 , 126–134 (2017).

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Saragusty, J. & Arav, A. Current progress in oocyte and embryo cryopreservation by slow freezing and vitrification. Reproduction 141 , 1–19 (2011).

Bracewell-Milnes, T., Norman-Taylor, J. & Nikolaou, D. Social egg freezing should be offered to single women approaching their late thirties: AGAINST: Women should be freezing their eggs earlier. BJOG 125 , 1580 (2018).

Rito, T., Naftaly, J., Gleicher, N. & Brivanlou, A. H. Self-correction of aneuploidy in human blastocysts and self-organizing gastruloids. Fertil. Steril. 112 , e127 (2019).

Chu, W. et al. Metagenomic analysis identified microbiome alterations and pathological association between intestinal microbiota and polycystic ovary syndrome. Fertil. Steril. 113 , 1286–1298 (2020). e1284.

Hug, K. Therapeutic perspectives of human embryonic stem cell reserach versus the moreal status of a human embryo- does one have to be compromised for the other? Medicina 42 , 107–114 (2006).

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Jones, H. W. Jr & Veeck, L. What is an embryo? Fertil. Steril. 77 , 658–659 (2002).

American Society for Reproductive Medicine. Ethical considerations of the new reproductive technologies. Ethics Comm. Opin. Fertil. Steril. 46 , 1–93 (1986).

Google Scholar  

Loren, A. W. et al. Fertility preservation for patients with cancer: American Society of Clinical Oncology clinical practice guideline update. J. Clin. Oncol. 31 , 2500–2510 (2013).

Gleicher, N. et al. The 2019 PGDIS position statement on transfer of mosaic embryos within a context of new information on PGT-A. Reprod. Biol. Endocrin 18 , 57 (2020).

Gleicher, N. et al. A single trophectoderm biopsy at blastocyst stage is mathematically unable to determine embryo ploidy accurately enough for clinical use. Reprod. Biol. Endocrinol. 15 , 33 (2017).

Patrizio, P. et al. Worldwide live births following the transfer of chromosomally “Abnormal” embryos after PGT/A: results of a worldwide web-based survey. J. Assist Reprod. Genet. 36 , 1599–1607 (2019).

Barmat, L. I. et al. Human preembryo development on autologous endometrial coculture versus conventional medium. Fertil. Steril. 70 , 1109–1113 (1998).

National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Examining the State of the Science of Mammalian Embryo Model Systems: Proceedings of a Workshop (2020).

Abbott, A. et al. 2016 in news: the science events that shaped the year. Nat. N. 540 , 496 (2016).

Gleicher, N., Kushnir, V. A., Albertini, D. A. & Barad, D. H. First birth following spindle transfer. Reprod. BioMedicine Online 35 , 542–543 (2017).

Research CfBEa. Therapeutic Cloning and Genome Modification (FDA, 2019).

van den Brink, S. C. et al. Symmetry breaking, germ layer specification and axial organisation in aggregates of mouse embryonic stem cells. Development 141 , 4231–4242 (2014).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We like to thank Min Yang, Jean Marx Santel, Adam Souza, and Amir Brivanlou, for data gathering and critical reading of the manuscript, and constructive criticism.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Stem Cell Biology and Molecular Embryology Laboratory, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, USA

Ali H. Brivanlou

The Center for Human Reproduction, New York, NY, USA

  • Norbert Gleicher

The Foundation for Reproductive Medicine, New York, NY, USA

Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

Both A.H.B. and N.G. have contributed to writing the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ali H. Brivanlou .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

A.H.B. and N.G. are co-founders of OvaNova Inc. A.H.B. is a co-founder of Rumi Scientific Inc.

Additional information

Peer review information Nature Communications thanks Alfonso Martinez Arias, Annelien Bredenoord and the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Brivanlou, A.H., Gleicher, N. The evolution of our understanding of human development over the last 10 years. Nat Commun 12 , 4615 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-24793-3

Download citation

Received : 09 June 2020

Accepted : 29 June 2021

Published : 29 July 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-24793-3

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Revisiting selected ethical aspects of current clinical in vitro fertilization (ivf) practice.

  • Anja von Schondorf-Gleicher
  • Lyka Mochizuki

Journal of Assisted Reproduction and Genetics (2022)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

research study on human development

Human Development & Life Course Studies

Human development & life course studies featured projects.

Monitoring the Future: Drug Use and Lifestyles of American Youth (MTF)

Human Development & Life Course Studies Funded Research

  • ACL-LIFE Life History Interview and Validation
  • Health and Well-being Over the Life Course and Across Multiple Generations
  • Household Income and Child Development in the First 3 years of Life – Valhalla Charitable Foundation Funding
  • Michigan Data Policy Fellowship: MEHF/MDHHS
  • Neurodevelopmental Pathways in Adolescent Health Risk Behavior
  • PSID 2021-2024: Continuity and Change in American Economic and Social Life
  • Puerto Rico Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PR-PSID)
  • Racial Disparities in Health: The Roles of Stress, Social Relations and the Cardiovascular System
  • Social Disadvantage and Its Impact on Pathogen Burden and Immune Dysfunction Across the Life Course
  • Spanking and hitting children: Trends and changes in risk factors in consecutive, longitudinal, national samples of parents from 1993-2022
  • The Transition from Childhood into Adulthood among PSID Children, 2021 and 2023
  • Transition to Adulthood within its Life Course & Intergenerational Family Context
  • Transitions from Preschool through High School: Family, Schools & Neighborhoods

Human Development & Life Course Studies Publications

  • Daniel P Keating . 2024. Adolescent Health Risk Behavior: The Road Ahead . Journal of Adolescent Health 74 (3) :397-399.   
  • Berenice Castillo, Schulenberg,John E, Andrew Grogan-Kaylor, Paul A. Toro . 2023. The prevalence and correlates of running away among adolescents in the United States . Journal of Community Psychology 51 (5) :1860-1875.   
  • Justin Jager, Amy Rauer, Jeremy Staff, Jennifer E. Lansford, Gregory S. Petit, Schulenberg,John E . 2023. The destabilization and destandardization of social roles across the adult life course: Considering aggregate social role instability and its variability from a historical-developmental perspective . Developmental Psychology 58 (3) :589-605.   
  • Erin Dunn, Daniel S. Busso, Kathryn A. Davis, Andrew D.A.C. Smith, Colter Mitchell , Henning Tiemeier, Ezra S. Susser . 2023. Sensitive periods for the effect of child maltreatment on psychopathology symptoms in adolescence . Complex Psychiatry 9 (1-4) :145-153.   
  • Frank, Colleen C., Mundy, Lindsey M., Jacqui Smith . 2023. Life course engagement in enriching activities: When and how does it matter for cognitive aging? . Psychology and Aging 38 (4) :263-276.   
  • Hardi, Felicia A., Goetschius, Leigh G., Tillem, Scott, McLoyd, Vonnie, Brooks-Gunn, Jeanne, Boone, Montana, Lopez-Duran, Nestor, Colter Mitchell , Luke Williamson Hyde , Christopher Stephen Monk . 2023. Early childhood household instability, adolescent structural neural network architecture, and young adulthood depression: A 21-year longitudinal study . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 61 :101253.   
  • Alexandre A Lussier, Yiwen Zhu, Brooke J Smith, Janine Cerutti, Fisher,Jonah, Phillip E Melton, Natasha M Wood, Sarah Cohen-Woods, Rae-Chi Huang, Colter Mitchell , Lisa Schneper, Daniel A Notterman, Andrew J Simpkin, Andrew D A C Smith, Matthew J Suderman, Esther Walton, Caroline L Relton, Kerry J Ressler, Erin C Dunn . 2023. Association between the timing of childhood adversity and epigenetic patterns across childhood and adolescence: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) prospective cohort . The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health 7 (8) :532-543.   
  • Huang, Cher X., Halfon, Neal, Narayan Sastry , Chung, Paul J., Schickedanz, Adam . 2023. Positive Childhood Experiences and Adult Health Outcomes . Pediatrics 152 (1) :e2022060951.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Felicia A. Hardi . 2023. Poverty, Brain Development, and Mental Health: Progress, Challenges, and Paths Forward . Annual Reviews 5
  • Sydney R. Wilhoite, Afsara B. Zaheed, Jordan D. Palms, Emily P. Morris, Ketlyne Sol , Alexa Martino, Laura Beth Zahodne . 2023. Mechanisms underlying the association between adverse childhood experiences and racial disparities in later-life cognition . Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society
  • John M Felt, Karra D Harrington, Nilam Ram, Kieran J O’Donnell, Martin J Sliwinski, Lizbeth 'Libby' Benson , Zhenyu Zhang, Michael J Meaney, Frank W Putnam, Jennie G Noll, Chad E Shenk . 2023. Receptive Language Abilities for Females Exposed to Early Life Adversity: Modification by Epigenetic Age Acceleration at Midlife in a 30-Year Prospective Cohort Study . The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 78 (4) :585-595.   
  • Walshe, Elizabeth A., Michael R Elliott , Cheng, Shukai, Romer, Daniel, Curry, Allison E., Grethlein, David, Gonzalez, Alexander K., Winston, Flaura K. . 2023. Driving Skills at Licensure and Time to First Crash . Pediatrics 152 (5) :e2022060817.   
  • Elizabeth S. Barnert, Lindsay M. Schlichte, Destiny G. Tolliver, Jaime La Charite, Christopher Biely, Rebecca Dudovitz, Kathryn Leifheit, Shirley Russ, Narayan Sastry , Cecile Yama, George M. Slavich, Adam Schickedanz . 2023. Parents’ Childhood Experiences and Offspring Involvement With the Legal System . JAMA Network Open 6 (10) :e2339648-e2339648.   
  • La Charite, Jaime, Khan, Mariam, Dudovitz, Rebecca, Nuckols, Teryl, Narayan Sastry , Huang, Cher, Lei, Yvonne, Schickedanz, Adam . 2023. Specific domains of positive childhood experiences (PCEs) associated with improved adult health: A nationally representative study . SSM - Population Health 24 :101558.   
  • Schmitz, Lauren L, Duffie, Elizabeth, Wei Zhao , Ratliff, Scott M, Ding, Jingzhong, Liu, Yongmei, Merkin, Sharon Stein, Jennifer Ann Smith , Seeman, Teresa . 2023. Associations of Early-Life Adversity With Later-Life Epigenetic Aging Profiles in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis . American Journal of Epidemiology 192 (12) :1991-2005.   
  • Nkwata,Allan Kizza, Jacqui Smith . 2023. Early learning difficulties, childhood stress, race, and risk of cognitive impairment among US adults over age 50: A cross-sectional analysis . Health Science Reports 6 (12) :e1756.   
  • Jordan D. Palms, Afsara B. Zaheed, Emily P. Morris, Alexa Martino, Lindsey Meister, Ketlyne Sol , Laura Beth Zahodne . 2023. Links between early-life contextual factors and later-life cognition and the role of educational attainment . Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 29 (8) :734-741.   
  • Michael I. Demidenko, Edward Huntley , Alexander S. Weigard, Daniel P Keating , Adriene M. Beltz . 2022. Neural heterogeneity underlying late adolescent motivational processing is linked to individual differences in behavioral sensation seeking . Journal of Neuroscience Research
  • Josh Ehrlich , Jenna Goldstein, Bonnie K. Swenor, Heather Whitson, Kenneth M Langa , Philip Todd Veliz . 2022. Addition of Vision Impairment to a Life-Course Model of Potentially Modifiable Dementia Risk Factors in the US . JAMA Neurology 79 (6) :623-626.   
  • Songqi Liu, Peter Bamberger, Mo Wang, Inbal Nahum-Shani , Mary Larimer, Samuel B. Bacharach . 2022. Behavior change versus stability during the college-to-work transition: Life course and the “stickiness” of alcohol misuse at career entry . Personnel Psychology
  • Marilyn Sinkewicz , Ola S Rostant , Kara Zivin , Ryan Jay McCammon , Philippa J Clarke . 2022. A life course view on depression: Social determinants of depressive symptom trajectories over 25 Years of Americans’ Changing Lives . SSM - Population Health 18 :101125.   
  • Sammy Ahmed, Alexa Ellis, Kaitlin P. Ward, Natasha Chaku, Pamela Davis-Kean . 2022. Working memory development from early childhood to adolescence using two nationally representative samples . Developmental Psychology 58 (10) :1962-1973.   
  • Daniel P Keating , Demidenko, Michael I. , Kelly, Dominic P. . 2022. Cognition in adolescence and the transition to adulthood . APA handbook of adolescent and young adult development :75-90.   
  • Lisa J. Crockett, Gustavo Carlo, Schulenberg,John E . 2022. APA Handbook of Adolescent and Young Adult Development .
  • Freida A. Blostein, Fisher,Jonah, John Dou, Lisa Schneper, Erin Bakshis Ware , Daniel A. Notterman, Colter Mitchell , Kelly M. Bakulski . 2022. Polymethylation scores for prenatal maternal smoke exposure persist until age 15 and are detected in saliva in the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing cohort . Epigenetics 17 (13) :2223-2240.   
  • Cope Feurer, Jennifer H. Suor, Jagan Jimmy, Heide Klumpp, Christopher Stephen Monk , K. Luan Phan, Katie L. Burkhouse . 2021. Differences in cortical thinning across development among individuals with and without anxiety disorders . Depression and Anxiety 38 (3) :372-381.   
  • Juliet Haarbauer-Krupa, Lydie A. Lebrun-Harris, Lindsey I. Black, Philip Todd Veliz , Jill Daugherty, Rebecca Desrocher, John E Schulenberg , Diane Pilkey, Matthew Breiding . 2021. Comparing prevalence estimates of concussion/head injury in U.S. children and adolescents in national surveys. . Annals of Epidemiology 54 :11-20.   
  • Pamela Davis-Kean , Tighe,Lauren Adams, Waters,Nicholas . 2021. The Role of Parent Educational Attainment in Parenting and Children’s Development . Current Directions in Psychological Science 30 (2) :186-192.   
  • Emily Bilek, Rachel C. Tomlinson, Andrew S. Whiteman, Timothy D. Johnson, Chelsea Benedict, K. Luan Phan, Christopher Stephen Monk , Kate D. Fitzgerald . 2021. Exposure-Focused CBT Outperforms Relaxation-Based Control in an RCT of Treatment for Child and Adolescent Anxiety . Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology
  • B. Chaarani, S. Hahn, N. Allgaier, S. Adise, M. M. Owens, A. C. Juliano, D. K. Yuan, H. Loso, A. Ivanciu, M. D. Albaugh, J. Dumas, S. Mackey, J. Laurent, M. Ivanova, D. J. Hagler, M. D. Cornejo, S. Hatton, A. Agrawal, L. Aguinaldo . 2021. Baseline brain function in the preadolescents of the ABCD Study . Nature Neuroscience 24 :1176–1186.   
  • Waters,Nicholas, Ahmed,Sammy F, Tang,Sandra, Frederick J Morrison , Pamela Davis-Kean . 2021. Pathways from socioeconomic status to early academic achievement: The role of specific executive functions . Early Childhood Research Quarterly 54 :321-331.   
  • Stefanie Mollborn, Paula Fomby , Goode,Joshua, Adenife Modile . 2021. A life course framework for understanding digital technology use in the transition to adulthood . Advances in Life Course Research 47 :100379.   
  • Paula Fomby , Ariane Ophir, Marcia J. Carlson . 2021. Family Complexity and Children’s Behavior Problems over Two U.S. Cohorts . Journal of Marriage and Family 83 (2) :340-357.   
  • Emily Treleaven , Caroline Whidden, Faith Cole, Kassoum Kayentao, Mohamed Bana Traoré, Djoumé Diakité, Seydou Sidibé, Tracy Kuo Lin, David Boettiger, Souleymane Cissouma, Vincent Sanogo, Nancy Padian, Ari Johnson, Jenny Liu . 2021. Relationship between symptoms, barriers to care and healthcare utilisation among children under five in rural Mali . Tropical Medicine & International Health 26 (8) :943-952.   
  • Toni M. Walker, Paul J. Frick, Tatiana M. Matlasz, Emily L. Robertson, Amy J. Mikolajewski, Colter Mitchell , Nestor Lopez-Duran, Christopher Stephen Monk , Luke Williamson Hyde . 2021. Psychometric Properties of a Semistructured Interview to Assess Limited Prosocial Emotions . Assessment (28) :1755-1764.   
  • Arianna M. Gard, Andrea M. Maxwell, Daniel S. Shaw, Colter Mitchell , Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Sara S. McLanahan, Erika E. Forbes, Christopher Stephen Monk , Luke Williamson Hyde . 2021. Beyond family-level adversities: Exploring the developmental timing of neighborhood disadvantage effects on the brain . Developmental Science 24 (1) :e12985.   
  • Arianna M. Gard, Tyler C. Hein, Colter Mitchell , Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Sarah S. McLanahan, Christopher Stephen Monk , Luke W. Hyde . 2021. Prospective longitudinal associations between harsh parenting and corticolimbic function during adolescence . Development and Psychopathology
  • William Magee, Michael R Elliott , Marilyn Sinkewicz, Jessica Marie Finlay , Megan E Patrick . 2021. Who looks on the bright side? Optimistic and pessimistic perceptual-response reflexes over American adulthood . Advances in Life Course Research 51 :100452.   
  • Philip Todd Veliz , Sean Esteban McCabe , James T. Eckner, John E Schulenberg . 2021. Trends in the Prevalence of Concussion Reported by US Adolescents, 2016-2020 . JAMA 325 (17) :1789-1791.   
  • Jacqui Smith , Marina Larkina . 2021. Early-life Health as a Lifelong Precursor of Self-Related Views of Aging in Later Life . The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 76 (5) :894-899.   
  • Abigail G. Richburg, Dominic P. Kelly, Pamela Davis-Kean , Abby Richburg . 2021. Depression, Anxiety, and Pubertal Timing: Current Research and Future Directions . University of Michigan Undergraduate Research Journal 15
  • Schickedanz, Adam, José J. Escarce, Halfon, Neal, Narayan Sastry , Chung, Paul J. . 2021. Intergenerational Associations between Parents’ and Children’s Adverse Childhood Experience Scores . Children 8 (9) :747.   
  • Rachel S. Bergmans, Jacqui Smith . 2021. Associations of mental health and chronic physical illness during childhood with major depression in later life . Aging & Mental Health 26 (9) :1813-1820.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Weybright, Elizabeth H, Caldwell, Linda L . 2020. More Bored Today Than Yesterday? National Trends in Adolescent Boredom From 2008 to 2017 . Journal of Adolescent Health 66 (3) :360-365.   
  • Sean Esteban McCabe , Tonda L. Hughes, Brady Thomas West , Rebecca J. Evans-Polce, Phil T Veliz, Kara Dickinson, Vita V. McCabe, Carol J. Boyd . 2020. Sexual Orientation, Adverse Childhood Experiences, and Comorbid DSM-5 Substance Use and Mental Health Disorders . The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 81 (6)
  • Tang,Sandra, Megan E Patrick . 2020. A latent class analysis of adolescents’ technology and interactive social media use: Associations with academics and substance use . Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies 2 (1) :50-60.   
  • Goode,Joshua, Paula Fomby , Stefanie Mollborn, Aubrey Limburg . 2020. Children’s Technology Time in Two US Cohorts . Child Indicators Research 13 :1107-1132.   
  • Robert Bozick, Narayan Sastry , Airan Liu . 2020. Health in Early Adolescence and Paid Employment . Youth & Society
  • Corina Benjet, William G Axinn , Hermosilla,Sabrina, Schulz,Paul Chapin, Cole,Faith, Laura Sampson, Dirgha Jibi Ghimire . 2020. Exposure to Armed Conflict in Childhood vs Older Ages and Subsequent Onset of Major Depressive Disorder . JAMA Network Open 3 (11) :e2019848-e2019848.   
  • Luke Williamson Hyde , Gard, Arianna Morgan, Rachel C. Tomlinson, S. Alexandra Burt, Colter Mitchell , Christopher Stephen Monk . 2020. An ecological approach to understanding the developing brain: Examples linking poverty, parenting, neighborhoods, and the brain . American Psychologist 75 (9) :1245-1259.   
  • Neika Sharifian, Ketlyne Sol , Laura Beth Zahodne , Toni C Antonucci . 2020. Social Relationships and Adaptation in Later Life . Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology
  • Michael I. Demidenko, Edward Huntley , Andrew Jahn, Moriah E. Thomason, Christopher Stephen Monk , Daniel P Keating . 2020. Cortical and subcortical response to the anticipation of reward in high and average/low risk-taking adolescents . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 44 :100798.   
  • Edward Huntley , Hilary A. Marusak, Sarah E. Berman, Clara G. Zundel, Joshua R.B. Hatfield, Daniel P Keating , Christine A. Rabinak . 2020. Adolescent substance use and functional connectivity between the ventral striatum and hippocampus . Behavioural Brain Research 390 :112678.   
  • M. Elizabeth Zinn, Edward Huntley , Daniel P Keating . 2020. Resilience in adolescence: Prospective Self moderates the association of early life adversity with externalizing problems . Journal of Adolescence 81 :61-72.   
  • Leigh G. Goetschius, Tyler C. Hein, Sara S. McLanahan, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Vonnie C. McLoyd, Hailey L. Dotterer, Nestor Lopez-Duran, Colter Mitchell , Luke Williamson Hyde , Christopher Stephen Monk , Adriene M. Beltz . 2020. Association of Childhood Violence Exposure With Adolescent Neural Network Density . JAMA Network Open 3 (9) :e2017850-e2017850.   
  • Leigh G. Goetschius, Tyler C. Hein, Colter Mitchell , Nestor L. Lopez-Duran, Vonnie C. McLoyd, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Sara S. McLanahan, Luke Williamson Hyde , Christopher Stephen Monk . 2020. Childhood violence exposure and social deprivation predict adolescent amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex white matter connectivity . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 45 :100849.   
  • Jenalee R. Doom, Melissa K. Peckins, Tyler C. Hein, Hailey L. Dotterer, Colter Mitchell , Nestor L. Lopez-Duran, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Sara McLanahan, Luke Williamson Hyde , James L. Abelson, Christopher Stephen Monk . 2020. Differential associations of parental harshness and parental disengagement with overall cortisol output at 15 years: Implications for adolescent mental health . Development and Psychopathology
  • Tang,Sandra, Megan E Patrick . 2020. A latent class analysis of adolescents’ technology and interactive social media use: Associations with academics and substance use . Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies 2 (1) :50-60.   
  • Laura Wray-Lake, Erin H. Arruda, John E Schulenberg . 2020. Civic development across the transition to adulthood in a national U.S. sample: Variations by race/ethnicity, parent education, and gender . Developmental Psychology 56 (10) :1948-1967.   
  • Tang,Sandra, Pamela Davis-Kean , Waters, Nicholas, Ahmed, Sammy F . 2019. Executive function and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations from early childhood to adolescence . Journal of educational psychology 111 (3) :446-458.   
  • Paula Fomby , Narayan Sastry . 2019. Data Collection on Sensitive Topics with Adolescents Using Interactive Voice Response Technology . methods, data, analyses 13 (1)
  • Edward Huntley , Daniel P Keating , Maslowsky, Julie , Owotomo, Olusegun . 2019. Adolescent Risk Behavior: Differentiating Reasoned And Reactive Risk-taking . Journal of Youth and Adolescence
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Peters, Amy T, Burkhouse, Katie L, Kujawa, Autumn , Afshar, Kaveh , Fitzgerald, Kate D, Hajcak, Greg , Phan, K L . 2019. Impact of pubertal timing and depression on error-related brain activity in anxious youth . Developmental psychobiology 61 (1) :69-80.   
  • Edward Huntley , Hehr, Aneesh , Marusak, Hilary A, Rabinak, Christine A . 2019. Effects of Duration and Midpoint of Sleep on Corticolimbic Circuitry in Youth . Chronic Stress 3
  • Edward Huntley , Daniel P Keating , Demidenko, Michael I, Martz, Meghan E . 2019. Adolescent Health Risk Behaviors: Convergent, Discriminant and Predictive Validity of Self-Report and Cognitive Measures . Journal of Youth and Adolescence 48 (9) :1765-1783.   
  • Luke Williamson Hyde , Colter Mitchell , Christopher Stephen Monk , Peckins, Melissa K, Roberts, Andrea G, Hein, Tyler C, Brooks-Gunn, Jeanne , McLanahan, Sara S, Lopez-Duran, Nestor L . 2019. Violence exposure and social deprivation is associated with cortisol reactivity in urban adolescents . Psychoneuroendocrinology 111 :104426.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kristine J Ajrouch , Noah J Webster . 2019. Convoys of social relations: Cohort similarities and differences over 25 years . Psychology and Aging 34 (8) :1158-1169.   
  • Pamela Davis-Kean , Alexa Ellis . 2019. An overview of issues in infant and developmental research for the creation of robust and replicable science . Infant Behavior and Development 57 :101339.   
  • Jennifer L. Maggs, Jeremy Staff, Megan E Patrick , Laura Wray-Lake . 2019. Very early drinking: Event history models predicting alcohol use initiation from age 4 to 11 years . Addictive Behaviors 89 :121-127.   
  • Donald J. Hagler Jr., ..., John E Schulenberg , et al. . 2019. Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study . NeuroImage 202 :116091.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Toni C Antonucci , Cranford, James A, Manalel, Jasmine A . 2018. Drinking Patterns Among Older Couples: Longitudinal Associations With Negative Marital Quality . The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 73 (4) :655-665.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Megan E Patrick , Deborah Denise Kloska , Martz, Meghan E . 2018. "I Am So Bored!": Prevalence Rates and Sociodemographic and Contextual Correlates of High Boredom Among American Adolescents . Youth & Society 50 (5) :688-710.   
  • Katherine Keyes , Patrick M O'Malley , John E Schulenberg , Gary, Dahsan S, Beardslee, Jordan , Prins, Seth J, Rutherford, Caroline . 2018. Age, period, and cohort effects in conduct problems among American adolescents from 1991 through 2015 . American Journal of Epidemiology 187 (3) :548-557.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Lord, Catherine, Kim, So Hyun, Grammer, Jennie , Benrey, Nurit . 2018. Stimulus processing and error monitoring in more-able kindergarteners with autism spectrum disorder: a short review and a preliminary Event-Related Potentials study . European Journal of Neuroscience 47 (6) :556-567.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , McClelland, Megan , Geldhof, John , GestsdÛttir, Steinunn , Cameron, Claire , Bowers, Ed , Duckworth, Angela , Little, Todd , Grammer, Jennie . 2018. Self-Regulation . Handbook of Life Course Health Development :275-298.   
  • Narayan Sastry , Paula Fomby , Katherine A McGonagle . 2018. Using the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) to Conduct Life Course Health Development Analysis . Handbook of Life Course Health Development :579-599.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Grammer, Jennie K, Gehring, William J . 2018. Associations between developmental changes in error-related brain activity and executive functions in early childhood . Psychophysiology 55 (3) :e13040.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Hartnett, Caroline Sten, Fingerman, Karen L . 2018. Without the ties that bind: U.S. young adults who lack active parental relationships . Advances in Life Course Research 35 :103-113.   
  • Pamela Davis-Kean , Meichu Chen , Olson, Sheryl L, Lansford, Jennifer E, Bates, John E, Pettit, Gregory S, Dodge, Kenneth A . 2018. Mapping the Growth of Heterogeneous Forms of Externalizing Problem Behavior Between Early Childhood and Adolescence: A Comparison of Parent and Teacher Ratings . Journal of abnormal child psychology 46 (5) :935-950.   
  • Meichu Chen , Pamela Davis-Kean , Jackson, James Sidney, Nazroo, James , Zilanawala, Afshin , BÈcares, Laia , Kelly, Yvonne , Panico, Lidia , Sacker, Amanda . 2018. Socioemotional wellbeing of mixed race/ethnicity children in the UK and US: Patterns and mechanisms . SSM - Population Health 5 :147-159.   
  • Barber, Jennifer S, Yasamin Yoko Kusunoki , Heather Gatny , Budnick, Jamie . 2018. The Dynamics of Intimate Partner Violence and the Risk of Pregnancy during the Transition to Adulthood . American Sociological Review 83 (5) :1020-1047.   
  • Colter Mitchell , Luke Williamson Hyde , Christopher Stephen Monk , Hein, Tyler C, Mattson, Whitney I, Dotterer, Hailey L, Lopez-Duran, Nestor , Thomason, Moriah E, Peltier, Scott J, Welsh, Robert C . 2018. Amygdala habituation and uncinate fasciculus connectivity in adolescence: A multi-modal approach . NeuroImage 183 :617-626.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Maslowsky, Julie , Jager, Justin . 2018. Substance Use and Abuse during Adolescence and the Transition to Adulthood are Developmental Phenomena: Conceptual and Empirical Consideration . Alcohol Use Disorders: A Developmental Science Approach to Etiology
  • Megan E Patrick , John E Schulenberg , Rhew, Isaac C, Lewis, Melissa A, Abdallah, Devon A, Larimer, Mary E, Lee, Christine M . 2018. Alcohol motivations and behaviors during months young adults experience social role transitions: Microtransitions in early adulthood . Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 32 (8) :895-903.   
  • Andrew L. Hicks, Mark S. Handcock, Narayan Sastry , Anne R. Pebley . 2018. Sequential Neighborhood Effects: The Effect of Long-Term Exposure to Concentrated Disadvantage on Children’s Reading and Math Test Scores . Demography 55 (1) :1-31.   
  • Tang,Sandra, Megan E Patrick . 2018. Technology and interactive social media use among 8th and 10th graders in the U.S. and associations with homework and school grades . Computers in Human Behavior 86 :34-44.   
  • Adam Schickedanz, Neal Halfon, Narayan Sastry , Paul J. Chung . 2018. Parents’ Adverse Childhood Experiences and Their Children’s Behavioral Health Problems . Pediatrics 142 (2) :e20180023.   
  • Grace Noppert , Candace S. Brown, Marianne Chanti-Ketterl, Katherine S. Hall, L. Kristin Newby, Harvey Jay Cohen, Miriam C. Morey . 2018. The Impact of Multiple Dimensions of Socioeconomic Status on Physical Functioning Across the Life Course . Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine
  • John E Schulenberg , Lloyd D Johnston , Patrick M. O'Malley, Jerald G Bachman , Richard Allen Miech , Megan E Patrick . 2018. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2017. Volume II, College Students & Adults Ages 19-55 .
  • Richard Allen Miech , Lloyd D Johnston , Patrick M. O'Malley, Jerald G Bachman , John E Schulenberg , Megan E Patrick . 2018. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2017. Volume I, Secondary School Students .
  • Megan E Patrick , Jennifer L Maggs , Wray-Lake, Laura . 2017. Early life predictors of alcohol-related attitudes among 11-year-old never drinkers . Addictive Behaviors 66 :26-32.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Polenick, Courtney Allyn, Zarit, Steven H, Bangerter, Lauren R, Seidel, Amber J, Fingerman, Karen L . 2017. Intergenerational Support and Marital Satisfaction: Implications of Beliefs About Helping Aging Parents . Journal of Marriage and Family 79 (1) :131-146.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2017. Discontinuous Patterns of Cigarette Smoking from Age 18 to 50 in the U.S.: A Repeated-Measures Latent Class Analysis . Nicotine & tobacco research 20 (1) :108-116.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Katherine Keyes , Wray-Lake, Laura , Shubert, Jennifer . 2017. The Developmental Course of Community Service Across the Transition to Adulthood in a National U.S. Sample . Developmental psychology 53 (12) :2397-2408.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kira S Birditt , Jacqui Smith , Wan, Wylie H . 2017. Work-Hour Trajectories and Depressive Symptoms Among Midlife and Older Married Couples . Work, Aging and Retirement 4 (1) :108-122.   
  • Lindsay H Ryan , Newton, Nicky J, Chauhan, Preet K, Chopik, William J . 2017. Effects of pre-retirement personality, health and job lock on post-retirement subjective well-being . Translational Issues in Psychological Science 3 (4) :378-387.   
  • Daniel P Keating . 2017. The Evolving Capacities of the Child: Neurodevelopment and Children’s Rights . Handbook of Children's Rights: Global and Multidisciplinary Perspectives :184-199.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Bauldry, Shawn , Shanahan, Michael J, Macmillan, Ross , Boardman, Jason D, O. Dean, Danielle , Cole, Veronica . 2016. Parental and adolescent health behaviors and pathways to adulthood . Social science research 58 :227-242.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , MacNamara, Annmarie , Vergés, Alvaro , Kujawa, Autumn , Fitzgerald, Kate D, Phan, K Luan . 2016. Age-related changes in emotional face processing across childhood and into young adulthood: Evidence from event-related potentials . Developmental psychobiology 58 (1) :27-38.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Richard Allen Miech , Carlier,Carola, Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston , John E Schulenberg . 2016. Self-reported reasons for vaping among 8th, 10th, and 12th graders in the US: Nationally-representative results . Drug and alcohol dependence 165 :275-278.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Sayer, Liana C, Bianchi, Suzanne M . 2016. Gender, Time Use, and Aging . Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences (Eighth Edition) :163-180.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Toni C Antonucci , Tighe, Lauren A . 2016. Intergenerational Ambivalence in Adolescence and Early Adulthood: Implications for Depressive Symptoms Over Time . Developmental psychology 52 (5) :824-834.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Wolff, Jennifer L, Spillman, Brenda C, Kasper, Judith D . 2016. A National Profile of Family and Unpaid Caregivers Who Assist Older Adults With Health Care Activities . Jama Internal Medicine 176 (3) :372-379.   
  • Mary Beth Ofstedal , Zimmer, Zachary , Jagger, Carol , Chiu, Chi-Tsun , Rojo, Florencia , Saito, Yasuhiko . 2016. Spirituality, religiosity, aging and health in global perspective: A review . Social Science and Medicine: Population Health 2 :373-381.   
  • Philippa J Clarke , Guimarães, Joanna M. N., Tate, Denise , Coeli, Claudia Medina, Griep, Rosane Harter, Fonseca, a Jesus Mendes da, Santos, Itamar S, Melo, E Caetano Prates, Chor, Dora . 2016. Social mobility and subclinical atherosclerosis in a middle-income country: Association of intra- and inter-generational social mobility with carotid intima-media thickness in the Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil) . Social science & medicine 169 :9-17.   
  • Schoeni, Bob, Brenner, Allison, Lê-Scherban, Félice . 2016. Childhood family wealth and mental health in a national cohort of young adults . SSM - Population Health 2 :798-806.   
  • Jessica Danielle Faul , David R Weir , Erin Bakshis Ware , ..., et al. . 2016. Genetic variants linked to education predict longevity . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113 (47) :13366-13371.   
  • Christopher Joseph Trentacosta , Pamela Davis-Kean , Colter Mitchell , Luke Williamson Hyde , Dolinoy, Dana . 2016. Environmental Contaminants and Child Development . Child Development Perspectives 10 (4) :228-233.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Wu, Minjie , Kujawa, Autumn , Lu, Lisa H, Fitzgerald, Daniel A, Klumpp, Heide , Fitzgerald, Kate D, Phan, K L . 2016. Age-related changes in amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotional face processing from childhood into young adulthood . Human brain mapping 37 (5) :1684-1695.   
  • Steven G Heeringa , Zhu, Motao , Rudisill, Toni M, Swedler, David , Redelmeier, Donald A . 2016. The association between handheld phone bans and the prevalence of handheld phone conversations among young drivers in the United States . Annals of Epidemiology 26 (12) :833–837.e1.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Montroy, Janelle J, Bowles, Ryan P, Skibbe, Lori E, McClelland, Megan M . 2016. The development of self-regulation across early childhood . Developmental psychology 52 (11) :1744-1762.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Lisa F. Berkman, Axel Borsch-Supan, Laura L. Carstensen, Linda P. Fried, Frank F. Furstenberg, Dana Goldman, Jackson, James Sidney, Martin Kohli, S. Jay Olshansky, John Rother, John W. Rowe, Julie Zissimopoulos . 2016. Society and the individual at the dawn of the 21st Century . Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, Eighth Edition :41-62.   
  • Kristine J Ajrouch , Toni C Antonucci , Abdulrahim, S . 2015. Family relations and health over the life course. A Lebanese perspective . Le Journal medical libanais.The Lebanese medical journal 63 (1) :8-14.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kristine J Ajrouch , Abdulrahim, S . 2015. Social Relations in Lebanon: Convoys Across the Life Course . The Gerontologist 22 (2) :825-835.   
  • Michael R Elliott , Curry, A E, Pfeiffer, M R, Durbin, D R . 2015. Young driver crash rates by licensing age, driving experience, and license phase . Accident; Analysis and Prevention 80 :243-250.   
  • Noah J Webster , Toni C Antonucci , Fuller-Iglesias, Heather . 2015. The complex nature of family support across the life span: Implications for psychological well-being . Developmental psychology 51 (3) :277-288.   
  • Xu, Hongwei, Xie, Yu, Huang, Guoying . 2015. Cognitive Ability: Social Correlates and Consequences in Contemporary China . Chinese Sociological Review 47 (4) :287-313.   
  • Katherine Keyes , John E Schulenberg , Jager, Justin . 2015. Historical Variation in Young Adult Binge Drinking Trajectories and Its Link to Historical Variation in Social Roles and Minimum Legal Drinking Age . Developmental psychology 51 (7) :962-974.   
  • Katherine Keyes , Patrick M O'Malley , Richard Allen Miech , John E Schulenberg , Jager, Justin , Hamilton, Ava . 2015. National multi-cohort time trends in adolescent risk preference and the relation with substance use and problem behavior from 1976 to 2011 . Drug and alcohol dependence 155 :267-274.   
  • Katherine Keyes , John E Schulenberg , Maslowsky, Julie , Hamilton, Ava . 2015. The Great Sleep Recession: Changes in Sleep Duration Among US Adolescents, 1991-2012 . Pediatrics 135 (3) :460-468.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Kujawa, Autumn , MacNamara, Annmarie , Fitzgerald, KateD , Phan, K L . 2015. Enhanced Neural Reactivity to Threatening Faces in Anxious Youth: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials . Journal of abnormal child psychology 43 (8) :1493-1501.   
  • Helen G Levy , Leininger, Lindsey . 2015. Child Health and Access to Medical Care . Future of Children 25 (1) :65-90.   
  • Jennifer L Maggs , Deborah Denise Kloska , Megan E Patrick , Patrick M O'Malley , John E Schulenberg , Staff, Jeremy . 2015. Predicting Young Adult Degree Attainment by Late Adolescent Marijuana Use . Journal of Adolescent Health 57 (2) :205-211.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Lloyd D Johnston , Patrick M O'Malley , Jerald G Bachman , John E Schulenberg , Megan E Patrick . 2015. Trends in use of marijuana and attitudes toward marijuana among youth before and after decriminalization: The case of California 2007-2013 . International Journal of Drug Policy 26 (4) :336-344.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Shanahan, M J, Boardman, J , Bauldry, S . 2015. The Sequencing of a College Degree during the Transition to Adulthood: Implications for Obesity . Journal of health and social behavior 56 (2) :281-295.   
  • Daniel Almirall , Peris, Tara S, Compton, Scott N, Kendall, Philip C, Birmaher, Boris , Sherrill, Joel , March, John , Gosch, Elizabeth , Ginsburg, Golda , Rynn, Moira , McCracken, James T, Keeton, Courtney P, Sakolsky, Dara , Suveg, Cynthia , Aschenbrand, Sasha , Iyengar, Satish , Walkup, John T, Albano, Anne Marie, Piacentini, John . 2015. Trajectories of change in youth anxiety during cognitive-behavior therapy . Journal of consulting and clinical psychology 83 (2) :239-252.   
  • Schoeni, Bob, Bianchi, Suzanne M, Hotz, V J, Seltzer, Judith A, Wiemers, Emily E . 2015. Intergenerational transfers and extended family roster: a new substudy of the Panel Study of Income Dynamics . Longitudinal and Life Course Studies 6 (3) :319-330.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Maslowsky, Julie . 2015. Contribution of Adolescence to the Life Course: What Matters Most in the Long Run? . Research in Human Development 12 (3-4) :319-326.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley . 2015. Trends and timing of cigarette smoking uptake among US young adults: survival analysis using annual national cohorts from 1976 to 2005 . Addiction 110 (7) :1171-1181.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2015. Foods and beverages offered in US public secondary schools through the National School Lunch Program from 2011-2013: Early evidence of improved nutrition and reduced disparities . Preventive medicine 78 :52-58.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Yeomans-Maldonado, Gloria . 2015. The effect of perceived risk on the combined used of alcohol and marijuana: Results from daily surveys . Addictive Behaviors Reports 2 :33-36.   
  • Natalie Colabianchi , Zhu, Wenfei , Howard, Virginia J, Wadley, Virginia G, Hutto, Brent , Blair, Steven N, Vena, John E, Rhodes, David , Hooker, Steven P . 2015. Association Between Objectively Measured Physical Activity and Cognitive Function in Older Adults-The Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke Study . Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 63 (12) :2447-2454.   
  • Pamela Davis-Kean , Simonton, Sharon Z., Zilanawala, A , Nazroo, J , Sacker, A , Kelly, Y . 2015. Race/ethnic disparities in early childhood BMI, obesity and overweight in the United Kingdom and United States . International journal of obesity 39 (3) :520-529.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Guo, Ying , Sun, Shuyan , Breit-Smith, Allison , Connor, Carol McDonald . 2015. Behavioral engagement and reading achievement in elementary-school-age children: A longitudinal cross-lagged analysis . Journal of educational psychology 107 (2) :332-347.   
  • Frederick J Morrison . 2015. The growth of self-regulation in the transition to school . Advances in reading intervention: research to practice to research :25-36.   
  • Frederick J Morrison . 2015. Understanding Growth in Self-Regulation: International Contributions . Early Education and Development 26 (5-6) :893-897.   
  • A. E. Curry, M. R. Pfeiffer, Michael R Elliott , D. R. Durbin . 2015. Association between New Jersey’s Graduated Driver Licensing decal provision and crash rates of young drivers with learners’ permits . Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention 21 (6) :421-423.   
  • Allison E. Curry, Melissa R. Pfeiffer, Dennis R. Durbin, Michael R Elliott , Konny H. Kim . 2015. Young driver licensing: Examination of population-level rates using New Jersey’s state licensing database . Accident Analysis & Prevention 76 :49-56.   
  • Katherine A McGonagle , Narayan Sastry . 2015. Cohort Profile: The Panel Study of Income Dynamics’ Child Development Supplement and Transition into Adulthood Study . International journal of epidemiology 44 (2) :415-422.   
  • F. K. Winston, J. H. Mirman, A. E. Curry, M. R. Pfeiffer, Michael R Elliott , D. R. Durbin . 2015. Engagement with the TeenDrivingPlan and diversity of teens’ supervised practice driving: lessons for internet-based learner driver interventions . Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention 21 (1) :4-9.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Patrick M. O'Malley . 2015. Trends in Drug Use Among Youth in the United States . Handbook of Adolescent Drug Use Prevention:Research, Intervention Strategies, and Practice :51-65.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , John E Schulenberg , Lloyd D Johnston , Bryant, Alison L, Merline, Alicia C . 2014. The decline of substance use in young adulthood: Changes in social activities, roles, and beliefs . :322.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Nevitt, Michael R, Almeida, David M . 2014. Daily interpersonal coping strategies Implications for self-reported well-being and cortisol . Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 32 (5) :687-705.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Norbert Schwarz , Carr, Deborah S., Cornman, J C . 2014. Happy Marriage, Happy Life? Marital Quality and Subjective Well-Being in Later Life . Journal of marriage and the family 76 (5) :930-948.   
  • Natalie Colabianchi , Coulton, CJ , Hibbert, JD , McClure, SM , Levers-Landis, CE , Davis, EM . 2014. Adolescent self-defined neighborhoods and activity spaces: spatial overlap and relations to physical activity and obesity. . Health Place 27 :22-29.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Frederick G Conrad , Norbert Schwarz , Stafford, Frank P., Cornman, J . 2014. Does Time Fly When You Are Having Fun? A Day Reconstruction Method Analysis . Journal of happiness studies 15 (3) :639-655.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Spillman, B C . 2014. Disability and care needs among older americans . The Milbank quarterly 92 (3) :509-541.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Spillman, Brenda C . 2014. The Residential Continuum From Home to Nursing Home: Size, Characteristics and Unmet Needs of Older Adults . The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 69 :S42-S50.   
  • Steven G Heeringa , Kessler,Ronald C, Gadermann, A M, Stein, M B, Ursano, R J, Colpe, L J, Fullerton, C S, Gilman, S E, Gruber, M J, Nock, M K, Rosellini, A J, Sampson, N A, Schoenbaum, M , Zaslavsky, A M . 2014. Classifying U.S. Army Military Occupational Specialties using the Occupational Information Network . Military medicine 179 (7) :752-761.   
  • Daniel Almirall , Griffin, B A, Ramchand, R , Slaughter, M E, Burgette, L F, McCaffery, D F . 2014. Estimating the causal effects of cumulative treatment episodes for adolescents using marginal structural models and inverse probability of treatment weighting . Drug and alcohol dependence 136 (March) :69-78.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Griffin, Jamie . 2014. Nonresponse bias in a longitudinal measurement design examining substance use across the transition out of high school . Drug and alcohol dependence 143 :232-238.   
  • Daniel Almirall , Murphy, Susan A., Kasari, Connie , Kaiser, Ann , Goods, Kelly , Nietfeld, Jennifer , Mathy, Pamela , Landa, Rebecca . 2014. Communication Interventions for Minimally Verbal Children With Autism: A Sequential Multiple Assignment Randomized Trial . Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 53 (6) :635-646.   
  • Kenneth M Langa , Kuzma, E , Llewellyn, D J, Wallace, R B, Lang, I A . 2014. History of alcohol use disorders and risk of severe cognitive impairment: a 19-year prospective cohort study . The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry : Official Journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry 22 (10) :1047-1054.   
  • Michael R Elliott , Mirman, J H, Curry, A E, Winston, F K, Wang, W , Schultheis, M T, Fisher Thiel, M C, Durbin, D R . 2014. Effect of theTteenDrivingPlan on the driving performance of teenagers before licensure: a randomized clinical trial . JAMA pediatrics 168 (8) :764-771.   
  • Colter Mitchell . 2014. Born Together-Reared Apart: The Landmark Minnesota Twin Study . Contemporary Sociology: A Journal of Reviews 43 (2) :262-264.   
  • Paula Fomby , Mollborn, Stefanie , Lawrence, Elizabeth , James-Hawkins, Laurie . 2014. When do socioeconomic resources matter most in early childhood? . Advances in Life Course Research 20 :56-69.   
  • Daniel Almirall , Schuler, Megan S, Griffin, Beth Ann, Ramchand, Rajeev , McCaffrey, Daniel F . 2014. Effectiveness of treatment for adolescent substance use: is biological drug testing sufficient? . Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs 75 (2) :358-370.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Jerald G Bachman , Matthew VanEseltine , Staff, Jeremy, Siennick, Sonja E, Osgood, D Wayne . 2014. Partnership Transitions and Antisocial Behavior in Young Adulthood: A Within-person, Multi-Cohort Analysis . Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 51 (6) :735-758.   
  • Lloyd D Johnston , Slater, S , Chriqui, J , Chaloupka, F J . 2014. Joint use policies: are they related to adolescent behavior? . Preventive medicine 69 (Suppl 1) :S37-S43.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Swartz, J R . 2014. The role of corticolimbic circuitry in the development of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents . Current topics in behavioral neurosciences 16 :133-148.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Swartz, J R, Phan, K L, Angstadt, M , Klumpp, H , Fitzgerald, K D . 2014. Altered activation of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in the context of emotional face distractors in children and adolescents with anxiety disorders . Depression and anxiety 31 (10) :870-879.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Natalie Colabianchi , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston , Hood, N E . 2014. Profits, commercial food supplier involvement, and school vending machine snack food availability: implications for implementing the new competitive foods rule . The Journal of school health 84 (7) :451-458.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2014. Accessibility over availability: associations between the school food environment and student fruit and green vegetable consumption . Childhood obesity (Print) 10 (3) :241-250.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2014. Alcohol and marijuana use patterns associated with unsafe driving among U.S. high school seniors: high use frequency, concurrent use, and simultaneous use . Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs 75 (3) :378-389.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2014. Energy Drinks, Soft Drinks, and Substance Use Among United States Secondary School Students . Journal of Addiction Medicine 8 (1) :6-13.   
  • Lindsay H Ryan , Windsor, Tim D, Gerstorf, Denis , Pearson, Elissa , Anstey, Kaarin J . 2014. Positive and negative social exchanges and cognitive aging in young-old adults: Differential associations across family, friend, and spouse domains . Psychology and aging 29 (1) :28-43.   
  • Kristine J Ajrouch , Toni C Antonucci , Abdulrahim, Sawsan . 2013. Stress, social relations, and psychological health over the life course: A focus on Lebanon . GeroPsych: The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry 26 (1) :15-27.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , Freedman-Doan, Peter C. . 2013. Adolescent Work Intensity, School Performance, and Substance Use: Links Vary by Race/Ethnicity and Socioeconomic Status. . Developmental psychology 49 (11) :2125-2134.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Bernard, J A, Peltier, S J, Wiggins, J L, Jaeggi, S M, Buschkuehl, M , Fling, B W, Kwak, Y , Jonides, J , Seidler, R D . 2013. Disrupted cortico-cerebellar connectivity in older adults . NeuroImage 83 :103-119.   
  • Kira S Birditt . 2013. Age Differences in Emotional Reactions to Daily Negative Social Encounters . The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 69 (4) :557-566.   
  • Philippa J Clarke , Patrick M O'Malley , John E Schulenberg , Natalie Colabianchi , Lloyd D Johnston , Lee, H . 2013. College expectations in high school mitigate weight gain over early adulthood: Findings from a national study of American youth. LID - 10.1002/oby.20176 [doi] . Obesity 21 (7) :1321-1327.   
  • Kenneth M Langa , Dutta, A , Henley, W , Robine, JM , Wallace, R B, Melzer, D . 2013. Longer Lived Parents: Protective Associations With Cancer Incidence and Overall Mortality . The journals of gerontology Series A: Biological sciences and medical sciences 68 (11) :1409-1418.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Fingerman, K L, Sechrist, J . 2013. Changing Views on Intergenerational Ties . Gerontology 59 (1) :64-70.   
  • Paula Fomby , Bosick, Stacey J . 2013. Family Instability and the Transition to Adulthood . Journal of Marriage and the Family 75 (5) :1266-1287.   
  • Vicki A Freedman , Schoeni, Bob, Martin, Leah, Spillman, B C, Andreski, P M, Cornman, J C, Crimmins, E M, Kramarow, E , Lubitz, J , Martin, Linda G, Merkin, S S, Seeman, T E, Waidmann, T A . 2013. Trends in late-life activity limitations in the United States: an update from five national surveys . Demography 50 (2) :661-671.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Hartnett, Caroline Sten, Furstenberg, Frank F, Fingerman, Karen L . 2013. Parental Support During Young Adulthood: Why Does Assistance Decline With Age? . Journal of Family Issues 34 (7) :975-1007.   
  • Daniela Elisabeth Hochfellner , Burkert, C . 2013. Employment in retirement: continuation of a working career or essential additional income? . Zeitschrift fur Gerontologie und Geriatrie 46 (3) :242-250.   
  • Kenneth M Langa , Hurd, Michael D, Martorell, Paco , Delavande, Adeline , Mullen, Kathleen J . 2013. Monetary Costs of Dementia in the United States . New England Journal of Medicine 368 (14) :1326-1334.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Jackson, Kristina M . 2013. Alcohol Use During the Transition From Middle School to High School: National Panel Data on Prevalence and Moderators. . Developmental Psychology 49 (11) :2147-2158.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Patrick M O'Malley , Jerald G Bachman , Jager, J . 2013. Historical variation in drug use trajectories across the transition to adulthood: the trend toward lower intercepts and steeper, ascending slopes . Development and psychopathology 25 (2) :527-543.   
  • Katherine Keyes , Richard Allen Miech . 2013. Age, period, and cohort effects in heavy episodic drinking in the US from 1985 to 2009 . Drug and alcohol dependence 132 (1-2) :140-148.   
  • Philippa J Clarke , Latham, Kenzie . 2013. The Role of Neighborhood Safety in Recovery From Mobility Limitations: Findings From a National Sample of Older Americans (1996-2008) . Research on aging 35 (4) :481-502.   
  • Sean Esteban McCabe , Brady Thomas West , Boyd, Carol J . 2013. Leftover Prescription Opioids and Nonmedical Use Among High School Seniors: A Multi-Cohort National Study . The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine 52 (4) :480-485.   
  • Michael R Elliott , Harlow, Sioban D., Paramsothy, P , Lisabeth, L D, Crawford, S L, Randolph, J F . 2013. Classifying menopause stage by menstrual calendars and annual interviews: need for improved questionnaires . Menopause (New York, N.Y.) 20 (7) :727-735.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Park, Sojung . 2013. Visible Women: Tales of Age, Gender and In/Visibility, by Christine Bell . Journal of women & aging 25 (2) :199-200.   
  • Natalie Colabianchi , Pate, R R, O'Neill, J R, Liese, A D, Janz, K F, Granberg, E M, Harsha, D W, Condrasky, M M, O'Neil, P M, Lau, E Y, Taverno Ross, S E . 2013. Factors associated with development of excessive fatness in children and adolescents: a review of prospective studies . Obesity Reviews 14 (8) :645-658.   
  • Megan E Patrick . 2013. Daily associations of alcohol use with sexual behaviour and condom use during Spring Break . Drug and Alcohol Review 32 (2) :215-217.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Jennifer L Maggs , Lewis, Melissa A, Lee, Christine M . 2013. Semester and event-specific motives for alcohol use during Spring Break: Associated protective strategies and negative consequences . Addictive Behaviors 38 (4) :1980-1987.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Sean Esteban McCabe , Singer, Eleanor, Boyd, Carol J, Cranford, James A . 2013. Incentives for college student participation in web-based substance use surveys . Addictive Behaviors 38 (3) :1710-1714.   
  • Jacqui Smith , Lindsay H Ryan , Röcke, Christina . 2013. The Day-to-Day Effects of Conscientiousness on Well-Being . Research in Human Development 10 (1) :9-25.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Lord, Catherine, Swartz, J R, Wiggins, J L, Carrasco, M . 2013. Amygdala habituation and prefrontal functional connectivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders . Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 52 (1) :84-93.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2013. Middle and high school drug testing and student illicit drug use: a national study 1998-2011 . The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine 52 (6) :707-715.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2013. Simultaneous alcohol and marijuana use among US high school seniors from 1976 to 2011: Trends, reasons, and situations . Drug and alcohol dependence 133 (1) :71-79.   
  • Oyserman,Daphna R, Norbert Schwarz , Uskul, Ayse K, Lee, Spike W. S., Xu, Alison Jing . 2013. How Successful You Have Been in Life Depends on the Response Scale Used: The Role of Cultural Mindsets in Pragmatic Inferences Drawn from Question Format . Social Cognition 31 (2) :222-236.   
  • Noah J Webster , Kristine J Ajrouch , Toni C Antonucci . 2013. Living Healthier, Living Longer: The Benefits of Residing in Community . Generations 37 (4) :28-32.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Wiggins, J L . 2013. A translational neuroscience framework for the development of socioemotional functioning in health and psychopathology . Development and psychopathology 25 (4 Pt 2) :1293-1309.   
  • Barber, Jennifer S, Yasamin Yoko Kusunoki . 2013. Young Women’s Relationships, Contraception, and Unintended Pregnancy in the United States . Fertility Rates and Population Decline: No Time for Children?
  • N. E. Hood, Natalie Colabianchi , Y. M. Terry-McElrath, Patrick M. O'Malley, Lloyd D Johnston . 2013. School wellness policies and foods and beverages available in schools . American Journal of Preventive Medicine 45 (2) :143-149.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Wong, Kristin M, Trinh, Sarah . 2012. The Role of the Family across the Lifespan . The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Couples and Family Relationships :49-65.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Bauldry, Shawn , Shanahan, Michael J, Boardman, Jason D, Macmillan, Ross . 2012. A life course model of self-rated health through adolescence and young adulthood . Social science & medicine 75 (7) :1311-1320.   
  • Miles S Kimball , Benjamin, Daniel J., Heffetz, Ori , Rees-Jones, Alex . 2012. What Do You Think Would Make You Happier? What Do You Think You Would Choose? . American Economic Review 102 (5) :2083-2110.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Toni C Antonucci . 2012. Enacted support during stressful life events in middle and older adulthood: An examination of the interpersonal context. . Psychology and aging 27 (3) :728-741.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Hope, Susannah , Brown, Edna E, Orbuch, Terri L . 2012. Developmental Trajectories of Marital Happiness Over 16 Years . Research in Human Development 9 (2) :126-144.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Wardjiman, E . 2012. Intergenerational relationships and aging . The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of adulthood and aging
  • John E Schulenberg , Patrick M O'Malley , Jerald G Bachman , Dever, B V, Dworkin, J B . 2012. Predicting risk-taking with and without substance use: the effects of parental monitoring, school bonding, and sports participation. . Prevention Science 113 (6) :605-615.   
  • Kira S Birditt , Fingerman, K L, Cheng, Y P, Tighe, L , Zarit, S . 2012. Parent-child relationships in young adulthood . Early adulthood in a family context
  • Kira S Birditt , Fingerman, Karen L, Cheng, Yen-Pi , Wesselmann, Eric D, Zarit, Steven , Furstenberg, Frank . 2012. Helicopter Parents and Landing Pad Kids: Intense Parental Support of Grown Children . Journal of Marriage and the Family 74 (4) :880-896.   
  • Xu, Hongwei, Luke, Nancy , Mberu, Blessing U, Goldberg, Rachel E . 2012. Migration Experience and Premarital Sexual Initiation in Urban Kenya: An Event History Analysis . Studies in family planning 43 (2) :115-126.   
  • Megan E Patrick , John E Schulenberg , Jager, Justin . 2012. Social role patterning in early adulthood in the USA: adolescent predictors and concurrent wellbeing across four distince configurations . Longitudinal and Life Course studies 3 (2) :190-210.   
  • Katherine A McGonagle , Schoeni, Bob, Vicki A Freedman . 2012. The Panel Study of Income Dynamics: overview, recent innovations, and potential for life course research . Longitudinal and Life course Studies 3 :268.   
  • Paula Fomby , Mollborn, Stefanie , Dennis, Jeff A . 2012. Extended household transitions, race/ethnicity, and early childhood cognitive outcomes . Social science research 41 (5) :1152-1165.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Neighbors, C , Lee, Christine M . 2012. A Hierarchy of 21st Birthday Drinking Norms . Journal of college student development 53 (4) :581-585.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Schoeni, Bob, John E Schulenberg , Wightman, P . 2012. Socioeconomic status and substance use among young adults: a comparison across constructs and drugs . Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs 73 (5) :772-782.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Schoon, Ingrid . 2012. The transition to adulthood across time and space: overview of Special section . Longitudinal and Life Course studies 3 (2) :164-172.   
  • Natalie Colabianchi , Taverno Ross, S E, Dowda, M , Saunders, R , Pate, R R . 2012. After-school setting, physical activity, and sedentary behavior in 5th grade boys and girls . Health & place 18 (5) :951-955.   
  • Yvonne M Terry-McElrath , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston . 2012. Factors Affecting Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Availability in Competitive Venues of US Secondary Schools . Journal of School Health 82 (1) :44-55.   
  • Caldwell,Cleopatra Howard, Toni C Antonucci , Williams, Terrinieka T, Mance, GiShawn . 2012. The Role of Prenatal Stress and Maternal Emotional Support on the Postpartum Depressive Symptoms of African American Adolescent Fathers . Journal of Black Psychology 38 (4) :455-470.   
  • Jennifer L Maggs , Lloyd D Johnston , Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , Wray-Lake, L . 2012. Associations Between Community Attachments and Adolescent Substance Use in Nationally Representative Samples . The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine 32 (5) :325-331.   
  • Elizabeth T. Gershoff, Jennifer E. Lansford, Holly R. Sexton, Pamela Davis-Kean , Arnold J. Sameroff . 2012. Longitudinal Links Between Spanking and Children’s Externalizing Behaviors in a National Sample of White, Black, Hispanic, and Asian American Families . Child development 83 (3) :838-843.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kira S Birditt , Sherman, Carey, Trinh, Sarah . 2011. Stability and change in the intergenerational family: a convoy approach . Ageing & Society 31 (7) :1084-1106.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , Freedman-Doan, Peter C., Trzesniewski, Kali H, Donnellan, M B . 2011. Adolescent Self-esteem: Differences by Race/Ethnicity, Gender, and Age . Self and Identity 10 (4) :445-473.   
  • David R Weir , Birnie, K , Cooper, R , Martin, R M, Kuh, D , Sayer, A A, Alvarado, B E, Bayer, A , Christensen, K , Cho, S , Cooper, C , Corley, J , Craig, L , Deary, I J, Demakakos, P , Ebrahim, S , Gallacher, J , Gow, A J, Gunnell, D , Haas, S , Hemmingsson, T , Inskip, H , Jang, S , Noronha, K , Osler, M , Palloni, A , Rasmussen, F , Santos-Eggimann, B , Spagnoli, J , Starr, J , Steptoe, A , Syddall, H , Tynelius, P , Whalley, L J, Zunzunegui, M V, Ben-Shlomo, Y , Hardy, R . 2011. Childhood socioeconomic position and objectively measured physical capability levels in adulthood: A systematic review and meta-analysis . PLoS ONE 6 :e15564.   
  • Rachel Carter , Caldwell,Cleopatra Howard, Toni C Antonucci , Matusko, N . 2011. Ethnicity, Perceived Pubertal Timing, Externalizing Behaviors, and Depressive Symptoms Among Black Adolescent Girls . Journal of Youth and Adolescence 40 (10) :1394-1406.   
  • Paula Fomby , Chase-Lansdale, P Lindsay, Cherlin, Andrew J, Guttmannova, Katarina , Ribar, David C, Coley, Rebekah Levine . 2011. Long-term implications of welfare reform for the development of adolescents and young adults . Children and Youth Services Review 33 (5) :678-688.   
  • Mary Beth Ofstedal , Wray, Linda, Chiu, C J . 2011. Diabetes-related change in physical disability from midlife to older adulthood: evidence from 1996-2003 Survey of Health and Living Status of the Elderly in Taiwan . Diabetes research and clinical practice 91 (3) :413-423.   
  • Philippa J Clarke , James S House , Lantz, Paula M., Marshall, Victor . 2011. The Social Structuring of Mental Health over the Adult Life Course: Advancing Theory in the Sociology of Aging . Social Forces 89 (4) :1287-1313.   
  • Jacqui Smith , Toni C Antonucci , Fingerman, Karen L, Berg, C A . 2011. Handbook of lifespan development .
  • Roderick J Little , James M Lepkowski , Ismail, Amid I, Ondersma, Steven , Willem Jedele, Jenefer M . 2011. Evaluation of a brief tailored motivational intervention to prevent early childhood caries . Community dentistry and oral epidemiology 39 (5) :433-448.   
  • Daniel P Keating . 2011. Society and early child developmental health disparities in the nature-and-nurture paradigm . Nature and nurture in early child development
  • Daniel P Keating . 2011. Cognitive Development . Encyclopedia of Adolescence
  • Natalie Colabianchi , Maslow, Andréa L . 2011. Youth Physical Activity Resource Use and Activity Measured by Accelerometry . American Journal of Health Behavior 35 (2) :219-227.   
  • Daniel P Keating , Maslowsky, J , Buvinger, E , Steinberg, L , Cauffman, E . 2011. Cost-Benefit Analysis Mediation of the Relationship Between Sensation Seeking and Risk Behavior. . Pers Individ Dif. 51 (7) :802-806.   
  • Sean Esteban McCabe , Brady Thomas West , Cranford, James A, Ross-Durow, Paula , Young, Amy , Teter, Christian J, Boyd, Carol J . 2011. Medical Misuse of Controlled Medications Among Adolescents . Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 165 (8) :729-735.   
  • Megan E Patrick , Jennifer L Maggs , Cooper, M Lynne, Lee, Christine M . 2011. Measurement of Motivations For and Against Sexual Behavior . Assessment 18 (4) :502-516.   
  • Megan E Patrick , John E Schulenberg . 2011. How trajectories of reasons for alcohol use relate to trajectories of binge drinking: National panel data spanning late adolescence to early adulthood . Developmental psychology 47 (2) :311-317.   
  • Megan E Patrick , John E Schulenberg , Patrick M O'Malley , Jennifer L Maggs , Deborah Denise Kloska , Lloyd D Johnston , Jerald G Bachman . 2011. Age-related changes in reasons for using alcohol and marijuana from ages 18 to 30 in a national sample . Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 25 (2) :330-339.   
  • Schoeni, Bob, Tom Buchmueller, Vicki A Freedman . 2011. Socioeconomic Status and Health Over the Life Course and Across Generations: Introduction to a Special Issue and Overview of a Unique Data Resource . The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy 13
  • Amy Dorothy Corning . 2011. Generational Memory and the Critical Period: Evidence for National and World Events . Public opinion quarterly 76 (1) :1-31.   
  • Liberzon, Israel, Christopher Stephen Monk , Lord, Catherine, Weng, S J, Carrasco, M , Swartz, J R, Wiggins, J L, Kurapati, N , Risi, S . 2011. Neural activation to emotional faces in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders . Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 52 (3) :296-305.   
  • Noah J Webster , Wiest, Maja , Schüz, Benjamin , Wurm, Susanne . 2011. Subjective well-being and mortality revisited: Differential effects of cognitive and emotional facets of well-being on mortality . Health Psychology 30 (6) :728-735.   
  • Christopher Stephen Monk , Lord, Catherine, Wiggins, J L, Peltier, S J, Ashinoff, S , Weng, S J, Carrasco, M , Welsh, R C . 2011. Using a self-organizing map algorithm to detect age-related changes in functional connectivity during rest in autism spectrum disorders . Brain research 1380 :187-197.   
  • Michael R Elliott , Zonfrillo, Mark R, Flannagan, Carol A, Durbin, Dennis R . 2011. Association Between Weight and Risk of Crash-Related Injuries for Children in Child Restraints . Pediatrics 128 (6) :1148-1152.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kira S Birditt , Kristine J Ajrouch . 2011. Convoys of social relations: Past, present, and future . Handbook of lifespan development
  • Julie Maslowsky, Daniel P Keating , Christopher Stephen Monk , John E Schulenberg . 2011. Planned versus unplanned risks: Neurocognitive predictors of subtypes of adolescents’ risk behavior . International Journal of Behavioral Development 35 (2) :152-160.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kira S Birditt , Fiori, Katherine L, Jackey, L . 2010. Convoys of social relations: Integrating life-span and life-course perspectives . Handbook of life-span development
  • Toni C Antonucci , Jackson, James Sidney . 2010. Life-course perspectives on late life health inequalities . Annual review of gerontology & geriatrics 29
  • Roderick J Little , Clark, Noreen M, Shah, Smita , Dodge, Julia A, Thomas, Lara J, Andridge, Rebecca R . 2010. An evaluation of asthma interventions for preteen students.(Report) . Journal of School Health 80 (2) :80-87.   
  • Philippa J Clarke , Patrick M O'Malley , John E Schulenberg , Lloyd D Johnston . 2010. Midlife Health and Socioeconomic Consequences of Persistent Overweight Across Early Adulthood: Findings From a National Survey of American Adults (1986-2008) . American Journal of Epidemiology 172 (5) :540-548.   
  • Amy Dorothy Corning . 2010. Emigration, Generation, and Collective Memories: The Presence of the Past for Emigrants from the Former Soviet Union . Social psychology quarterly 73 (3) :223-244.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Jackson, James Sidney, Fuller-Iglesias, Heather . 2010. Theoretical perspectives on life span and life course development . Life-course perspectives on late life health inequalities 29
  • Frederick J Morrison , Son, Seung-Hee . 2010. The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children . Developmental psychology 46 (5) :1103-1118.   
  • John E Schulenberg , Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , Jennifer L Maggs , Lloyd D Johnston , Staff, Jeremy, Maslowsky, J . 2010. Substance use changes and social role transitions: Proximal developmental effects on ongoing trajectories from late adolescence through early adulthood . Development and psychopathology 22 (4) :917-932.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , Goodkind, Sara , Shook, Jeffrey J . 2009. Are girls really becoming more delinquent? Testing the gender convergence hypothesis by race and ethnicity, 1976 - 2005 . Children and Youth Services Review 31 (8) :885-895.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Kim, J . 2009. The Black-White difference in age trajectories of functional health over the life course . Social science & medicine 36 (2) :126-132.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Sutin, Angelina R, Costa, Paul T, Eaton, William W . 2009. Personality and career success: Concurrent and longitudinal relations . European Journal of Personality 23 (2) :71-84.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , Lloyd D Johnston , John E Schulenberg , Vaughn, Michael G . 2009. Race/ethnicity, socioeconomic factors, and smoking among early adolescent girls in the United States . Women and Smoking: An Interdisciplinary Examination of Socioeconomic Influences 104 (Supplement 1) :S42-S49.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Matthews, J S, Ponitz, Claire Cameron . 2009. Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement . Journal of educational psychology 101 (3) :689-704.   
  • Matthew VanEseltine , Silver, Eric , Silver, Stacy J . 2009. Tattoo Acquisition: A Prospective Longitudinal Study of Adolescents . Deviant Behavior 30 (6) :511-538.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , John E Schulenberg , Messersmith, Emily E . 2008. The optimism of American youth . Young people facing the future : an international survey :163.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Burrage, Marie S, Ponitz, Claire Cameron, McCready, Elizabeth A, Sims, Brian C, Jewkes, Abigail M . 2008. Age- and Schooling-Related Effects on Executive Functions in Young Children: A Natural Experiment . Child Neuropsychology 14 (6) :510-524.   
  • Frederick J Morrison , Cameron Ponitz, Claire E, McClelland, Megan M, Jewkes, Abigail M, Connor, Carol McDonald, Farris, Carrie L . 2008. Touch your toes! Developing a direct measure of behavioral regulation in early childhood . Early Childhood Research Quarterly 23 (2) :141-158.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Lin, F R, Ceh, K , Bervinchak, D , Riley, A , Niparko, J K . 2007. Development of a communicative performance scale for pediatric cochlear implantation . Ear and hearing 28 (5) :703-712.   
  • Jacqui Smith . 2007. The Allure of Wisdom . Human development 50 (6) :367-370.   
  • Jerald G Bachman , Patrick M O'Malley , John E Schulenberg , Lloyd D Johnston , Yamaguchi, Ryoko . 2007. Religiosity and Adolescent Substance Use: The Role of Individual and Contextual Influences . Social problems 54 (2) :308-327.   
  • Kristine J Ajrouch , Toni C Antonucci , Blandon, Alysia Y . 2005. Social Networks Among Men and Women: The Effects of Age and Socioeconomic Status . Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 60 (6) :S311.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Eaton, W W, Brennan, K . 2005. Mental health disparities across education and sex: a prospective analysis examining how they persist over the life course . The journals of gerontology Series B: Psychological sciences and social sciences 60 (2) :93-98.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kristine J Ajrouch . 2003. The effect of social relations with children on the education-health link in men and women aged 40 and over . Social science & medicine 56 (5) :949.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Eaton, William , Liang, Kung-Yee . 2003. Occupational Stratification Over the Life Course: A Comparison of Occupational Trajectories Across Race and Gender During the 1980s and 1990s . Work and Occupations 30 (4) :440-473.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Breitner, J C. S., Zandi, P P, Khachaturian, A S, Anthony, J C, Mayer, L . 2002. Incidence of AD may decline in the early 90s for men, later for women: The Cache County study . Neurology 58 (2) :209-218.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Hauser, Robert M . 2001. Socioeconomic Status and Health at Midlife: A Comparison of Educational Attainment with Occupation-Based Indicators . Annals of Epidemiology 11 (2) :75-84.   
  • Richard Allen Miech , Walrath, Christine , dosReis, Susan , Liao, Qinghong , Holden, E W, De Carolis, Gary , Santiago, Rolando , Leaf, Philip . 2001. Referral Source Differences in Functional Impairment Levels for Children Served in the Comprehensive Community Mental Health Services for Children and Their Families Program . Journal of Child and Family Studies 10 (3) :385-397.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Vandewater, Elizabeth A., Lansford, Jennifer E . 2000. Adulthood and aging: Social processes and development . Encyclopedia of psychology, Vol. 1. :79-85.   
  • William G Axinn , Barber, Jennifer S, Arland Thornton . 1999. Values and beliefs as determinants of outcomes in children’s lives . Transitions to adulthood in a changing economy : no work, no family, no future?
  • Toni C Antonucci . 1998. A Career With Meaning . PsycCRITIQUES 43 (9) :610-611.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Neugarten, Dail A . 1998. Child, Aging, and Developmental Psychology - The Meaning of Age: Selected Papers of Bernice L Neugarten . Contemporary psychology 43 (9) :610.   
  • William G Axinn , Pearce, Lisa D . 1998. The impact of family religious life on the quality of mother-child relations . American Sociological Review 63 (6): 810-828
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kahn, Robert L. . 1990. Convoys over the life course: Attachment, roles, and social support . Psychogerontologie : een inleidend leerboek over ouder worden, persoonlijkheid, zingeving, levensloop en tijd, sociale context, gezondheid en interventie 1. druk, 2. opl :81.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Jackson, James Sidney . 1989. Successful aging and life course reciprocity . Human ageing and later life : multidisciplinary perspectives :83.   
  • Robert L. Kahn, Toni C Antonucci . 1981. Convoys of social support : a life-course approach . Aging: social change :338-405.   
  • Toni C Antonucci , Kahn, Robert L. . 1980. Convoys over the life course: Attachment, roles and social support . Life-span development and behavior 3
  • Toni C Antonucci , J. Beals . 1980. Social support networks during normative transitions in women’s lives .
  • Toni C Antonucci . 1978. Attachments across the life-span: A generational perspective . Work, family roles and support systems 3 :37.   
  • Toni C Antonucci . 1976. Attachment: A life-span concept . Human Development 19 (3) :135.   
  • Toni C Antonucci . 1973. The relationship between attachment and contingency learning in infancy .
  • Jasmine A. Manalel, Toni C Antonucci . . Development of social convoys: Trajectories of convoy structure and composition from childhood through adulthood . Developmental Psychology

Featured Faculty

Photo of Katherine A McGonagle

Katherine A McGonagle

Photo of Daniel P Keating

Daniel P Keating

Featured Topics

Featured series.

A series of random questions answered by Harvard experts.

Explore the Gazette

Read the latest.

Christina Warinner speaking.

Got milk? Does it give you problems?

Full body portrait of Molly F. Przeworski.

Cancer risk, wine preference, and your genes

Doctor holding endoscope.

Excited about new diet drug? This procedure seems better choice.

Good genes are nice, but joy is better.

Harvard Staff Writer

Harvard study, almost 80 years old, has proved that embracing community helps us live longer, and be happier

Part of the tackling issues of aging series.

A series on how Harvard researchers are tackling the problematic issues of aging.

W hen scientists began tracking the health of 268 Harvard sophomores in 1938 during the Great Depression, they hoped the longitudinal study would reveal clues to leading healthy and happy lives.

They got more than they wanted.

After following the surviving Crimson men for nearly 80 years as part of the Harvard Study of Adult Development , one of the world’s longest studies of adult life, researchers have collected a cornucopia of data on their physical and mental health.

Of the original Harvard cohort recruited as part of the Grant Study, only 19 are still alive, all in their mid-90s. Among the original recruits were eventual President John F. Kennedy and longtime Washington Post editor Ben Bradlee. (Women weren’t in the original study because the College was still all male.)

In addition, scientists eventually expanded their research to include the men’s offspring, who now number 1,300 and are in their 50s and 60s, to find out how early-life experiences affect health and aging over time. Some participants went on to become successful businessmen, doctors, lawyers, and others ended up as schizophrenics or alcoholics, but not on inevitable tracks.

“Loneliness kills. It’s as powerful as smoking or alcoholism.” Robert Waldinger, psychiatrist, Massachusetts General Hospital

During the intervening decades, the control groups have expanded. In the 1970s, 456 Boston inner-city residents were enlisted as part of the Glueck Study, and 40 of them are still alive. More than a decade ago, researchers began including wives in the Grant and Glueck studies.

Over the years, researchers have studied the participants’ health trajectories and their broader lives, including their triumphs and failures in careers and marriage, and the finding have produced startling lessons, and not only for the researchers.

“The surprising finding is that our relationships and how happy we are in our relationships has a powerful influence on our health,” said Robert Waldinger , director of the study, a psychiatrist at Massachusetts General Hospital and a professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School . “Taking care of your body is important, but tending to your relationships is a form of self-care too. That, I think, is the revelation.”

Close relationships, more than money or fame, are what keep people happy throughout their lives, the study revealed. Those ties protect people from life’s discontents, help to delay mental and physical decline, and are better predictors of long and happy lives than social class, IQ, or even genes. That finding proved true across the board among both the Harvard men and the inner-city participants.

“The people who were the most satisfied in their relationships at age 50 were the healthiest at age 80,” said Robert Waldinger with his wife Jennifer Stone.

Rose Lincoln/Harvard Staff Photographer

The long-term research has received funding from private foundations, but has been financed largely by grants from the National Institutes of Health, first through the National Institute of Mental Health, and more recently through the National Institute on Aging.

Researchers who have pored through data, including vast medical records and hundreds of in-person interviews and questionnaires, found a strong correlation between men’s flourishing lives and their relationships with family, friends, and community. Several studies found that people’s level of satisfaction with their relationships at age 50 was a better predictor of physical health than their cholesterol levels were.

“When we gathered together everything we knew about them about at age 50, it wasn’t their middle-age cholesterol levels that predicted how they were going to grow old,” said Waldinger in a popular TED Talk . “It was how satisfied they were in their relationships. The people who were the most satisfied in their relationships at age 50 were the healthiest at age 80.”

He recorded his TED talk, titled “What Makes a Good Life? Lessons from the Longest Study on Happiness,” in 2015, and it has been viewed 13,000,000 times.

The researchers also found that marital satisfaction has a protective effect on people’s mental health. Part of a study found that people who had happy marriages in their 80s reported that their moods didn’t suffer even on the days when they had more physical pain. Those who had unhappy marriages felt both more emotional and physical pain.

Those who kept warm relationships got to live longer and happier, said Waldinger, and the loners often died earlier. “Loneliness kills,” he said. “It’s as powerful as smoking or alcoholism.”

According to the study, those who lived longer and enjoyed sound health avoided smoking and alcohol in excess. Researchers also found that those with strong social support experienced less mental deterioration as they aged.

In part of a recent study , researchers found that women who felt securely attached to their partners were less depressed and more happy in their relationships two-and-a-half years later, and also had better memory functions than those with frequent marital conflicts.

“When the study began, nobody cared about empathy or attachment. But the key to healthy aging is relationships, relationships, relationships.” George Vaillant, psychiatrist

“Good relationships don’t just protect our bodies; they protect our brains,” said Waldinger in his TED talk. “And those good relationships, they don’t have to be smooth all the time. Some of our octogenarian couples could bicker with each other day in and day out, but as long as they felt that they could really count on the other when the going got tough, those arguments didn’t take a toll on their memories.”

Since aging starts at birth, people should start taking care of themselves at every stage of life, the researchers say.

“Aging is a continuous process,” Waldinger said. “You can see how people can start to differ in their health trajectory in their 30s, so that by taking good care of yourself early in life you can set yourself on a better course for aging. The best advice I can give is ‘Take care of your body as though you were going to need it for 100 years,’ because you might.”

The study, like its remaining original subjects, has had a long life, spanning four directors, whose tenures reflected their medical interests and views of the time.

Under the first director, Clark Heath, who stayed from 1938 until 1954, the study mirrored the era’s dominant view of genetics and biological determinism. Early researchers believed that physical constitution, intellectual ability, and personality traits determined adult development. They made detailed anthropometric measurements of skulls, brow bridges, and moles, wrote in-depth notes on the functioning of major organs, examined brain activity through electroencephalograms, and even analyzed the men’s handwriting.

Now, researchers draw men’s blood for DNA testing and put them into MRI scanners to examine organs and tissues in their bodies, procedures that would have sounded like science fiction back in 1938. In that sense, the study itself represents a history of the changes that life brings.

6 factors predicting healthy aging According to George Vaillant’s book “Aging Well,” from observations of Harvard men in long-term aging study

Physically active.

Absence of alcohol abuse and smoking

Having mature mechanisms to cope with life’s ups and downs

Healthy weight

Stable marriage.

Psychiatrist George Vaillant, who joined the team as a researcher in 1966, led the study from 1972 until 2004. Trained as a psychoanalyst, Vaillant emphasized the role of relationships, and came to recognize the crucial role they played in people living long and pleasant lives.

In a book called “Aging Well,” Vaillant wrote that six factors predicted healthy aging for the Harvard men: physical activity, absence of alcohol abuse and smoking, having mature mechanisms to cope with life’s ups and downs, and enjoying both a healthy weight and a stable marriage. For the inner-city men, education was an additional factor. “The more education the inner city men obtained,” wrote Vaillant, “the more likely they were to stop smoking, eat sensibly, and use alcohol in moderation.”

Vaillant’s research highlighted the role of these protective factors in healthy aging. The more factors the subjects had in place, the better the odds they had for longer, happier lives.

“When the study began, nobody cared about empathy or attachment,” said Vaillant. “But the key to healthy aging is relationships, relationships, relationships.”

“We want to find out how it is that a difficult childhood reaches across decades to break down the body in middle age and later.” Robert Waldinger

The study showed that the role of genetics and long-lived ancestors proved less important to longevity than the level of satisfaction with relationships in midlife, now recognized as a good predictor of healthy aging. The research also debunked the idea that people’s personalities “set like plaster” by age 30 and cannot be changed.

“Those who were clearly train wrecks when they were in their 20s or 25s turned out to be wonderful octogenarians,” he said. “On the other hand, alcoholism and major depression could take people who started life as stars and leave them at the end of their lives as train wrecks.”

The study’s fourth director, Waldinger has expanded research to the wives and children of the original men. That is the second-generation study, and Waldinger hopes to expand it into the third and fourth generations. “It will probably never be replicated,” he said of the lengthy research, adding that there is yet more to learn.

“We’re trying to see how people manage stress, whether their bodies are in a sort of chronic ‘fight or flight’ mode,” Waldinger said. “We want to find out how it is that a difficult childhood reaches across decades to break down the body in middle age and later.”

Lara Tang ’18, a human and evolutionary biology concentrator who recently joined the team as a research assistant, relishes the opportunity to help find some of those answers. She joined the effort after coming across Waldinger’s TED talk in one of her classes.

“That motivated me to do more research on adult development,” said Tang. “I want to see how childhood experiences affect developments of physical health, mental health, and happiness later in life.”

Asked what lessons he has learned from the study, Waldinger, who is a Zen priest, said he practices meditation daily and invests time and energy in his relationships, more than before.

“It’s easy to get isolated, to get caught up in work and not remembering, ‘Oh, I haven’t seen these friends in a long time,’ ” Waldinger said. “So I try to pay more attention to my relationships than I used to.”

Share this article

Also in this series:.

research study on human development

Probe of Alzheimer’s follows paths of infection

Starting with microbes, Harvard-MGH researchers outline a devastating chain of events

research study on human development

To age better, eat better

Much of life is beyond our control, but dining smartly can help us live healthier, longer

senior health care

The balance in healthy aging

To grow old well requires minimizing accidents, such as falling, as well as ailments

research study on human development

How old can we get? It might be written in stem cells

No clock, no crystal ball, but lots of excitement — and ambition — among Harvard scientists

Harvard Alzheimers Research

Plotting the demise of Alzheimer’s

New study is major test for power of early action

You might like

Biomolecular archaeologist looks at why most of world’s population has trouble digesting beverage that helped shape civilization

Full body portrait of Molly F. Przeworski.

Biologist separates reality of science from the claims of profiling firms

Doctor holding endoscope.

Study finds minimally invasive treatment more cost-effective over time, brings greater weight loss

Epic science inside a cubic millimeter of brain

Researchers publish largest-ever dataset of neural connections

How far has COVID set back students?

An economist, a policy expert, and a teacher explain why learning losses are worse than many parents realize

research study on human development

Society for the Study of Human Development

Research in Human Development 

Editors: Jennifer Brown Urban and Miriam Linver

Research in Human Development (RHD)  is the flagship journal of SSHD.  RHD is a lifespan, interdisciplinary, and integrative journal which focuses on thematic special issues.

To view the most recent table of contents, click here . To access exclusive content of RHD,  first Log In and then go to Members Access . If you are not a member, you will need to Join or Renew your membership. Research in Human Development is published by Taylor & Francis

Editorial Board

Anthony Ong, Cornell University Rachel Razza, Syracuse University Kim Ferguson, Sarah Lawrence College Zoe Klemfus, University of California, Irvine Laura Wray Lake, University of California, Los Angeles Rochelle Dalla, University of Nebraska - Lincoln Alison Sidle Fuligni, California State University, Los Angeles Valentina Lucia La Rosa, University of Catania

Editorial Scope

Research in Human Development seeks to promote an inclusive, integrative, and interdisciplinary approach to the study of human development across the entire lifespan. The integration of the person and their context across the life course is the focus of the scholarship promoted by this journal. Specifically, an interdisciplinary focus that incorporates biological, psychological, sociological, anthropological, economic, and historical perspectives will be stressed in the pages of RHD. To encompass such breadth, the publication will incorporate a variety of research methods, including quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods approaches. The journal seeks to promote scholarship on the social, cultural, and national (global) contexts of human development across diverse ethnic and racial groups employing multiple methods. Finally, RHD seeks to promote scholarship that informs public policies and social programs that promote positive and healthy human development for all people. In short, by providing a forum for research that is lifespan-oriented, interdisciplinary, contextual, and methodologically diverse, the journal will continue to significantly advance the field of human development.

The audience consists of researchers, educators, policy makers, and practitioners in all fields which address human development.

Special Issue Proposals

RHD publishes thematic issues devoted to the integration of theory and research across the lifespan representing multiple disciplines and methodologies. This journal uses Routledge’s Submission Portal to manage the submission process. You will need an ORCiD ID to log in to the submission portal. If you do not already have an ORCiD account, creating one takes 30 seconds and will allow you to login to a variety of other publishers’ systems as well as uniquely identifying your contribution to the scholarly record.

We are particularly interested in proposals for special issues that provide cutting-edge perspectives on pressing topics such as gun violence, Autism, whole reading vs phonics, bullying, abortion rights and access, and global implications of climate change. We are also interested in special issues that focus on development in historically marginalized populations (e.g., racial/ethnic, LGBTQIA+, (dis)ability) and use less conventional analytical techniques (e.g., community-based participatory approaches, culturally responsive methods, person-centered analyses).

If you have an idea for a proposal, we recommend reaching out to the co-editors to talk through the feasibility and fit with RHD. Once you are ready to submit your proposal (no more than 5 single-spaced pages, please), it should describe:

  • The theme for the issue and its significance for the field;
  • How the collected papers meet the interdisciplinary, multi method, and lifespan goals of the journal;
  • Biographical information of the editors, especially highlighting prior editorial experience;
  • The titles and authors of each potential paper, as well as a paragraph describing the scope/questions/data source/analytic strategies of each paper;
  • How the papers come together in important ways, as a collection, to inform the theme;
  • List of proposed contributors and their titles and affiliations; and
  • References, as appropriate

Most issues have 5 papers of about 25 pages each (not counting the title and abstract pages, but including everything else). The organization may vary by project. For example: a very brief introduction of a few pages, plus 5 empirical papers; or, a brief introduction, plus 1 theoretical/conceptual/literature-based manuscript and 4 empirical papers; or, a very brief introduction, plus 4 empirical pieces and a discussion essay. These proposals will be reviewed by the editors in consultation with the Editorial Board. Once a proposal has been reviewed and accepted, the manuscripts must be accompanied by a statement that they have not been published elsewhere and that the papers have not been submitted simultaneously for publication elsewhere. Authors are responsible for obtaining permission to reproduce copyrighted material from other sources and are required to sign an agreement for the transfer of copyright to the publisher. All accepted manuscripts, artwork, and photographs become the property of the publisher.

If you have a Special Issue idea or proposal, please send an email to Drs. Urban ( [email protected] )   and Linver ( [email protected] ).

Open Access

You have the option to publish open access in this journal via our Open Select publishing program. Publishing open access means that your article will be free to access online immediately on publication, increasing the visibility, readership and impact of your research. Articles published Open Select with Taylor & Francis typically receive 95% more citations* and over 7 times as many downloads** compared to those that are not published Open Select.

Your research funder or your institution may require you to publish your article open access. Visit our Author Services website to find out more about open access policies and how you can comply with these.

You will be asked to pay an article publishing charge (APC) to make your article open access and this cost can often be covered by your institution or funder. Use our APC finder to view the APC for this journal.

Please visit our Author Services website if you would like more information about our Open Select Program.

*Citations received up to 9th June 2021 for articles published in 2016-2020 in journals listed in Web of Science®. Data obtained on 9th June 2021, from Digital Science's Dimensions platform, available at https://app.dimensions.ai **Usage in 2018-2020 for articles published in 2016-2020.

Peer Review and Ethics

Taylor & Francis is committed to peer-review integrity and upholding the highest standards of review. Once your special issue proposal has been assessed for suitability by the editors, the guest editor(s) will be responsible for managing single blind peer review by independent, anonymous expert referees. If any of the issue authors have shared earlier versions of their manuscript on a preprint server, please be aware that anonymity cannot be guaranteed. Further information on our preprints policy and citation requirements can be found on our Preprints Author Services page which also contains our guidance on publishing ethics and more information about what to expect during peer review.

Logo for Maricopa Open Digital Press

Research in Developmental Psychology

What you’ll learn to do: examine how to do research in lifespan development.

Desk shown from above, pair of hands seen gesturing towards a graph

How do we know what changes and stays the same (and when and why) in lifespan development? We rely on research that utilizes the scientific method so that we can have confidence in the findings. How data are collected may vary by age group and by the type of information sought. The developmental design (for example, following individuals as they age over time or comparing individuals of different ages at one point in time) will affect the data and the conclusions that can be drawn from them about actual age changes. What do you think are the particular challenges or issues in conducting developmental research, such as with infants and children? Read on to learn more.

Learning outcomes

  • Explain how the scientific method is used in researching development
  • Compare various types and objectives of developmental research
  • Describe methods for collecting research data (including observation, survey, case study, content analysis, and secondary content analysis)
  • Explain correlational research
  • Describe the value of experimental research
  • Compare the advantages and disadvantages of developmental research designs (cross-sectional, longitudinal, and sequential)
  • Describe challenges associated with conducting research in lifespan development

Research in Lifespan Development

How do we know what we know.

question mark

An important part of learning any science is having a basic knowledge of the techniques used in gathering information. The hallmark of scientific investigation is that of following a set of procedures designed to keep questioning or skepticism alive while describing, explaining, or testing any phenomenon. Not long ago a friend said to me that he did not trust academicians or researchers because they always seem to change their story. That, however, is exactly what science is all about; it involves continuously renewing our understanding of the subjects in question and an ongoing investigation of how and why events occur. Science is a vehicle for going on a never-ending journey. In the area of development, we have seen changes in recommendations for nutrition, in explanations of psychological states as people age, and in parenting advice. So think of learning about human development as a lifelong endeavor.

Personal Knowledge

How do we know what we know? Take a moment to write down two things that you know about childhood. Okay. Now, how do you know? Chances are you know these things based on your own history (experiential reality), what others have told you, or cultural ideas (agreement reality) (Seccombe and Warner, 2004). There are several problems with personal inquiry or drawing conclusions based on our personal experiences.

Our assumptions very often guide our perceptions, consequently, when we believe something, we tend to see it even if it is not there. Have you heard the saying, “seeing is believing”? Well, the truth is just the opposite: believing is seeing. This problem may just be a result of cognitive ‘blinders’ or it may be part of a more conscious attempt to support our own views. Confirmation bias is the tendency to look for evidence that we are right and in so doing, we ignore contradictory evidence.

Philosopher Karl Popper suggested that the distinction between that which is scientific and that which is unscientific is that science is falsifiable; scientific inquiry involves attempts to reject or refute a theory or set of assumptions (Thornton, 2005). A theory that cannot be falsified is not scientific. And much of what we do in personal inquiry involves drawing conclusions based on what we have personally experienced or validating our own experience by discussing what we think is true with others who share the same views.

Science offers a more systematic way to make comparisons and guard against bias. One technique used to avoid sampling bias is to select participants for a study in a random way. This means using a technique to ensure that all members have an equal chance of being selected. Simple random sampling may involve using a set of random numbers as a guide in determining who is to be selected. For example, if we have a list of 400 people and wish to randomly select a smaller group or sample to be studied, we use a list of random numbers and select the case that corresponds with that number (Case 39, 3, 217, etc.). This is preferable to asking only those individuals with whom we are familiar to participate in a study; if we conveniently chose only people we know, we know nothing about those who had no opportunity to be selected. There are many more elaborate techniques that can be used to obtain samples that represent the composition of the population we are studying. But even though a randomly selected representative sample is preferable, it is not always used because of costs and other limitations. As a consumer of research, however, you should know how the sample was obtained and keep this in mind when interpreting results. It is possible that what was found was limited to that sample or similar individuals and not generalizable to everyone else.

Scientific Methods

The particular method used to conduct research may vary by discipline and since lifespan development is multidisciplinary, more than one method may be used to study human development. One method of scientific investigation involves the following steps:

  • Determining a research question
  • Reviewing previous studies addressing the topic in question (known as a literature review)
  • Determining a method of gathering information
  • Conducting the study
  • Interpreting the results
  • Drawing conclusions; stating limitations of the study and suggestions for future research
  • Making the findings available to others (both to share information and to have the work scrutinized by others)

The findings of these scientific studies can then be used by others as they explore the area of interest. Through this process, a literature or knowledge base is established. This model of scientific investigation presents research as a linear process guided by a specific research question. And it typically involves quantitative research , which relies on numerical data or using statistics to understand and report what has been studied.

Another model of research, referred to as qualitative research, may involve steps such as these:

  • Begin with a broad area of interest and a research question
  • Gain entrance into a group to be researched
  • Gather field notes about the setting, the people, the structure, the activities, or other areas of interest
  • Ask open-ended, broad “grand tour” types of questions when interviewing subjects
  • Modify research questions as the study continues
  • Note patterns or consistencies
  • Explore new areas deemed important by the people being observed
  • Report findings

In this type of research, theoretical ideas are “grounded” in the experiences of the participants. The researcher is the student and the people in the setting are the teachers as they inform the researcher of their world (Glazer & Strauss, 1967). Researchers should be aware of their own biases and assumptions, acknowledge them, and bracket them in efforts to keep them from limiting accuracy in reporting. Sometimes qualitative studies are used initially to explore a topic and more quantitative studies are used to test or explain what was first described.

A good way to become more familiar with these scientific research methods, both quantitative and qualitative, is to look at journal articles, which are written in sections that follow these steps in the scientific process. Most psychological articles and many papers in the social sciences follow the writing guidelines and format dictated by the  American Psychological Association  (APA). In general, the structure follows: abstract (summary of the article), introduction or literature review, methods explaining how the study was conducted, results of the study, discussion and interpretation of findings, and references.

Link to Learning

Brené Brown is a bestselling author and social work professor at the University of Houston. She conducts grounded theory research by collecting qualitative data from large numbers of participants. In Brené Brown’s TED Talk The Power of Vulnerability , Brown refers to herself as a storyteller-researcher as she explains her research process and summarizes her results.

Research Methods and Objectives

The main categories of psychological research are descriptive, correlational, and experimental research. Research studies that do not test specific relationships between variables are called  descriptive, or qualitative, studies . These studies are used to describe general or specific behaviors and attributes that are observed and measured. In the early stages of research, it might be difficult to form a hypothesis, especially when there is not any existing literature in the area. In these situations designing an experiment would be premature, as the question of interest is not yet clearly defined as a hypothesis. Often a researcher will begin with a non-experimental approach, such as a descriptive study, to gather more information about the topic before designing an experiment or correlational study to address a specific hypothesis. Some examples of descriptive questions include:

  • “How much time do parents spend with their children?”
  • “How many times per week do couples have intercourse?”
  • “When is marital satisfaction greatest?”

The main types of descriptive studies include observation, case studies, surveys, and content analysis (which we’ll examine further in the module). Descriptive research is distinct from  correlational research , in which psychologists formally test whether a relationship exists between two or more variables.  Experimental research  goes a step further beyond descriptive and correlational research and randomly assigns people to different conditions, using hypothesis testing to make inferences about how these conditions affect behavior. Some experimental research includes explanatory studies, which are efforts to answer the question “why” such as:

  • “Why have rates of divorce leveled off?”
  • “Why are teen pregnancy rates down?”
  • “Why has the average life expectancy increased?”

Evaluation research is designed to assess the effectiveness of policies or programs. For instance, research might be designed to study the effectiveness of safety programs implemented in schools for installing car seats or fitting bicycle helmets. Do children who have been exposed to the safety programs wear their helmets? Do parents use car seats properly? If not, why not?

Research Methods

We have just learned about some of the various models and objectives of research in lifespan development. Now we’ll dig deeper to understand the methods and techniques used to describe, explain, or evaluate behavior.

All types of research methods have unique strengths and weaknesses, and each method may only be appropriate for certain types of research questions. For example, studies that rely primarily on observation produce incredible amounts of information, but the ability to apply this information to the larger population is somewhat limited because of small sample sizes. Survey research, on the other hand, allows researchers to easily collect data from relatively large samples. While this allows for results to be generalized to the larger population more easily, the information that can be collected on any given survey is somewhat limited and subject to problems associated with any type of self-reported data. Some researchers conduct archival research by using existing records. While this can be a fairly inexpensive way to collect data that can provide insight into a number of research questions, researchers using this approach have no control over how or what kind of data was collected.

Types of Descriptive Research

Observation.

Observational studies , also called naturalistic observation, involve watching and recording the actions of participants. This may take place in the natural setting, such as observing children at play in a park, or behind a one-way glass while children are at play in a laboratory playroom. The researcher may follow a checklist and record the frequency and duration of events (perhaps how many conflicts occur among 2-year-olds) or may observe and record as much as possible about an event as a participant (such as attending an Alcoholics Anonymous meeting and recording the slogans on the walls, the structure of the meeting, the expressions commonly used, etc.). The researcher may be a participant or a non-participant. What would be the strengths of being a participant? What would be the weaknesses?

In general, observational studies have the strength of allowing the researcher to see how people behave rather than relying on self-report. One weakness of self-report studies is that what people do and what they say they do are often very different. A major weakness of observational studies is that they do not allow the researcher to explain causal relationships. Yet, observational studies are useful and widely used when studying children. It is important to remember that most people tend to change their behavior when they know they are being watched (known as the Hawthorne effect ) and children may not survey well.

Case Studies

Case studies  involve exploring a single case or situation in great detail. Information may be gathered with the use of observation, interviews, testing, or other methods to uncover as much as possible about a person or situation. Case studies are helpful when investigating unusual situations such as brain trauma or children reared in isolation. And they are often used by clinicians who conduct case studies as part of their normal practice when gathering information about a client or patient coming in for treatment. Case studies can be used to explore areas about which little is known and can provide rich detail about situations or conditions. However, the findings from case studies cannot be generalized or applied to larger populations; this is because cases are not randomly selected and no control group is used for comparison. (Read The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat by Dr. Oliver Sacks as a good example of the case study approach.)

A person is checking off boxes on a paper survey

Surveys  are familiar to most people because they are so widely used. Surveys enhance accessibility to subjects because they can be conducted in person, over the phone, through the mail, or online. A survey involves asking a standard set of questions to a group of subjects. In a highly structured survey, subjects are forced to choose from a response set such as “strongly disagree, disagree, undecided, agree, strongly agree”; or “0, 1-5, 6-10, etc.” Surveys are commonly used by sociologists, marketing researchers, political scientists, therapists, and others to gather information on many variables in a relatively short period of time. Surveys typically yield surface information on a wide variety of factors, but may not allow for an in-depth understanding of human behavior.

Surveys are useful in examining stated values, attitudes, opinions, and reporting on practices. However, they are based on self-report, or what people say they do rather than on observation, and this can limit accuracy. Validity refers to accuracy and reliability refers to consistency in responses to tests and other measures; great care is taken to ensure the validity and reliability of surveys.

Content Analysis

Content analysis  involves looking at media such as old texts, pictures, commercials, lyrics, or other materials to explore patterns or themes in culture. An example of content analysis is the classic history of childhood by Aries (1962) called “Centuries of Childhood” or the analysis of television commercials for sexual or violent content or for ageism. Passages in text or television programs can be randomly selected for analysis as well. Again, one advantage of analyzing work such as this is that the researcher does not have to go through the time and expense of finding respondents, but the researcher cannot know how accurately the media reflects the actions and sentiments of the population.

Secondary content analysis, or archival research, involves analyzing information that has already been collected or examining documents or media to uncover attitudes, practices, or preferences. There are a number of data sets available to those who wish to conduct this type of research. The researcher conducting secondary analysis does not have to recruit subjects but does need to know the quality of the information collected in the original study. And unfortunately, the researcher is limited to the questions asked and data collected originally.

Correlational and Experimental Research

Correlational research.

When scientists passively observe and measure phenomena it is called correlational research . Here, researchers do not intervene and change behavior, as they do in experiments. In correlational research, the goal is to identify patterns of relationships, but not cause and effect. Importantly, with correlational research, you can examine only two variables at a time, no more and no less.

So, what if you wanted to test whether spending money on others is related to happiness, but you don’t have $20 to give to each participant in order to have them spend it for your experiment? You could use a correlational design—which is exactly what Professor Elizabeth Dunn (2008) at the University of British Columbia did when she conducted research on spending and happiness. She asked people how much of their income they spent on others or donated to charity, and later she asked them how happy they were. Do you think these two variables were related? Yes, they were! The more money people reported spending on others, the happier they were.

Understanding Correlation

Scatterplot of the association between happiness and ratings of the past month, a positive correlation (r = .81)

With a positive correlation , the two variables go up or down together. In a scatterplot, the dots form a pattern that extends from the bottom left to the upper right (just as they do in Figure 1). The r value for a positive correlation is indicated by a positive number (although, the positive sign is usually omitted). Here, the r value is .81. For the example above, the direction of the association is positive. This means that people who perceived the past month as being good reported feeling happier, whereas people who perceived the month as being bad reported feeling less happy.

A negative correlation is one in which the two variables move in opposite directions. That is, as one variable goes up, the other goes down. Figure 2 shows the association between the average height of males in a country (y-axis) and the pathogen prevalence (or commonness of disease; x-axis) of that country. In this scatterplot, each dot represents a country. Notice how the dots extend from the top left to the bottom right. What does this mean in real-world terms? It means that people are shorter in parts of the world where there is more disease. The r-value for a negative correlation is indicated by a negative number—that is, it has a minus (–) sign in front of it. Here, it is –.83.

Scatterplot showing the association between average male height and pathogen prevalence, a negative correlation (r = –.83).

Experimental Research

Experiments  are designed to test  hypotheses  (or specific statements about the relationship between  variables ) in a controlled setting in an effort to explain how certain factors or events produce outcomes. A variable is anything that changes in value. Concepts are operationalized  or transformed into variables in research which means that the researcher must specify exactly what is going to be measured in the study. For example, if we are interested in studying marital satisfaction, we have to specify what marital satisfaction really means or what we are going to use as an indicator of marital satisfaction. What is something measurable that would indicate some level of marital satisfaction? Would it be the amount of time couples spend together each day? Or eye contact during a discussion about money? Or maybe a subject’s score on a marital satisfaction scale? Each of these is measurable but these may not be equally valid or accurate indicators of marital satisfaction. What do you think? These are the kinds of considerations researchers must make when working through the design.

The experimental method is the only research method that can measure cause and effect relationships between variables. Three conditions must be met in order to establish cause and effect. Experimental designs are useful in meeting these conditions:

  • The independent and dependent variables must be related.  In other words, when one is altered, the other changes in response. The independent variable is something altered or introduced by the researcher; sometimes thought of as the treatment or intervention. The dependent variable is the outcome or the factor affected by the introduction of the independent variable; the dependent variable  depends on the independent variable. For example, if we are looking at the impact of exercise on stress levels, the independent variable would be exercise; the dependent variable would be stress.
  • The cause must come before the effect.  Experiments measure subjects on the dependent variable before exposing them to the independent variable (establishing a baseline). So we would measure the subjects’ level of stress before introducing exercise and then again after the exercise to see if there has been a change in stress levels. (Observational and survey research does not always allow us to look at the timing of these events which makes understanding causality problematic with these methods.)
  • The cause must be isolated.  The researcher must ensure that no outside, perhaps unknown variables, are actually causing the effect we see. The experimental design helps make this possible. In an experiment, we would make sure that our subjects’ diets were held constant throughout the exercise program. Otherwise, the diet might really be creating a change in stress level rather than exercise.

A basic experimental design involves beginning with a sample (or subset of a population) and randomly assigning subjects to one of two groups: the  experimental group or the control group . Ideally, to prevent bias, the participants would be blind to their condition (not aware of which group they are in) and the researchers would also be blind to each participant’s condition (referred to as “ double blind “). The experimental group is the group that is going to be exposed to an independent variable or condition the researcher is introducing as a potential cause of an event. The control group is going to be used for comparison and is going to have the same experience as the experimental group but will not be exposed to the independent variable. This helps address the placebo effect, which is that a group may expect changes to happen just by participating. After exposing the experimental group to the independent variable, the two groups are measured again to see if a change has occurred. If so, we are in a better position to suggest that the independent variable caused the change in the dependent variable . The basic experimental model looks like this:

The major advantage of the experimental design is that of helping to establish cause and effect relationships. A disadvantage of this design is the difficulty of translating much of what concerns us about human behavior into a laboratory setting.

Developmental Research Designs

Now you know about some tools used to conduct research about human development. Remember,  research methods  are tools that are used to collect information. But it is easy to confuse research methods and research design. Research design is the strategy or blueprint for deciding how to collect and analyze information. Research design dictates which methods are used and how. Developmental research designs are techniques used particularly in lifespan development research. When we are trying to describe development and change, the research designs become especially important because we are interested in what changes and what stays the same with age. These techniques try to examine how age, cohort, gender, and social class impact development.

Cross-sectional designs

The majority of developmental studies use cross-sectional designs because they are less time-consuming and less expensive than other developmental designs. Cross-sectional research designs are used to examine behavior in participants of different ages who are tested at the same point in time. Let’s suppose that researchers are interested in the relationship between intelligence and aging. They might have a hypothesis (an educated guess, based on theory or observations) that intelligence declines as people get older. The researchers might choose to give a certain intelligence test to individuals who are 20 years old, individuals who are 50 years old, and individuals who are 80 years old at the same time and compare the data from each age group. This research is cross-sectional in design because the researchers plan to examine the intelligence scores of individuals of different ages within the same study at the same time; they are taking a “cross-section” of people at one point in time. Let’s say that the comparisons find that the 80-year-old adults score lower on the intelligence test than the 50-year-old adults, and the 50-year-old adults score lower on the intelligence test than the 20-year-old adults. Based on these data, the researchers might conclude that individuals become less intelligent as they get older. Would that be a valid (accurate) interpretation of the results?

Text stating that the year of study is 2010 and an experiment looks at cohort A with 20 year olds, cohort B of 50 year olds and cohort C with 80 year olds

No, that would not be a valid conclusion because the researchers did not follow individuals as they aged from 20 to 50 to 80 years old. One of the primary limitations of cross-sectional research is that the results yield information about age differences  not necessarily changes with age or over time. That is, although the study described above can show that in 2010, the 80-year-olds scored lower on the intelligence test than the 50-year-olds, and the 50-year-olds scored lower on the intelligence test than the 20-year-olds, the data used to come up with this conclusion were collected from different individuals (or groups of individuals). It could be, for instance, that when these 20-year-olds get older (50 and eventually 80), they will still score just as high on the intelligence test as they did at age 20. In a similar way, maybe the 80-year-olds would have scored relatively low on the intelligence test even at ages 50 and 20; the researchers don’t know for certain because they did not follow the same individuals as they got older.

It is also possible that the differences found between the age groups are not due to age, per se, but due to cohort effects. The 80-year-olds in this 2010 research grew up during a particular time and experienced certain events as a group. They were born in 1930 and are part of the Traditional or Silent Generation. The 50-year-olds were born in 1960 and are members of the Baby Boomer cohort. The 20-year-olds were born in 1990 and are part of the Millennial or Gen Y Generation. What kinds of things did each of these cohorts experience that the others did not experience or at least not in the same ways?

You may have come up with many differences between these cohorts’ experiences, such as living through certain wars, political and social movements, economic conditions, advances in technology, changes in health and nutrition standards, etc. There may be particular cohort differences that could especially influence their performance on intelligence tests, such as education level and use of computers. That is, many of those born in 1930 probably did not complete high school; those born in 1960 may have high school degrees, on average, but the majority did not attain college degrees; the young adults are probably current college students. And this is not even considering additional factors such as gender, race, or socioeconomic status. The young adults are used to taking tests on computers, but the members of the other two cohorts did not grow up with computers and may not be as comfortable if the intelligence test is administered on computers. These factors could have been a factor in the research results.

Another disadvantage of cross-sectional research is that it is limited to one time of measurement. Data are collected at one point in time and it’s possible that something could have happened in that year in history that affected all of the participants, although possibly each cohort may have been affected differently. Just think about the mindsets of participants in research that was conducted in the United States right after the terrorist attacks on September 11, 2001.

Longitudinal research designs

Middle aged woman holding own photograph of her younger self.

Longitudinal   research involves beginning with a group of people who may be of the same age and background (cohort) and measuring them repeatedly over a long period of time. One of the benefits of this type of research is that people can be followed through time and be compared with themselves when they were younger; therefore changes with age over time are measured. What would be the advantages and disadvantages of longitudinal research? Problems with this type of research include being expensive, taking a long time, and subjects dropping out over time. Think about the film, 63 Up , part of the Up Series mentioned earlier, which is an example of following individuals over time. In the videos, filmed every seven years, you see how people change physically, emotionally, and socially through time; and some remain the same in certain ways, too. But many of the participants really disliked being part of the project and repeatedly threatened to quit; one disappeared for several years; another died before her 63rd year. Would you want to be interviewed every seven years? Would you want to have it made public for all to watch?   

Longitudinal research designs are used to examine behavior in the same individuals over time. For instance, with our example of studying intelligence and aging, a researcher might conduct a longitudinal study to examine whether 20-year-olds become less intelligent with age over time. To this end, a researcher might give an intelligence test to individuals when they are 20 years old, again when they are 50 years old, and then again when they are 80 years old. This study is longitudinal in nature because the researcher plans to study the same individuals as they age. Based on these data, the pattern of intelligence and age might look different than from the cross-sectional research; it might be found that participants’ intelligence scores are higher at age 50 than at age 20 and then remain stable or decline a little by age 80. How can that be when cross-sectional research revealed declines in intelligence with age?

The same person, "Person A" is 20 years old in 2010, 50 years old in 2040, and 80 in 2070.

Since longitudinal research happens over a period of time (which could be short term, as in months, but is often longer, as in years), there is a risk of attrition. Attrition occurs when participants fail to complete all portions of a study. Participants may move, change their phone numbers, die, or simply become disinterested in participating over time. Researchers should account for the possibility of attrition by enrolling a larger sample into their study initially, as some participants will likely drop out over time. There is also something known as  selective attrition— this means that certain groups of individuals may tend to drop out. It is often the least healthy, least educated, and lower socioeconomic participants who tend to drop out over time. That means that the remaining participants may no longer be representative of the whole population, as they are, in general, healthier, better educated, and have more money. This could be a factor in why our hypothetical research found a more optimistic picture of intelligence and aging as the years went by. What can researchers do about selective attrition? At each time of testing, they could randomly recruit more participants from the same cohort as the original members, to replace those who have dropped out.

The results from longitudinal studies may also be impacted by repeated assessments. Consider how well you would do on a math test if you were given the exact same exam every day for a week. Your performance would likely improve over time, not necessarily because you developed better math abilities, but because you were continuously practicing the same math problems. This phenomenon is known as a practice effect. Practice effects occur when participants become better at a task over time because they have done it again and again (not due to natural psychological development). So our participants may have become familiar with the intelligence test each time (and with the computerized testing administration). Another limitation of longitudinal research is that the data are limited to only one cohort.

Sequential research designs

Sequential research designs include elements of both longitudinal and cross-sectional research designs. Similar to longitudinal designs, sequential research features participants who are followed over time; similar to cross-sectional designs, sequential research includes participants of different ages. This research design is also distinct from those that have been discussed previously in that individuals of different ages are enrolled into a study at various points in time to examine age-related changes, development within the same individuals as they age, and to account for the possibility of cohort and/or time of measurement effects. In 1965, K. Warner Schaie described particular sequential designs: cross-sequential, cohort sequential, and time-sequential. The differences between them depended on which variables were focused on for analyses of the data (data could be viewed in terms of multiple cross-sectional designs or multiple longitudinal designs or multiple cohort designs). Ideally, by comparing results from the different types of analyses, the effects of age, cohort, and time in history could be separated out.

Challenges Conducting Developmental Research

The previous sections describe research tools to assess development across the lifespan, as well as the ways that research designs can be used to track age-related changes and development over time. Before you begin conducting developmental research, however, you must also be aware that testing individuals of certain ages (such as infants and children) or making comparisons across ages (such as children compared to teens) comes with its own unique set of challenges. In the final section of this module, let’s look at some of the main issues that are encountered when conducting developmental research, namely ethical concerns, recruitment issues, and participant attrition.

Ethical Concerns

You may already know that Institutional Review Boards (IRBs) must review and approve all research projects that are conducted at universities, hospitals, and other institutions (each broad discipline or field, such as psychology or social work, often has its own code of ethics that must also be followed, regardless of institutional affiliation). An IRB is typically a panel of experts who read and evaluate proposals for research. IRB members want to ensure that the proposed research will be carried out ethically and that the potential benefits of the research outweigh the risks and potential harm (psychological as well as physical harm) for participants.

What you may not know though, is that the IRB considers some groups of participants to be more vulnerable or at-risk than others. Whereas university students are generally not viewed as vulnerable or at-risk, infants and young children commonly fall into this category. What makes infants and young children more vulnerable during research than young adults? One reason infants and young children are perceived as being at increased risk is due to their limited cognitive capabilities, which makes them unable to state their willingness to participate in research or tell researchers when they would like to drop out of a study. For these reasons, infants and young children require special accommodations as they participate in the research process. Similar issues and accommodations would apply to adults who are deemed to be of limited cognitive capabilities.

When thinking about special accommodations in developmental research, consider the informed consent process. If you have ever participated in scientific research, you may know through your own experience that adults commonly sign an informed consent statement (a contract stating that they agree to participate in research) after learning about a study. As part of this process, participants are informed of the procedures to be used in the research, along with any expected risks or benefits. Infants and young children cannot verbally indicate their willingness to participate, much less understand the balance of potential risks and benefits. As such, researchers are oftentimes required to obtain written informed consent from the parent or legal guardian of the child participant, an adult who is almost always present as the study is conducted. In fact, children are not asked to indicate whether they would like to be involved in a study at all (a process known as assent) until they are approximately seven years old. Because infants and young children cannot easily indicate if they would like to discontinue their participation in a study, researchers must be sensitive to changes in the state of the participant (determining whether a child is too tired or upset to continue) as well as to parent desires (in some cases, parents might want to discontinue their involvement in the research). As in adult studies, researchers must always strive to protect the rights and well-being of the minor participants and their parents when conducting developmental research.

Recruitment

An additional challenge in developmental science is participant recruitment. Recruiting university students to participate in adult studies is typically easy.  Unfortunately, young children cannot be recruited in this way. Given these limitations, how do researchers go about finding infants and young children to be in their studies?

The answer to this question varies along multiple dimensions. Researchers must consider the number of participants they need and the financial resources available to them, among other things. Location may also be an important consideration. Researchers who need large numbers of infants and children may attempt to recruit them by obtaining infant birth records from the state, county, or province in which they reside. Researchers can choose to pay a recruitment agency to contact and recruit families for them.  More economical recruitment options include posting advertisements and fliers in locations frequented by families, such as mommy-and-me classes, local malls, and preschools or daycare centers. Researchers can also utilize online social media outlets like Facebook, which allows users to post recruitment advertisements for a small fee. Of course, each of these different recruitment techniques requires IRB approval. And if children are recruited and/or tested in school settings, permission would need to be obtained ahead of time from teachers, schools, and school districts (as well as informed consent from parents or guardians).

And what about the recruitment of adults? While it is easy to recruit young college students to participate in research, some would argue that it is too easy and that college students are samples of convenience. They are not randomly selected from the wider population, and they may not represent all young adults in our society (this was particularly true in the past with certain cohorts, as college students tended to be mainly white males of high socioeconomic status). In fact, in the early research on aging, this type of convenience sample was compared with another type of convenience sample—young college students tended to be compared with residents of nursing homes! Fortunately, it didn’t take long for researchers to realize that older adults in nursing homes are not representative of the older population; they tend to be the oldest and sickest (physically and/or psychologically). Those initial studies probably painted an overly negative view of aging, as young adults in college were being compared to older adults who were not healthy, had not been in school nor taken tests in many decades, and probably did not graduate high school, let alone college. As we can see, recruitment and random sampling can be significant issues in research with adults, as well as infants and children. For instance, how and where would you recruit middle-aged adults to participate in your research?

A tired looking mother closes her eyes and rubs her forehead as her baby cries.

Another important consideration when conducting research with infants and young children is attrition . Although attrition is quite common in longitudinal research in particular (see the previous section on longitudinal designs for an example of high attrition rates and selective attrition in lifespan developmental research), it is also problematic in developmental science more generally, as studies with infants and young children tend to have higher attrition rates than studies with adults.  Infants and young children are more likely to tire easily, become fussy, and lose interest in the study procedures than are adults. For these reasons, research studies should be designed to be as short as possible – it is likely better to break up a large study into multiple short sessions rather than cram all of the tasks into one long visit to the lab. Researchers should also allow time for breaks in their study protocols so that infants can rest or have snacks as needed. Happy, comfortable participants provide the best data.

Conclusions

Lifespan development is a fascinating field of study – but care must be taken to ensure that researchers use appropriate methods to examine human behavior, use the correct experimental design to answer their questions, and be aware of the special challenges that are part-and-parcel of developmental research. After reading this module, you should have a solid understanding of these various issues and be ready to think more critically about research questions that interest you. For example, what types of questions do you have about lifespan development? What types of research would you like to conduct? Many interesting questions remain to be examined by future generations of developmental scientists – maybe you will make one of the next big discoveries!

Woman reading to two young children

Lifespan development is the scientific study of how and why people change or remain the same over time. As we are beginning to see, lifespan development involves multiple domains and many ages and stages that are important in and of themselves, but that are also interdependent and dynamic and need to be viewed holistically. There are many influences on lifespan development at individual and societal levels (including genetics); cultural, generational, economic, and historical contexts are often significant. And how developmental research is designed and data are collected, analyzed, and interpreted can affect what is discovered about human development across the lifespan.

Lifespan Development Copyright © 2020 by Julie Lazzara is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Library homepage

  • school Campus Bookshelves
  • menu_book Bookshelves
  • perm_media Learning Objects
  • login Login
  • how_to_reg Request Instructor Account
  • hub Instructor Commons
  • Download Page (PDF)
  • Download Full Book (PDF)
  • Periodic Table
  • Physics Constants
  • Scientific Calculator
  • Reference & Cite
  • Tools expand_more
  • Readability

selected template will load here

This action is not available.

Social Sci LibreTexts

1.8: Research in Lifespan Development

  • Last updated
  • Save as PDF
  • Page ID 60385
  • Lumen Learning

\( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

\( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)

\( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)

\( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)

\( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)

\( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)

\( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

\( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\)

\( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\)

\( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)

\( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\)

\( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)

\( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\)

\( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)

\( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)

\( \newcommand{\vectorA}[1]{\vec{#1}}      % arrow\)

\( \newcommand{\vectorAt}[1]{\vec{\text{#1}}}      % arrow\)

\( \newcommand{\vectorB}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

\( \newcommand{\vectorC}[1]{\textbf{#1}} \)

\( \newcommand{\vectorD}[1]{\overrightarrow{#1}} \)

\( \newcommand{\vectorDt}[1]{\overrightarrow{\text{#1}}} \)

\( \newcommand{\vectE}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash{\mathbf {#1}}}} \)

Learning Outcomes

  • Explain how the scientific method is used in researching development
  • Compare various types and objectives of developmental research

How do we know what we know?

A question mark.

An important part of learning any science is having a basic knowledge of the techniques used in gathering information. The hallmark of scientific investigation is that of following a set of procedures designed to keep questioning or skepticism alive while describing, explaining, or testing any phenomenon. Not long ago a friend said to me that he did not trust academicians or researchers because they always seem to change their story. That, however, is exactly what science is all about; it involves continuously renewing our understanding of the subjects in question and an ongoing investigation of how and why events occur. Science is a vehicle for going on a never-ending journey. In the area of development, we have seen changes in recommendations for nutrition, in explanations of psychological states as people age, and in parenting advice. So think of learning about human development as a lifelong endeavor.

Personal Knowledge

How do we know what we know? Take a moment to write down two things that you know about childhood. Okay. Now, how do you know? Chances are you know these things based on your own history (experiential reality), what others have told you, or cultural ideas (agreement reality) (Seccombe and Warner, 2004). There are several problems with personal inquiry, or drawing conclusions based on our personal experiences. Read the following sentence aloud:

Paris in the the spring

Are you sure that is what it said? Read it again:

If you read it differently the second time (adding the second “the”) you just experienced one of the problems with relying on personal inquiry; that is, the tendency to see what we believe. Our assumptions very often guide our perceptions, consequently, when we believe something, we tend to see it even if it is not there. Have you heard the saying, “seeing is believing”? Well, the truth is just the opposite: believing is seeing. This problem may just be a result of cognitive ‘blinders’ or it may be part of a more conscious attempt to support our own views. Confirmation bias is the tendency to look for evidence that we are right and in so doing, we ignore contradictory evidence.

Philosopher Karl Popper suggested that the distinction between that which is scientific and that which is unscientific is that science is falsifiable; scientific inquiry involves attempts to reject or refute a theory or set of assumptions (Thornton, 2005). A theory that cannot be falsified is not scientific. And much of what we do in personal inquiry involves drawing conclusions based on what we have personally experienced or validating our own experience by discussing what we think is true with others who share the same views.

Science offers a more systematic way to make comparisons and guard against bias. One technique used to avoid sampling bias is to select participants for a study in a random way. This means using a technique to ensure that all members have an equal chance of being selected. Simple random sampling may involve using a set of random numbers as a guide in determining who is to be selected. For example, if we have a list of 400 people and wish to randomly select a smaller group or sample to be studied, we use a list of random numbers and select the case that corresponds with that number (Case 39, 3, 217, etc.). This is preferable to asking only those individuals with whom we are familiar to participate in a study; if we conveniently chose only people we know, we know nothing about those who had no opportunity to be selected. There are many more elaborate techniques that can be used to obtain samples that represent the composition of the population we are studying. But even though a randomly selected representative sample is preferable, it is not always used because of costs and other limitations. As a consumer of research, however, you should know how the sample was obtained and keep this in mind when interpreting results. It is possible that what was found was limited to that sample or similar individuals and not generalizable to everyone else.

Scientific Methods

The particular method used to conduct research may vary by discipline and since lifespan development is multidisciplinary, more than one method may be used to study human development. One method of scientific investigation involves the following steps:

  • Determining a research question
  • Reviewing previous studies addressing the topic in question (known as a literature review)
  • Determining a method of gathering information
  • Conducting the study
  • Interpreting the results
  • Drawing conclusions; stating limitations of the study and suggestions for future research
  • Making the findings available to others (both to share information and to have the work scrutinized by others)

The findings of these scientific studies can then be used by others as they explore the area of interest. Through this process, a literature or knowledge base is established. This model of scientific investigation presents research as a linear process guided by a specific research question. And it typically involves quantitative research , which relies on numerical data or using statistics to understand and report what has been studied.

Another model of research, referred to as qualitative research, may involve steps such as these:

  • Begin with a broad area of interest and a research question
  • Gain entrance into a group to be researched
  • Gather field notes about the setting, the people, the structure, the activities or other areas of interest
  • Ask open-ended, broad “grand tour” types of questions when interviewing subjects
  • Modify research questions as the study continues
  • Note patterns or consistencies
  • Explore new areas deemed important by the people being observed
  • Report findings

In this type of research, theoretical ideas are “grounded” in the experiences of the participants. The researcher is the student and the people in the setting are the teachers as they inform the researcher of their world (Glazer & Strauss, 1967). Researchers should be aware of their own biases and assumptions, acknowledge them and bracket them in efforts to keep them from limiting accuracy in reporting. Sometimes qualitative studies are used initially to explore a topic and more quantitative studies are used to test or explain what was first described.

A good way to become more familiar with these scientific research methods, both quantitative and qualitative, is to look at journal articles, which are written in sections that follow these steps in the scientific process. Most psychological articles and many papers in the social sciences follow the writing guidelines and format dictated by the American Psychological Association (APA). In general, the structure follows: abstract (summary of the article), introduction or literature review, methods explaining how the study was conducted, results of the study, discussion and interpretation of findings, and references.

Link to Learning

Brené Brown is a bestselling author and social work professor at the University of Houston. She conducts grounded theory research by collecting qualitative data from large numbers of participants. In Brené Brown’s TED Talk The Power of Vulnerability , Brown refers to herself as a storyteller-researcher as she explains her research process and summarizes her results.

https://assessments.lumenlearning.co...essments/16500

Research Methods and Objectives

The main categories of psychological research are descriptive, correlational, and experimental research. Research studies that do not test specific relationships between variables are called descriptive, or qualitative, studies . These studies are used to describe general or specific behaviors and attributes that are observed and measured. In the early stages of research it might be difficult to form a hypothesis, especially when there is not any existing literature in the area. In these situations designing an experiment would be premature, as the question of interest is not yet clearly defined as a hypothesis. Often a researcher will begin with a non-experimental approach, such as a descriptive study, to gather more information about the topic before designing an experiment or correlational study to address a specific hypothesis. Some examples of descriptive questions include:

  • “How much time do parents spend with children?”
  • “How many times per week do couples have intercourse?”
  • “When is marital satisfaction greatest?”

The main types of descriptive studies include observation, case studies, surveys, and content analysis (which we’ll examine further in the module). Descriptive research is distinct from correlational research , in which psychologists formally test whether a relationship exists between two or more variables. Experimental research goes a step further beyond descriptive and correlational research and randomly assigns people to different conditions, using hypothesis testing to make inferences about how these conditions affect behavior. Some experimental research includes explanatory studies, which are efforts to answer the question “why” such as:

  • “Why have rates of divorce leveled off?”
  • “Why are teen pregnancy rates down?”
  • “Why has the average life expectancy increased?”

Evaluation research is designed to assess the effectiveness of policies or programs. For instance, research might be designed to study the effectiveness of safety programs implemented in schools for installing car seats or fitting bicycle helmets. Do children who have been exposed to the safety programs wear their helmets? Do parents use car seats properly? If not, why not?

This Crash Course video provides a brief overview of psychological research, which we’ll cover in more detail on the coming pages.

Thumbnail for the embedded element "Psychological Research: Crash Course Psychology #2"

A YouTube element has been excluded from this version of the text. You can view it online here: http://pb.libretexts.org/lsdm/?p=62

You can view the transcript for “Psychological Research: Crash Course Psychology #2” here (opens in new window) .

https://assessments.lumenlearning.co...essments/16501

[glossary-page] [glossary-term]correlational research:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]research that formally tests whether a relationship exists between two or more variables, however, correlation does not imply causation[/glossary-definition]

[glossary-term]descriptive studies:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]research focused on describing an occurrence[/glossary-definition]

[glossary-term]evaluation research:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]research designed to assess the effectiveness of policies or programs[/glossary-definition]

[glossary-term]experimental research:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]research that involves randomly assigning people to different conditions and using hypothesis testing to make inferences about how these conditions affect behavior; the only method that measures cause and effect between variables[/glossary-definition]

[glossary-term]explanatory studies:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]research that tries to answer the question “why”[/glossary-definition]

[glossary-term]qualitative research:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]theoretical ideas are “grounded” in the experiences of the participants, who answer open-ended questions[/glossary-definition]

[glossary-term]quantitative research:[/glossary-term] [glossary-definition]involves numerical data that are quantified using statistics to understand and report what has been studied[/glossary-definition] [/glossary-page]

Contributors and Attributions

  • Psyc 200 Lifespan Psychology. Authored by : Laura Overstreet. Located at : http://opencourselibrary.org/econ-201/ . License : CC BY: Attribution
  • question mark. Authored by : Alexas_Fotos. Located at : https://pixabay.com/en/question-mark-tissue-structure-1098294/ . License : CC0: No Rights Reserved
  • Descriptive Research. Provided by : Lumen Learning. Located at : https://courses.lumenlearning.com/waymaker-psychology/chapter/reading-clinical-or-case-studies/ . License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Psychological Research: Crash Course Psychology #2. Provided by : CrashCourse. Located at : https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hFV71QPvX2I . License : Other . License Terms : Standard YouTube License

Impact of the circular economy on human development: evidence from Germany

  • Published: 02 May 2024

Cite this article

research study on human development

  • Naila Erum   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-0806-2882 1 ,
  • Kazi Musa   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-6686-0199 1 ,
  • Saira Tufail 2 ,
  • Jamaliah Said   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-1912-2529 1 &
  • Nor Balkish Zakaria   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1795-9854 1  

101 Accesses

2 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

The circular economy has garnered significant consideration due to its ability to contribute to human development. This study presents a comprehensive analysis of the individual effects of four categories of circular economy, namely renewable energy consumption (REC), recycle, reuse, and repair, on human development. For this purpose, we collected time series data of Germany from 1990 to 2021 and applied a dynamic ARDL simulation technique to compile empirical results. The findings show that the REC has positive and significant impact on human development. Reuse and Recycle have an inverse and significant influence on human development in Germany. Whereas human development is neutral to repair. Additionally, the control variables, environmental tax and industrial employment also have negative impacts on human development. Based on the findings, the study suggests that policymakers should design suitable, efficient, and targeted measures to foster the role of each category of circular economy for human development in Germany.

Graphical abstract

We examined the impact of various categories of circular economy on human development in Germany. The findings indicate that REC is positively and significantly associated with human development. The repair has a neutral impact on human development as a category of circular economy. The study also finds that reuse and recycling negatively and significantly affect human development in Germany.

research study on human development

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

research study on human development

Similar content being viewed by others

research study on human development

Exploring the impact of circular economy practices on ecological footprint, inflation rate, and renewable energy consumption: evidence from G20 economies

research study on human development

Asymmetric Impacts of Renewable Energy on Human Development: Exploring the Role of Carbon Emissions, Economic Growth, and Urbanization in European Union Countries

research study on human development

The dynamic analysis of renewable energy’s contribution to the dimensions of sustainable development and energy security

Data availability.

We used secondary data for this study. If needed, we can provide data and materials used in the analysis.

Abbas, J., Al-Sulaiti, K., Lorente, D. B., Shah, S. A. R., & Shahzad, U. Reset the industry redux through corporate social responsibility: The COVID-19 tourism impact on hospitality firms through business model innovation. In Economic Growth and Environmental Quality in a Post-Pandemic World (pp. 177–201). Routledge (2022).

Abokersh, M. H., Norouzi, M., Boer, D., Cabeza, L. F., Casa, G., Prieto, C., Jiménez, L., & Valles, M. (2021). A framework for sustainable evaluation of thermal energy storage in circular economy. Renewable Energy, 175 , 686–701.

Adedoyin, F. F., Erum, N., Taşkin, D., & Chebab, D. (2023). Energy policy simulation in times of crisis: Revisiting the impact of renewable and non-renewable energy production on environmental quality in Germany. Energy Reports, 9 , 4749–4762.

Article   Google Scholar  

Alaerts, L., Van Acker, K., Rousseau, S., De Jaeger, S., Moraga, G., Dewulf, J., De Meester, S., Van Passel, S., Compernolle, T., Bachus, K., Vrancken, K., & Eyckmans, J. (2019). Towards a more direct policy feedback in circular economy monitoring via a societal needs perspective. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 149 , 363–371.

Alnajem, M., Mostafa, M. M., & ElMelegy, A. R. (2021). Mapping the first decade of circular economy research: A bibliometric network analysis. Journal of Industrial and Production Engineering, 38 (1), 29–50.

Androniceanu, A., Kinnunen, J., & Georgescu, I. (2021). Circular economy as a strategic option to promote sustainable economic growth and effective human development. Journal of International Studies., 14 (1), 2071–8330.

Baars, J., Domenech, T., Bleischwitz, R., Melin, H. E., & Heidrich, O. (2021). Circular economy strategies for electric vehicle batteries reduce reliance on raw materials. Nature Sustainability, 4 (1), 71–79.

Balsalobre-Lorente, D., Abbas, J., He, C., Pilař, L., & Shah, S. A. R. (2023). Tourism, urbanization and natural resources rents matter for environmental sustainability: The leading role of AI and ICT on sustainable development goals in the digital era. Resources Policy, 82 , 103445.

Beccarello, M., & Di Foggia, G. (2018). Moving towards a circular economy: Economic impacts of higher material recycling targets. Materials Today Proceedings, 5 (1), 531–543.

Ben Mbarek, M., Saidi, K., & Rahman, M. M. (2018). Renewable and non-renewable energy consumption, environmental degradation and economic growth in Tunisia. Quality & Quantity, 52 , 1105–1119.

Brando, P., Macedo, M., Silvério, D., Rattis, L., Paolucci, L., Alencar, A., Coe, M., & Amorim, C. (2020). Amazon wildfires: Scenes from a foreseeable disaster. Flora, 268 , 151609.

Bracquene, E., Peeters, J. R., Burez, J., De Schepper, K., Duflou, J. R., & Dewulf, W. (2019). Repairability evaluation for energy-related products. Procedia CIRP, 80 , 536–541.

Chang, Q., & Goplerud, M. (2024). Generalized Kernel Regularized Least Squares. Political Analysis, 32 (2), 157–171.

Chauhan, A., Jakhar, S. K., & Chauhan, C. (2021). The interplay of circular economy with industry 4.0 enabled smart city drivers of healthcare waste disposal. Journal of Cleaner Production., 10 (279), 123854.

Chioatto, E., & Sospiro, P. (2023). Transition from waste management to circular economy: The European Union roadmap. Environment, Development and Sustainability, 25 (1), 249–276.

Chui, K. T., Lytras, M. D., & Visvizi, A. (2018). Energy sustainability in smart cities: Artificial intelligence, smart monitoring, and optimization of energy consumption. Energies 11, 2869. Energy, 158 , 1080–1091.

Google Scholar  

Corona, B., Shen, L., Reike, D., Carreón, J. R., & Worrell, E. (2019). Towards sustainable development through the circular economy—A review and critical assessment on current circularity metrics. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 151 , 104498.

Corvellec, H., Stowell, A. F., & Johansson, N. (2022). Critiques of the circular economy. Journal of Industrial Ecology, 26 (2), 421–432.

Daú, G., Scavarda, A., Scavarda, L. F., & Portugal, V. J. T. (2019). The healthcare sustainable supply chain 4.0: The circular economy transition conceptual framework with the corporate social responsibility mirror. Sustainability, 11 (12), 3259.

Deloitte (2016), Study on Socioeconomic Impacts of Increased Reparability of Increased Reparability: Final Report , Prepared for the European Commission, DG ENV. European Commission, Brussels.

Desing, H., Brunner, D., Takacs, F., Nahrath, S., Frankenberger, K., & Hischier, R. (2020). A circular economy within the planetary boundaries: Towards a resource-based, systemic approach. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 155 , 104673.

D'Urzo, M., & Campagnaro, C. (2023). Design-led repair & reuse: An approach for an equitable, bottom-up, innovation-driven circular economy. Journal of Cleaner Production, 387 , 135724.

Geissdoerfer, M., Savaget, P., Bocken, N. M., & Hultink, E. J. (2017). The Circular Economy–A new sustainability paradigm?. Journal of Cleaner Production, 143 , 757–768.

Hao, Y., Wang, Y., Wu, Q., Sun, S., Wang, W., & Cui, M. (2020). What affects residents’ participation in the circular economy for sustainable development? Evidence from China. Sustainable Development, 28 (5), 1251–1268.

Horbach, J., & Rammer, C. (2020). Circular economy innovations, growth and employment at the firm level: Empirical evidence from Germany. Journal of Industrial Ecology, 24 (3), 615–625.

International Energy Agency (2020). Germany 2020 - Energy Policy Review. Accessed from https://iea.blob.core.windows.net/assets/60434f12-7891-4469-b3e4-1e82ff898212/Germany_2020_Energy_Policy_Review.pdf . Accessed on 26-04-2024.

Islam, K. N., Sarker, T., Taghizadeh-Hesary, F., Atri, A. C., & Alam, M. S. (2021). Renewable energy generation from livestock waste for a sustainable circular economy in Bangladesh. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews, 139 , 110695.

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Jaeger-Erben, M., Frick, V., & Hipp, T. (2021). Why do users (not) repair their devices? A study of the predictors of repair practices. Journal of Cleaner Production., 286 , 125382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.125382

Jang, Y. C., Lee, G., Kwon, Y., Lim, J. H., & Jeong, J. H. (2020). Recycling and management practices of plastic packaging waste towards a circular economy in South Korea. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 158 , 104798.

Jordan, S., & Philips, A. Q. (2018). Cointegration testing and dynamic simulations of autoregressive distributed lag models. Symposium (international) on Combustion, 18 (4), 902–923.

Kazancoglu, I., Sagnak, M., Kumar Mangla, S., & Kazancoglu, Y. (2021). Circular economy and the policy: A framework for improving the corporate environmental management in supply chains. Business Strategy and the Environment, 30 (1), 590–608.

Khajuria, A., Atienza, V. A., Chavanich, S., Henning, W., Islam, I., Kral, U., & Li, J. (2022). Accelerating circular economy solutions to achieve the 2030 agenda for sustainable development goals. Circular Economy, 1 (1), 100001.

Kirchherr, J., Reike, D., & Hekkert, M. (2017). Conceptualizing the circular economy: An analysis of 114 definitions. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 127 , 221–232.

Knäble, D., de Quevedo Puente, E., Pérez-Cornejo, C., & Baumgärtler, T. (2022). The impact of the circular economy on sustainable development: A European panel data approach. Sustainable Production and Consumption, 34 , 233–243.

Laitala, K., Klepp, I. G., Haugrønning, V., Throne-Holst, H., & Strandbakken, P. (2021). Increasing repair of household appliances, mobile phones and clothing: Experiences from consumers and the repair industry. Journal of Cleaner Production., 282 , 125349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.125349

Lederer, J., Gassner, A., Kleemann, F., & Fellner, J. (2020). Potentials for a circular economy of mineral construction materials and demolition waste in urban areas: A case study from Vienna. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 161 , 104942.

Lee, J. W. (2019). Long-run dynamics of renewable energy consumption on carbon emissions and economic growth in the European Union. International Journal of Sustainable Development & World Ecology, 26 (1), 69–78.

Llorente-González, L. J., & Vence, X. (2020). How labour-intensive is the circular economy? A policy-orientated structural analysis of the repair, reuse and recycling activities in the European Union. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 162 , 105033.

Luttenberger, L. R. (2020). Waste management challenges in transition to circular economy–case of Croatia. Journal of Cleaner Production, 256 , 120495.

Marrucci, L., Daddi, T., & Iraldo, F. (2021). The contribution of green human resource management to the circular economy and performance of environmental certified organisations. Journal of Cleaner Production, 319 , 128859.

Maslyuk, S. Energy Consumption Transition and Human Development. In  Institutions, Efficiency and Evolving Energy Technologies, 34th IAEE International Conference, June 19–23, 2011 . International Association for Energy Economics. (2011)

Mavropoulos, A., & Nilsen, A. W. (2020). Industry 40 and circular economy: Towards a wasteless future or a wasteful planet? John Wiley & Sons.

Mauss, N. A., Bühner, D., & Fottner, J. (2023). Applicability and limitations of change management for circular economy in manufacturing companies. Procedia Computer Science, 217 , 998–1007.

McLaren, D., Niskanen, J., & Anshelm, J. (2020). Reconfiguring repair: Contested politics and values of repair challenge instrumental discourses found in circular economies literature. Resources Conservation Recycling X, 8 , 100046.

McVeigh, M. K., Dalhammar, C., & Richter, J. L. Planned obsolescence–Built not to last. Stockholm: European Liberal Forum and Fores. (2019).

Mendonça, A.Kd.S., de Andrade Conradi Barni, G., Moro, M.F., Bornia, A.C., Kupek, E., Fernandes, L., 2020. Hierarchical modeling of the 50 largest economies to verify the

Micah, A. E., Bhangdia, K., Cogswell, I. E., Lasher, D., Lidral-Porter, B., Maddison, E. R., & Hlongwa, M. M. (2023). Global investments in pandemic preparedness and COVID-19: development assistance and domestic spending on health between 1990 and 2026. The Lancet Global Health, 11 (3), e385–e413.

Mies, A., & Gold, S. (2021). Mapping the social dimension of the circular economy. Journal of Cleaner Production, 321 , 128960.

Moraga, G., Huysveld, S., Mathieux, F., Blengini, G. A., Alaerts, L., Van Acker, K., & Dewulf, J. (2019). Circular economy indicators: What do they measure? Resources Conservation and Recycling, 146 , 452–461.

Morseletto, P. (2020). Targets for a circular economy. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 153 , 104553.

Mukoro, V., Gallego-Schmid, A., & Sharmina, M. (2021). Life cycle assessment of renewable energy in Africa. Sustainable Production and Consumption, 28 , 1314–1332.

Murray, A., Skene, K., & Haynes, K. (2017). The circular economy: An interdisciplinary exploration of the concept and application in a global context. Journal of Business Ethics, 140 , 369–380.

Neligan, A., Baumgartner, R. J., Geissdoerfer, M., & Schöggl, J. P. (2023). Circular disruption: Digitalisation as a driver of circular economy business models. Business Strategy and the Environment, 32 (3), 1175–1188.

Nelson, A., & Neguriță, O. (2020). Big data-driven smart cities: Internet of things devices and environmentally sustainable urban development. Geopolitics History and International Relations, 12 (2), 37–43. https://doi.org/10.22381/GHIR12220205

Neumayer, E. (2016). Human development and sustainability. The Capability Approach and Sustainability , 154–172.

Neumann, J., Petranikova, M., Meeus, M., Gamarra, J. D., Younesi, R., Winter, M., & Nowak, S. (2022). Recycling of lithium-ion batteries—current state of the art, circular economy, and next generation recycling. Advanced Energy Materials, 12 (17), 2102917.

NY State Senate, Bill S149 Establishes the right to repair act. [Online]. Available https://www.nysenate.gov/legislation/bills/2021/s149 . Accessed 01 Oct 2022 (2020)

Ofori, D., & Opoku Mensah, A. (2022). Sustainable electronic waste management among households: A circular economy perspective from a developing economy. Management of Environmental Quality: An International Journal, 33 (1), 64–85.

Ogunmakinde, O. E., Egbelakin, T., & Sher, W. (2022). Contributions of the circular economy to the UN sustainable development goals through sustainable construction. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 178 , 106023.

Ouedraogo, N. S. (2013). Energy consumption and human development: Evidence from a panel cointegration and error correction model. Energy, 63 , 28–41.

Pahunang, R. R., Buonerba, A., Senatore, V., Oliva, G., Ouda, M., Zarra, T., & Naddeo, V. (2021). Advances in technological control of greenhouse gas emissions from wastewater in the context of circular economy. Science of the Total Environment, 792 , 148479.

Pajunen, N., & Holuszko, M. E. (2022). Circular economy in electronics and the future of e-waste. Electronic Waste: Recycling and Reprocessing for a Sustainable Future., 10 , 299–314.

Pesaran, M. H., Shin, Y., & Smith, R. J. (2001). Bounds testing approaches to the analysis of level relationships. Journal of Applied Econometrics, 16 (3), 289–326.

Puntillo, P. (2023). Circular economy business models: Towards achieving sustainable development goals in the waste management sector—Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Corporate Social Responsibility and Environmental Management, 30 (2), 941–954.

Reike, D., Vermeulen, W. J., & Witjes, S. (2018). The circular economy: New or refurbished as CE 3.0?—exploring controversies in the conceptualization of the circular economy through a focus on history and resource value retention options. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 135 , 246–264.

Sabbaghi, M., Cade, W., Behdad, S., & Bisantz, A. M. (2017). The current status of the consumer electronics repair industry in the US: A survey-based study. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 116 , 137–151.

Sanchez-Loor, D. A., & Zambrano-Monserrate, M. A. (2015). Causality analysis between electricity consumption, real GDP, foreign direct investment, human development and remittances in Colombia, Ecuador and Mexico. International Journal of Energy Economics and Policy, 5 (3), 746–753.

Sarkodie, S. A., & Owusu, P. A. (2020). How to apply the novel dynamic ARDL simulations (dynardl) and Kernel-based regularized least squares (krls). MethodsX, 7 , 101160.

Sasmaz, M. U., Sakar, E., Yayla, Y. E., & Akkucuk, U. (2020). The relationship between renewable energy and human development in OECD countries: A panel data analysis. Sustainability, 12 (18), 7450.

Schäfer, P., & Schmidt, M. (2021). Model-based analysis of the limits of recycling for its contribution to climate change mitigation. Nachhaltigkeits Management Forum, 29 (2), 65–75.

Schröder, P., Lemille, A., & Desmond, P. (2020). Making the circular economy work for human development. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 156 , 104686.

Scotland, Z. W. (2015). Circular economy evidence building programme: Remanufacturing study.

Shah, S. A. R., Zhang, Q., Abbas, J., Tang, H., & Al-Sulaiti, K. I. (2023). Waste management, quality of life and natural resources utilization matter for renewable electricity generation: The main and moderate role of environmental policy. Utilities Policy, 82 , 101584.

Shamsuyeva, M., & Endres, H. J. (2021). Plastics in the context of the circular economy and sustainable plastics recycling: Comprehensive review on research development, standardization and market. Composites Part C Open Access, 6 , 100168.

Shanmugam, V., Das, O., Neisiany, R. E., Babu, K., Singh, S., Hedenqvist, M. S., & Ramakrishna, S. (2020). Polymer recycling in additive manufacturing: An opportunity for the circular economy. Materials Circular Economy, 2 , 1–11.

Sharma, N. K., Govindan, K., Lai, K. K., Chen, W. K., & Kumar, V. (2021). The transition from linear economy to circular economy for sustainability among SMEs: A study on prospects, impediments, and prerequisites. Business Strategy and the Environment, 30 (4), 1803–1822.

Shishegar, S., Duchesne, S., Pelletier, G., & Ghorbani, R. (2021). A smart predictive framework for system-level storm water management optimization. Journal of Environmental Management, 278 , 111505.

Silva, V. U., Nascimento, M. F., Oliveira, P. R., Panzera, T. H., Rezende, M. O., Silva, D. A. L., & Christoforo, A. L. (2021). Circular vs. linear economy of building materials: A case study for particleboards made of recycled wood and biopolymer vs. conventional particleboards. Construction and Building Materials, 285 , 122906.

Simoens, M. C., & Leipold, S. (2021). Trading radical for incremental change: The politics of a circular economy transition in the German packaging sector. Journal of Environmental Policy Planning, 23 (6), 822–836.

Sinha, E. (2022). Circular economy—A way forward to sustainable development: Identifying conceptual overlaps and contingency factors at the microlevel. Sustainable Development, 30 (4), 771–783.

Spencer, G., Corbin, J. H., & Miedema, E. (2019). Sustainable development goals for health promotion: A critical frame analysis. Health Promotion International, 34 (4), 847–858.

Steffen, W., Richardson, K., Rockström, J., Cornell, S. E., Fetzer, I., Bennett, E. M., Biggs, R., Carpenter, S. R., De Vries, W., De Wit, C. A., & Folke, C. (2015). Planetary boundaries: Guiding human development on a changing planet. Science, 347 (6223), 1259855. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1259855

Strielkowski, W., Streimikiene, D., Fomina, A., & Semenova, E. (2019). Internet of energy (IoE) and high-renewables electricity system market design. Energies, 12 (24), 4790.

Svensson-Hoglund, S., Russell, J. D., & Richter, J. L. (2023). A process approach to product repair from the perspective of the individual. Circular Economy and Sustainability., 3 (3), 1327–1359.

Sverko Grdic, Z., Krstinic Nizic, M., & Rudan, E. (2020). Circular economy concept in the context of economic development in EU countries. Sustainability, 12 (7), 3060.

Ul-Durar, S., Awan, U., Varma, A., Memon, S., & Mention, A. L. (2023). Integrating knowledge management and orientation dynamics for organization transition from eco-innovation to circular economy. Journal of Knowledge Management., 27 (8), 2217–2248.

Valencia, A., Zhang, W., & Chang, N. B. (2022). Sustainability transitions of urban food-energy-water-waste infrastructure: A living laboratory approach for circular economy. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 177 , 105991.

Velasco-Muñoz, J. F., Mendoza, J. M. F., Aznar-Sánchez, J. A., & Gallego-Schmid, A. (2021). Circular economy implementation in the agricultural sector: Definition, strategies and indicators. Resources Conservation and Recycling, 170 , 105618.

Wang, S., Abbas, J., Al-Sulati, K. I., & Shah, S. A. (2024b). The impact of economic corridor and tourism on local community’s quality of life under one belt one road context. Evaluation Review, 48 (2), 312–345.

Wang, X., Li, Y., Liu, N., & Zhang, Y. (2023a). An urban material flow analysis framework and measurement method from the perspective of urban metabolism. Journal of Cleaner Production, 257 , 120564.

Wu, C., Chen, B., Huang, X., & Wei, Y. D. (2020). Effect of land-use change and optimization on the ecosystem service values of Jiangsu province. China. Ecological Indicators, 117 , 106507.

Wuni, I. Y. (2023). A systematic review of the critical success factors for implementing circular economy in construction projects. Sustainable Development, 31 (3), 1195–1213.

Yang, Y., Pilanci, M., & Wainwright, M. J. (2017). Randomized sketches for kernels: Fast and optimal nonparametric regression.

Zhang, X., Husnain, M., Yang, H., Ullah, S., Abbas, J., & Zhang, R. (2022). Corporate business strategy and tax avoidance culture: Moderating role of gender diversity in an emerging economy. Frontiers in Psychology, 13 , 827553.

Zhang, X., & Liu, Y. (2022). Circular economy is game-changing municipal wastewater treatment technology towards energy and carbon neutrality. Chemical Engineering Journal, 429 , 132114.

Zhuang, G. L., Shih, S. G., & Wagiri, F. (2023). Circular economy and sustainable development goals: Exploring the potentials of reusable modular components in circular economy business model. Journal of Cleaner Production, 414 , 137503.

Zink, T., & Geyer, R. (2017). Circular economy rebound. Journal of Industrial Ecology, 21 (3), 593–602.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to the Accounting Research Institute (ARI- HICoE), Universiti Teknologi MARA, Shah Alam, Malaysia, and the Ministry of Higher Education for providing research funding.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Accounting Research Institute (HICoE), University Teknologi MARA (UiTM), Shah Alam, Selangor, Malaysia

Naila Erum, Kazi Musa, Jamaliah Said & Nor Balkish Zakaria

Department of Economics, Fatima Jinnah Women University, Rawalpindi, Punjab, Pakistan

Saira Tufail

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

ST has performed empirical analysis. KM wrote a literature review. NE worked on the introduction and conclusion. NBZ worked on results and discussions. JS reviewed the manuscript and provided fruitful comments.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Naila Erum .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The corresponding author confirms on behalf of all authors that we do not have any conflicting interests upon publication of this paper.

Consent for publication

On behalf of all authors, I provide our support for the publication of identifiable details in this Journal. This consent is provided after discussion with co-authors, including ST, KM, JS and NBZ.

Ethical approval

The author(s) adhered to the accepted moral standards of a genuine research study.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Erum, N., Musa, K., Tufail, S. et al. Impact of the circular economy on human development: evidence from Germany. Environ Dev Sustain (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10668-024-04918-6

Download citation

Received : 16 September 2023

Accepted : 11 April 2024

Published : 02 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10668-024-04918-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Renewable energy consumption
  • Dynamic ARDL
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. Stages of Human Development: What It Is & Why It’s Important (2024)

    research study on human development

  2. Human Growth And Development Example Essay Example

    research study on human development

  3. PPT

    research study on human development

  4. Textbook of Human Development By S.C. Srivastava

    research study on human development

  5. Stages of Human Development: What It Is & Why It’s Important (2024)

    research study on human development

  6. (PDF) PSYCHOLOGY OF HUMAN DEVELOPMENT

    research study on human development

VIDEO

  1. Difference between Growth and Development

  2. General Principles of Human Development

  3. Creating a healthier future through human development

  4. Discoveries from the Lab Part 2: Human Studies

  5. What is Human Development Throughout The Lifespan?

  6. Theory and Research in Human Development: Developmental Psychology

COMMENTS

  1. The evolution of our understanding of human development over the last 10 years

    Abstract. As it fulfills an irresistible need to understand our own origins, research on human development occupies a unique niche in scientific and medical research. In this Comment, we explore the progress in our understanding of human development over the past 10 years. The focus is on basic research, clinical applications, and ethical ...

  2. Research in Human Development

    Editorial Scope. Research in Human Development ( RHD) promotes conceptual, empirical, and methodological integrative and interdisciplinary approaches to the study of human development across the entire life span. The journal emphasizes theory and research concerning person-context relationships across the life course, and the employment of a variety of quantitative and qualitative methods (e.g ...

  3. The evolution of our understanding of human development over ...

    As it fulfills an irresistible need to understand our own origins, research on human development occupies a unique niche in scientific and medical research. In this Comment, we explore the ...

  4. The Biosocial Approach to Human Development, Behavior, and Health

    The biosocial approach also underscores the importance of the life course, as assessments of both biological and social features throughout human development over time, and across generations, are needed to achieve a full understanding of social and physical well-being. We conclude with a brief review of the papers in the volume, which showcase ...

  5. Human Development & Life Course Studies

    Human Development and Life Course Studies research covers individual development across the lifespan, placing human development within social, cultural, and historical context. With the abundance of panel data sets within SRC, this research theme helps bring theoretical and applied focus to such studies. ... Research in Human Development 12 (3 ...

  6. The Human Development Approach: An Overview: Oxford Development Studies

    The human development (HD) approach puts the improvement of people's lives as the central objective of development. This paper provides an overview of major aspects of the approach. It shows how it emerged with the evolution of development thought and a widening of development objectives The paper explores the two-way relationship between HD ...

  7. Developmental Psychology

    Although most articles in this journal address human development, studies of other species are appropriate if they have important implications for human development. ... approaches and thinking to a range of questions about emotional development and to inspire new directions for future research. Identity Development Process and Content: Special ...

  8. Research in Human Development: Vol 20, No 3-4 (Current issue)

    Zena R. Mello et al. Article | Published online: 26 Sep 2023. COLORISM BEFORE AND AFTER THE ONE DROP RULE. Robert L. Reece. Article | Published online: 8 Oct 2023. View all latest articles. Explore the current issue of Research in Human Development, Volume 20, Issue 3-4, 2023.

  9. Developmental Psychology Studies Human Development Across the Lifespan

    Developmental psychologists study human growth and development over the lifespan, including physical, cognitive, social, intellectual, perceptual, personality and emotional growth. Developmental psychologists working in colleges and universities tend to focus primarily on research or teaching. Others working in more applied settings like health ...

  10. PDF THE STUDY OF HUMAN DEVELOPMENT

    well-known mathematician and writer, defines science as "the human activity of finding an order in nature by organizing the scattered meaningless facts under universal concepts" (p. 225). Science is the process through which we organize bits of information. This process lends meaning and significance to otherwise The Study of Human Development5

  11. Over nearly 80 years, Harvard study has been showing how to live a

    Lara Tang '18, a human and evolutionary biology concentrator who recently joined the team as a research assistant, relishes the opportunity to help find some of those answers. She joined the effort after coming across Waldinger's TED talk in one of her classes. "That motivated me to do more research on adult development," said Tang.

  12. Journal

    Research in Human Development Editors: Jennifer Brown Urban and Miriam Linver. Research in Human Development (RHD) is the flagship journal of SSHD. RHD is a lifespan, interdisciplinary, and integrative journal which focuses on thematic special issues. To view the most recent table of contents, click here.To access exclusive content of RHD, first Log In and then go to Members Access.

  13. PDF Research Methods in Human Development

    Rev. ed. of: Research methods in human development / Paul C. Cozby, PatriciaE. Worden, Daniel W Kee. 1989. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 1-55934-875-5 1. Social sciences-Methodology. I. Brown, Kathleen W II. Cozby, Paul C. Research methods in human development. H61.R4657 1998 300'.72-DC21 98-16053 CIP

  14. 1.11: Developmental Research Designs

    Longitudinal research designs Figure 2. Longitudinal research studies the same person or group of people over an extended period of time. Longitudinal research involves beginning with a group of people who may be of the same age and background (cohort) and measuring them repeatedly over a long period of time. One of the benefits of this type of research is that people can be followed through ...

  15. (PDF) Human Development, Theories of

    relations and on the integration of multiple attributes of the. individual (e.g., physiological, cognitive, emotional, motiva-. tional, and behavioral characteristics) and multiple levels of. the ...

  16. Research in Developmental Psychology

    Lifespan development is a fascinating field of study - but care must be taken to ensure that researchers use appropriate methods to examine human behavior, use the correct experimental design to answer their questions, and be aware of the special challenges that are part-and-parcel of developmental research.

  17. The Study of Human Development

    The scientific study of human development began with studies of childhood during the nineteenth century. Adolescence was not considered a separate phase of development until the twentieth century, when scientific interest in aging also began. ... Developmental research has important applications. Developmental scientists study developmental ...

  18. List of issues Research in Human Development

    Browse the list of issues and latest articles from Research in Human Development. All issues. Special issues. Latest articles. Volume 20 2023. Volume 19 2022. Volume 18 2021. Volume 17 2020. Volume 16 2019.

  19. 1.8: Research in Lifespan Development

    Scientific Methods. The particular method used to conduct research may vary by discipline and since lifespan development is multidisciplinary, more than one method may be used to study human development. One method of scientific investigation involves the following steps: Determining a research question.

  20. (PDF) Human Growth and Development

    Chapter 20. Human Growth and Development. Barry Bogin. Centre for Global Health & Human Development, School of Sport, Exercise & Health Sciences, Loughborough Univ ersity, Loughborough, UK ...

  21. Impact of the circular economy on human development: evidence from

    The circular economy has garnered significant consideration due to its ability to contribute to human development. This study presents a comprehensive analysis of the individual effects of four categories of circular economy, namely renewable energy consumption (REC), recycle, reuse, and repair, on human development. For this purpose, we collected time series data of Germany from 1990 to 2021 ...

  22. AHRQ Seeks Examples of Impact for Development of Impact Case Studies

    Since 2004, the agency has developed more than 400 Impact Case Studies that illustrate AHRQ's contributions to healthcare improvement. Available online and searchable via an interactive map , the Impact Case Studies help to tell the story of how AHRQ-funded research findings, data and tools have made an impact on the lives of millions of ...

  23. Using generative AI in human resource development: an applied research

    Our goal was to identify the benefits and drawbacks of using this technology in HRD practice. The theoretical framework for the study was provided by the Activity Theory, an often used approach in Human-Computer Interaction research. The study participants were HRD doctoral students from two large public universities in the USA.