• Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2023 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Problem-Solving Strategies and Obstacles

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

Sean is a fact-checker and researcher with experience in sociology, field research, and data analytics.

a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

JGI / Jamie Grill / Getty Images

  • Application
  • Improvement

From deciding what to eat for dinner to considering whether it's the right time to buy a house, problem-solving is a large part of our daily lives. Learn some of the problem-solving strategies that exist and how to use them in real life, along with ways to overcome obstacles that are making it harder to resolve the issues you face.

What Is Problem-Solving?

In cognitive psychology , the term 'problem-solving' refers to the mental process that people go through to discover, analyze, and solve problems.

A problem exists when there is a goal that we want to achieve but the process by which we will achieve it is not obvious to us. Put another way, there is something that we want to occur in our life, yet we are not immediately certain how to make it happen.

Maybe you want a better relationship with your spouse or another family member but you're not sure how to improve it. Or you want to start a business but are unsure what steps to take. Problem-solving helps you figure out how to achieve these desires.

The problem-solving process involves:

  • Discovery of the problem
  • Deciding to tackle the issue
  • Seeking to understand the problem more fully
  • Researching available options or solutions
  • Taking action to resolve the issue

Before problem-solving can occur, it is important to first understand the exact nature of the problem itself. If your understanding of the issue is faulty, your attempts to resolve it will also be incorrect or flawed.

Problem-Solving Mental Processes

Several mental processes are at work during problem-solving. Among them are:

  • Perceptually recognizing the problem
  • Representing the problem in memory
  • Considering relevant information that applies to the problem
  • Identifying different aspects of the problem
  • Labeling and describing the problem

Problem-Solving Strategies

There are many ways to go about solving a problem. Some of these strategies might be used on their own, or you may decide to employ multiple approaches when working to figure out and fix a problem.

An algorithm is a step-by-step procedure that, by following certain "rules" produces a solution. Algorithms are commonly used in mathematics to solve division or multiplication problems. But they can be used in other fields as well.

In psychology, algorithms can be used to help identify individuals with a greater risk of mental health issues. For instance, research suggests that certain algorithms might help us recognize children with an elevated risk of suicide or self-harm.

One benefit of algorithms is that they guarantee an accurate answer. However, they aren't always the best approach to problem-solving, in part because detecting patterns can be incredibly time-consuming.

There are also concerns when machine learning is involved—also known as artificial intelligence (AI)—such as whether they can accurately predict human behaviors.

Heuristics are shortcut strategies that people can use to solve a problem at hand. These "rule of thumb" approaches allow you to simplify complex problems, reducing the total number of possible solutions to a more manageable set.

If you find yourself sitting in a traffic jam, for example, you may quickly consider other routes, taking one to get moving once again. When shopping for a new car, you might think back to a prior experience when negotiating got you a lower price, then employ the same tactics.

While heuristics may be helpful when facing smaller issues, major decisions shouldn't necessarily be made using a shortcut approach. Heuristics also don't guarantee an effective solution, such as when trying to drive around a traffic jam only to find yourself on an equally crowded route.

Trial and Error

A trial-and-error approach to problem-solving involves trying a number of potential solutions to a particular issue, then ruling out those that do not work. If you're not sure whether to buy a shirt in blue or green, for instance, you may try on each before deciding which one to purchase.

This can be a good strategy to use if you have a limited number of solutions available. But if there are many different choices available, narrowing down the possible options using another problem-solving technique can be helpful before attempting trial and error.

In some cases, the solution to a problem can appear as a sudden insight. You are facing an issue in a relationship or your career when, out of nowhere, the solution appears in your mind and you know exactly what to do.

Insight can occur when the problem in front of you is similar to an issue that you've dealt with in the past. Although, you may not recognize what is occurring since the underlying mental processes that lead to insight often happen outside of conscious awareness .

Research indicates that insight is most likely to occur during times when you are alone—such as when going on a walk by yourself, when you're in the shower, or when lying in bed after waking up.

How to Apply Problem-Solving Strategies in Real Life

If you're facing a problem, you can implement one or more of these strategies to find a potential solution. Here's how to use them in real life:

  • Create a flow chart . If you have time, you can take advantage of the algorithm approach to problem-solving by sitting down and making a flow chart of each potential solution, its consequences, and what happens next.
  • Recall your past experiences . When a problem needs to be solved fairly quickly, heuristics may be a better approach. Think back to when you faced a similar issue, then use your knowledge and experience to choose the best option possible.
  • Start trying potential solutions . If your options are limited, start trying them one by one to see which solution is best for achieving your desired goal. If a particular solution doesn't work, move on to the next.
  • Take some time alone . Since insight is often achieved when you're alone, carve out time to be by yourself for a while. The answer to your problem may come to you, seemingly out of the blue, if you spend some time away from others.

Obstacles to Problem-Solving

Problem-solving is not a flawless process as there are a number of obstacles that can interfere with our ability to solve a problem quickly and efficiently. These obstacles include:

  • Assumptions: When dealing with a problem, people can make assumptions about the constraints and obstacles that prevent certain solutions. Thus, they may not even try some potential options.
  • Functional fixedness : This term refers to the tendency to view problems only in their customary manner. Functional fixedness prevents people from fully seeing all of the different options that might be available to find a solution.
  • Irrelevant or misleading information: When trying to solve a problem, it's important to distinguish between information that is relevant to the issue and irrelevant data that can lead to faulty solutions. The more complex the problem, the easier it is to focus on misleading or irrelevant information.
  • Mental set: A mental set is a tendency to only use solutions that have worked in the past rather than looking for alternative ideas. A mental set can work as a heuristic, making it a useful problem-solving tool. However, mental sets can also lead to inflexibility, making it more difficult to find effective solutions.

How to Improve Your Problem-Solving Skills

In the end, if your goal is to become a better problem-solver, it's helpful to remember that this is a process. Thus, if you want to improve your problem-solving skills, following these steps can help lead you to your solution:

  • Recognize that a problem exists . If you are facing a problem, there are generally signs. For instance, if you have a mental illness , you may experience excessive fear or sadness, mood changes, and changes in sleeping or eating habits. Recognizing these signs can help you realize that an issue exists.
  • Decide to solve the problem . Make a conscious decision to solve the issue at hand. Commit to yourself that you will go through the steps necessary to find a solution.
  • Seek to fully understand the issue . Analyze the problem you face, looking at it from all sides. If your problem is relationship-related, for instance, ask yourself how the other person may be interpreting the issue. You might also consider how your actions might be contributing to the situation.
  • Research potential options . Using the problem-solving strategies mentioned, research potential solutions. Make a list of options, then consider each one individually. What are some pros and cons of taking the available routes? What would you need to do to make them happen?
  • Take action . Select the best solution possible and take action. Action is one of the steps required for change . So, go through the motions needed to resolve the issue.
  • Try another option, if needed . If the solution you chose didn't work, don't give up. Either go through the problem-solving process again or simply try another option.

You can find a way to solve your problems as long as you keep working toward this goal—even if the best solution is simply to let go because no other good solution exists.

Sarathy V. Real world problem-solving .  Front Hum Neurosci . 2018;12:261. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00261

Dunbar K. Problem solving . A Companion to Cognitive Science . 2017. doi:10.1002/9781405164535.ch20

Stewart SL, Celebre A, Hirdes JP, Poss JW. Risk of suicide and self-harm in kids: The development of an algorithm to identify high-risk individuals within the children's mental health system . Child Psychiat Human Develop . 2020;51:913-924. doi:10.1007/s10578-020-00968-9

Rosenbusch H, Soldner F, Evans AM, Zeelenberg M. Supervised machine learning methods in psychology: A practical introduction with annotated R code . Soc Personal Psychol Compass . 2021;15(2):e12579. doi:10.1111/spc3.12579

Mishra S. Decision-making under risk: Integrating perspectives from biology, economics, and psychology . Personal Soc Psychol Rev . 2014;18(3):280-307. doi:10.1177/1088868314530517

Csikszentmihalyi M, Sawyer K. Creative insight: The social dimension of a solitary moment . In: The Systems Model of Creativity . 2015:73-98. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9085-7_7

Chrysikou EG, Motyka K, Nigro C, Yang SI, Thompson-Schill SL. Functional fixedness in creative thinking tasks depends on stimulus modality .  Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts . 2016;10(4):425‐435. doi:10.1037/aca0000050

Huang F, Tang S, Hu Z. Unconditional perseveration of the short-term mental set in chunk decomposition .  Front Psychol . 2018;9:2568. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02568

National Alliance on Mental Illness. Warning signs and symptoms .

Mayer RE. Thinking, problem solving, cognition, 2nd ed .

Schooler JW, Ohlsson S, Brooks K. Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. J Experiment Psychol: General . 1993;122:166-183. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.2.166

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

  • Client Login

Currie Management Consultants

13 Dimensions of Problem-Solving, a Unique Approach to Conundrums

Feb 3, 2021

This is an excerpt of a larger work “12 Currie Success Principles”

Success Principle #6 is Execution. Problem-solving falls under this category as it requires decisiveness and action.

The first step in solving a problem is to envision a scenario in which the problem does not exist.  Some psychology experts claim that one can only imagine that which one has seen. Others will beg to differ. Our imagination is vast and limitless. To begin this process, it is important that you recognize and accept this as one of the infinite possibilities of the human mind. Access to a vision of what you want something to look like is key. Give yourself permission to be a visionary.

This process of problem solving emerges as 13 dimensions evolving over four levels. But remember, when presented with a problem to solve, we must understand that the solution lies within our reach—emotionally, mentally, and in a manifested, material state.  Below are the dimensions in sequences.  But before you begin this process, whether it is short-term for a small problem or longer-term for a large issue, read through all of the dimensions and ruminate on them for a few days, if possible.  This may not be a luxury that can be afforded for emergencies that need a swiftly executed solution, but once you become familiar with the process, this program becomes a skill set that is forever in your leadership toolbox. It can be accessed for an infinite number of purposes.

Here’s a very abbreviated version of the process, and you are welcomed to call or email if you have questions or want to learn more. Enjoy!

When you are ready to begin the process, you must set a specific, non-negotiable goal. This goal has come from your vision of what would things “look like” when that problem is solved. The goal must be in writing and must be communicated clearly to all team members in order to gain consensus. The goal must be the overarching common goal, which is embraced by all. There have been no strategies set, and no other processes articulated—just the goal. After the goal is solid, begin.

These are in list form, and it is part of a larger work, but the entire program is available at The Currie Training Center and is offered as a group class or individually (to set up a session contact me at robin@curriemanagement.com )

Level One: Identification of the problem and the possible solution(s).

1 – Purpose of the Solution : What is the purpose that the solution you seek will serve? Why do we want to do this? What will change?  What will be better if this happens?

2 – The Looming Adversary : What major challenge could block the way? Assess your team’s tolerance for change. Brainstorm about obstacles, whether real or perceived, and uncover reasons why the problem may be considered as “here to stay.”

3 – Determine the Protocols , and processes that will define your solution. Think of these as service-oriented actions—they should include development of self and the team as individuals. The problem-solving process should include a gain in knowledge, experience, and skills for all the people involved in the project.

4 – Material Form is now clearly communicated to the team. What will the ideal outcome look like? Make sure all team members hold this vision.

Level Two: Commitment to the Solution (or proposed solutions).

5 – Take Ownership of the outcome, or responsibility for the success or failure to solve the problem.

6 – Ensure Engagement of as many perspectives as possible. Ignoring or dismissing another’s perspective should not be allowed on an effective team.

7 – Attune to subtle cues, synchronicities, related case studies, and signs. Listen to anecdotal suggestions. Watch for the “sleeping giants” on your team—draw them out and listen attentively.

8 – Command Integrity: This is a good time to review your progress to ensure that it aligns with your company culture, your own moral compass, and that a truly harmless solution is emerging. Your company may operate under Machiavellian principles, Buddhist teachings, or fall somewhere in between, but remember that integrity of a business is a driving force in customer retention.

Level Three: Action.

9 – Formalize and create the specific steps toward your proposed solution. Create a detailed project map with deadlines and measurement markers.

10 – Address Dimension #2 (Looming Adversary) . Has it materialized? Is it still there? Has the team discovered a different challenge that now must be assessed? Resist the temptation to skip this step, as obstacles cannot be ignored.

11 – Revisit Dimension #3 (Determine the protocols) . Adjust, fine-tune, calibrate, and manipulate. Do what is needed to ensure your process is still in alignment with the goal, and the vision of the problem when it is corrected.

12 – Examine the outcome. The results should be pending now, or perhaps even visible.

Level Four: Execute

13 – It’s Go Time. Ready to launch. Take Flight. Remember to revisit the entire process in order to fine-tune the project. This process can be used in small ways, such as creating an improvement plan for an underperforming employee. Or it can be used as a process to integrate an acquisition. This methodology is flexible and adaptable like a successful and nimble business enterprise should be. When a team can successfully process a problem or create an initiative that bears fruit—that is where you have magic!

Developing your highest quality employees helps them achieve their full potential, perform at a high level, and stay engaged, which improves retention rate. Click here to see the The Currie Training Center Leadership program offerings.

Recent Posts

  • A Different Gap Analysis…
  • Title: Dimensions of Problem Solving: A Unique Approach to Conundrums
  • Think Your Safety Standards Are Up to Par? (Think Again)
  • 2022: Shared Insights from Currie Best Practice Groups

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Personality traits and complex problem solving: Personality disorders and their effects on complex problem-solving ability

Ulrike kipman.

1 College of Education, Institute of Educational Sciences and Research, Salzburg, Austria

Stephan Bartholdy

2 Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany

Marie Weiss

3 Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria

Wolfgang Aichhorn

4 Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Paracelsus Medical Private University, Salzburg, Austria

Günter Schiepek

5 Institute of Synergetics and Psychotherapy Research, Paracelsus Medical Private University, Salzburg, Austria

Associated Data

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Complex problem solving (CPS) can be interpreted as the number of psychological mechanisms that allow us to reach our targets in difficult situations, that can be classified as complex, dynamic, non-transparent, interconnected, and multilayered, and also polytelic. The previous results demonstrated associations between the personality dimensions neuroticism, conscientiousness, and extraversion and problem-solving performance. However, there are no studies dealing with personality disorders in connection with CPS skills. Therefore, the current study examines a clinical sample consisting of people with personality and/or depressive disorders. As we have data for all the potential personality disorders and also data from each patient regarding to potential depression, we meet the whole range from healthy to impaired for each personality disorder and for depression. We make use of a unique operationalization: CPS was surveyed in a simulation game, making use of the microworld approach. This study was designed to investigate the hypothesis that personality traits are related to CPS performance. Results show that schizotypal, histrionic, dependent, and depressive persons are less likely to successfully solve problems, while persons having the additional behavioral characteristics of resilience, action orientation, and motivation for creation are more likely to successfully solve complex problems.

Introduction

A problem arises when a person is unable to reach the desired goal. Problem-solving refers to the cognitive activities aimed at removing the obstacle separating the present situation from the target situation ( Betsch et al., 2011 ). In our daily lives, we are constantly confronted with new challenges and a plethora of possibilities to address them. Accordingly, problem-solving requires the ability to identify these possibilities and select the best option in the unfamiliar situations. It is, therefore, an important competence to deal with new conditions, adapt to changing circumstances, and react flexibly to new challenges ( Kipman, 2020 ).

Even tasks for which the sequence of choices to be taken is relatively straight-forward, such as in the process of navigating to a certain destination in a foreign city or cooperative decision-making during psychotherapy, appear as a highly diversified process, when considered in detail ( Schiepek, 2009 ; Schiepek et al., 2016a ). However, most problems we face in everyday life are not as well defined and do not necessarily have an unambiguous solution. The ability to deal with such sophisticated problems, i.e., complex problem solving (CPS) , is of particular relevance in everyday settings.

Funke (2001 , 2003 , 2012) and Dörner and Funke (2017) , identified five dimensions along which complex problems can be characterized: (i) The complexity of the problem arises from the number of variables contributing to the problem, which in turn affect the number of possible solutions. (ii) The connectivity of the problem arises from the number of interconnections between these variables. (iii) The dynamics of the problem arise from changes in the problem variables or their interconnections over time. These changes can be a result of the person’s actions or are inherent to the problem, i.e., characteristics of the variables themselves or a result of interactions between the variables. (iv) The non-transparency of a problem refers to the extent to which the target situation, the variables involved, their interactions and dynamics cannot be ascertained. (v) Finally, complex problems are usually polytelic , i.e., they have more than one target situation.

Accordingly, CPS requires the ability to model the problem space, i.e., understand which variables are involved and how they are interconnected, the ability to handle a large number of variables at the same time, judge the relevance and success probability of possibilities, identify the interconnections between variables and the inherent dynamics thereof, judge the consequences of one’s own actions with regards to the problem space, and collect relevant knowledge to deal with non-transparency.

Tasks to measure this complex set of abilities were developed by Dörner (1980 , 1986) , who criticized that the measurement of general intelligence tended to use simple tasks that are not comparable with the level of complexity of real-world problems. He proposed measuring intelligent behavior in computerized environments specifically adapted to simulate the properties of sophisticated problems in everyday settings ( Danner et al., 2011b ). cf. Dörner et al. (1983) in research used settings referred to as Microworlds to assess the way participants acted under heterogeneous, dynamic, and non-transparent conditions. Participants were instructed to administrate a tiny German village by the name of Lohhausen by creating the ideal conditions for the village and its inhabitants ( Hussy, 1998 , p. 140–141). This microworld comprised more than 2,000 variables, guaranteeing an elevated level of complexity, which also required a high-level operationalization of CPS. However, the general validity of the performance at Lohhausen turned out to be a questionable issue, since the performance was operationalized as a factor composed of 6 main criteria, some of which were subjective assessments. Accordingly, the parameter definition for CPS performance was rather ambiguous. The reason for this ambiguity is that the vague description of the objective, i.e., to establish a respectable standard of well-being among the inhabitants—gave room for subjective interpretation (cf. Hussy, 1998 , p. 146–150). Since then, the psychometric validity of the CPS performance in complex microworlds has been demonstrated by several researchers (e.g., Wittmann and Hattrup, 2004 ; Danner et al., 2011a ).

Because of the high-translational relevance of the topic, the question arises how and which individual differences contribute to more or less efficient solving of the complex problems, such as Microworlds. Individual differences in problem-solving have been described along a cognitive dimension, i.e., the problem-solving style , and an emotional–motivational dimension, i.e., the problem orientation ( D’Zurilla et al., 2011 ). Cognitively, problems can be solved in a rational style , i.e., systematically and deliberate, in an impulsive style , i.e., careless, hurried, and often incomplete, or in an avoidance style via passivity and inaction leading to procrastination ( D’Zurilla et al., 2002 , as cited in D’Zurilla et al., 2011 ). Emotionally, people with a positive problem orientation , see problems as an opportunity for success, i.e., a “challenge” and are confident that the problem is solvable, and that they will be able to solve it. People with a negative problem orientation view problems as an opportunity for failure, i.e., a “threat” and doubt their ability to solve the problem ( D’Zurilla et al., 2011 ).

Some studies have already related basic personality traits, such as the BIG-5, to the way a person tackles complex problems. For example, it has been demonstrated that individuals who score high in conscientiousness, openness for experience, and extraversion also have higher problem-solving abilities. In contrast, individuals with higher scores in neuroticism show poor problem-solving abilities ( D’Zurilla et al., 2011 ). McMurran et al. (2001) demonstrate that this is a result of the way in which neurotic individuals approach problems. Neuroticisms was significantly associated with an impulsive or avoidant problem-solving style, and a negative problem orientation. Vice versa, Arslan (2016) identified a positive relationship between constructive problem-solving and being extrovert, receptive, and open to new learning experiences, and also high in tolerability and accountability.

The present study seeks to extend these findings to individuals with “extreme” levels of personality traits, i.e., individuals with personality disorders, taking into consideration the way in which personality characteristics manifest in everyday situations, such as work–place situations. Following the most current diagnostic approach to personality disorders as outlined in the ICD-11, the individual accentuations of 9 disorder-relevant personality traits were taken into account, including:

  • (i) Paranoid traits , i.e., the extent of mistrust toward others.
  • (ii) Schizoid traits , i.e., the inability to express feelings and experience pleasure, resulting in fierce separation from affective contacts and also friends and social gatherings with an excessive preference for the magical worlds.
  • (iii) Antisocial traits , i.e., the extent of disregard for social obligations and callous lack of involvement in feelings for others, resulting in aggressive behavior.
  • (iv) Borderline traits , i.e., the tendency to act out impulses without regard to consequences, associated with unpredictable and erratic moods.
  • (v) Histrionic traits , i.e., the tendency to overdramatize and show a theatrical, exaggerated expression of feelings, suggestibility, egocentricity, hedonism, and a constant desire for recognition, external stimuli, and attention.
  • (vi) Dependent traits , i.e., excessive and inappropriate agreeableness ( Costa and McCrae, 1986 ) resulting in major anxiety about separation, feelings of helplessness, and a tendency to subordinate oneself to the desires of others.
  • (vii) Schizotypal traits , i.e., extreme levels of introversion, resulting in social disengagement.
  • (viii) Obsessive-compulsive (anankastic) traits , i.e., excessive conscientiousness, involving feelings of doubt, perfectionism, and inflexibility.
  • (ix) Depressive traits , i.e., the tendency toward persistent feelings of sadness and loss of interest.

Few studies have assessed problem-solving, much less CPS, in patients with personality disorders. Previous research shows, that patients with histrionic and narcissistic personality types show an impulsive problem-solving style, whereas avoidant and dependent individuals show a negative problem orientation ( McMurran et al., 2007 ). In addition, people who are in a depressive mood ( Lyubomirsky et al., 1999 ), or even clinically depressed and anxious have difficulties generating effective solutions to problems ( Marx et al., 1992 ). Accordingly, we hypothesize a negative association between high accentuations of disorder-related personality traits and CPS. The aim of the present study was to explore, which disorders were most severely affected and whether this association also manifested in work-related situations.

Action-orientated problem-solving is particularly required in areas where people are under a lot of stress, for example, in entrepreneurship, team leading in the clinical settings, or firefighting. Especially when a work-related crisis appears, action-oriented problem-solving is important, because it unites handling both novel and routine demands ( Rudolph and Repenning, 2002 , as cited in Rudolph et al., 2009 ). Rudolph et al. (2009) found that only by taking action, information cues become available. Accordingly, both CPS and everyday situations in the work-place require the ability to cope with stressful events and protect oneself from the negative effects of stress, i.e., resilience ( Lee and Cranford, 2008 , as cited in Wagnild and Young, 1993 ; Fletcher and Sarkar, 2013 ). Indeed, individuals with a high trait resilience are more willing to take action in problem-solving ( Li and Yang, 2009 , as cited in Li et al., 2013 ). This is consistent with previous research demonstrating that effective problem-solving abilities go along with high-psychological resilience ( Garcia-Dia et al., 2013 ; Williamson et al., 2013 ; Crowther et al., 2016 , as cited in Pinar et al., 2018 ). Pinar et al. (2018) even found that problem-solving competencies can be increased by increasing psychological resilience and self-confidence levels. Accordingly, identifying which personality disorders are most severely affected in these areas may also provide hints for psychotherapy.

Materials and methods

Participants.

The present study included data from N = 242 adults (49.1% male) with personality disorders and/or depressive disorders, with ages ranging from 17 to 48 years (mean: 26.5 years). The participants were given five assessment batteries and a set of demographic variables, which included game experience. They were also given a commercial complex problem-solving (CPS) game known as Cities: Skylines involving the construction and managing of a city like a mayor would with the goal of growing the city while not running out of money. Participants were patients from psychiatric and psychosomatic hospitals, who got follow-up treatment directly after leaving the hospital. The treatment took place in a panel practice for aftercare where the CPS experiment was done (see Figure 1 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-788402-g001.jpg

Exemplary model of some (not all) factors that influence the number of inhabitants and the general happiness of the population in Cities: Skylines (CSL). The number of related variables illustrates the complexity, connectivity, and polytely in the simulated environment.

Personality questionnaires

In order to obtain a comprehensive diagnosis and measure disordered personality traits in a continuous fashion, three personality questionnaires were used, including the PSSI, SCID-5-PD, and MMPI-II. While the PSSI scores were used in the statistical analysis, SCID-5-PD scores and MMPI-II scores were used to confirm the PSSI diagnosis. Furthermore, in order to assess the manifestation of disordered personality traits in work-related situations, we used the BIP.

The Persönlichkeits-Stil und Störungs-Inventar (PSSI; Kuhl and Kazen, 2009 ) is a self-report instrument that measures the comparative manifestation of the character traits. These are designed as non-pathological analogs of the personality disorders described in the psychiatric diagnostic manuals DSM-IV and ICD-10. The PSSI comprises 140 items assigned to 14 scales: PN (willful-paranoid), SZ (independent-schizoid), ST (intuitive-schizotypal), BL (impulsive-borderline), HI (agreeable-histrionic), NA (ambitious-narcissistic), SU (self-critical-avoidant), AB (loyal-dependent), ZW (conscientious-compulsive—anankastic), NT (critical-negativistic), DP (calm-depressive), SL (helpful-selfless), RH (optimistic-rhapsodic), and AS (self-assertive-antisocial). Patients rate each item on a 4-point Likert scale (from 0 to 3) and continuous scale values are calculated as the sum of the 10 item ratings belonging to a scale. Accordingly, a maximum value of 30 can be achieved for each scale. In this study, we focused on the nine traits PN, SZ, ST, BL, HI, AB, ZW, DP, and AS, as the other measured traits are not listed as personality disorders in the ICD-10 or DSM-V.

The Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-5—Persönlichkeitsstörungen (SCID-5-PD; First et al., 2019 ) is a semi-structured diagnostic questionnaire that can be used to evaluate the 10 personality disorders included in the DSM-5 in clusters A, B, and C, as well as disorders in the category “not otherwise specified personality disorder.” Each DSM-5 criterion is assigned corresponding interview questions to assist the interviewer in assessing the criterion. It is possible to utilize the SCID-5-PD to categorically diagnose personality disorders (present or absent) ( First et al., 2019 ). In addition, regulations are also included which can be used to create dimensional ratings.

The MMPI ® –2 ( Butcher et al., 2000 ) is a revised and completely re-normed version of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI). With the help of the MMPI ® –2, a relatively complete picture of the personality structure can be obtained in an economical way.

The Bochumer Inventar zur berufsbezogenen Persönlichkeitsbeschreibung (BIP; Hossiep and Paschen, 2019 ) measures personality traits in a work-related context. A total of 210 items are assigned to 4 global dimensions including 14 subscales. These include work orientation (diligence, agility, and focus), professional approach ( performance-, creativity-, and management motivation), social competencies (sensitivity, social skills, sociability, teamwork, and assertiveness), and mental constitution (emotional stability, resilience, and self-confidence) on a continuous scale. Patients respond to each item on a 6-point Likert scale.

Game experience

As possible previous experience with the CPS game may affect the level of problem-solving efficiency during the test, participants were asked to rate their previous engagement with simulation-based urban development games on a 4-point Likert scale with response options running from “none” to “very much.” The same poll also featured a listing of 20 symbols from Cities: Skylines, in combination with their meanings (e.g., “no electricity”) for participants to make use of during their quest. At the end, participants were asked to rate their experience based on a 5-point scaling reaching from 1 (extremely simple) to 5 (super challenging). At last, the researcher also marked on each poll sheet, whether (a) the individual patient was able to accomplish the mission (Success, Failure, or Patient Breakup), and (b) the exact time frame of the testing session (morning, afternoon, or evening).

Cities: Skylines (CSL)

The computer-based simulation game Cities: Skylines ( Paradox Interactive, 2015a ), which can be downloaded from Steam for about 30 dollars, explores the construction and management of a city and was implemented in the current study as a Microworld scenario. Much like in the successful microworld Lohhausen ( Dörner et al., 1983 ), gamers in Cities: Skylines basically act in lieu of the city’s mayor, taking over all of his authority and duties. As promised in the user manual, it “offers endless sandbox play in a city that keeps offering new areas, resources, and technologies to explore, continually presenting the player with new challenges to overcome” ( Paradox Interactive, 2015b , p. 4). The game fulfills the parameters of Brehmer and Dörner’s (1993) microworlds and meets the standards of complex problems according to Funke ’s ( 2001; 2012 ). The examples below illustrate the way in which these features are relevant for Cities: Skylines (see Figure 2 ; see also de Kooter, 2015 ; Paradox Interactive, 2015b ):

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-788402-g002.jpg

Procedure of the study.

  • (i) Complexity is fulfilled because the system is made up of a variety of components including a vast series of different constructions (areas, basic resources, roads, constructions, electricity, water supplies, etc.), options (fiscal matters, budgeting, credit, traffic management, security, healthcare, and education), and parameters (population density, inhabitant satisfaction, environmental issues, and delinquency). As an example, while purchasing a wind turbine, the participant may weigh related costs, budgeted funds for the week, potential noise pollution, the way the turbine blends into the landscape vs. the rate of efficiency, along with the hardware required to connect the device to the town’s existing network, etc.
  • (ii) Connectivity is fulfilled because the parameters in the model are heavily interconnected. Each component is related to at least one other element (see Figure 2 ) implementing a network of correlations and interdependencies. As an example, residential zones should not be located in proximity to wind turbines, as the amount of noise pollution caused by their operation might affect the quality of life in that zone, which again might make the area less attractive and lower the property values.
  • (iii) Dynamics are fulfilled because the demands of the population are subject to autonomous change, while other variables, e.g., zoning requirements also depend in part on the actions of the participants. While the dynamics of the game cause the population and the territory of the city to grow, the whole infrastructure becomes inadequate over time and needs to be adapted. Water and electricity infrastructures, the number of schools, clinics, municipal cemeteries, etc., that used to suffice for the population then need to be expanded. Moreover, depending on its frequentation, each building or road has a certain life span until it is left abandoned and will have to be replaced.
  • (iv) Non-transparency is not featured as an essential part of the Cities: Skylines gameplay, but is instead primarily caused by its connectivity and intricacy. While playing the game, the number of variables and their interconnections make active exploration essential. Independent of the player’s actions; however, there are also very non-transparent features, such as random death waves or an (unexpectedly) higher incidence of fires in the area following the first construction of a firefighter center by the player.
  • (v) Polytely arises since the objective to increase the population of the city requires the simultaneous achievement of a large number of minor tasks, which may be conflicting (e.g., strategic allocation of bus stops for both students and employees). The situation is further complicated by unforeseen complications (e.g., water pollution causing disease spread), which force the player to abandon his/her ongoing task and give full attention to the new issue. The source of the problem must be evaluated while new strategies for potential solutions are weighed against proven approaches. For the current research, each patient was provided with identical settings, including a sizeable, completely functional city with a number of 2,600 residents, 50,000 game money points, and a general population satisfaction level of 90%. Their subsequent task was to boost the population of the cities to 5,000 residents while making sure that the residents were not poorly (as measured by an average satisfaction level of at least 75%) and the bank balance remained positive. On the contrary, the task was left unaccomplished if (a) the population of the urban areas dropped to 1,000, (b) the balance of the account dropped to 0, or (c) the maximum game time of 120 min had elapsed. Patients received the tip, that it was necessary to set priorities and focus on the mission.

Based on the task of raising the number of inhabitants of the city, a parameter of CPS performance was calculated as the average growth of the population relative to the target size of 5,000:

Gamers were instructed not to modify the time settings during the game, to allow for comparable measurements across participants.

Given that the participants were patients from psychiatric and psychosomatic hospitals, many of them lacked game experience. To increase test fairness between patients with different levels of game experience, all the participants were provided with a brief introduction on how to handle a list of fundamental game features:

  • • placement of streets, buildings, water pumps, and wind turbines;
  • • positioning of roads, structures, water pipes, and turbines;
  • • division of zones (housing, businesses, and industries/offices zones) and the mode of bulldozing;
  • • structural survey of power, water lines, and waste collection;
  • • search for the info stats to view the requirements of the residents;

Statistical analysis

For all the statistical analyses, SPSS version 26.0 (2020) was used.

On the basis of the ICD-11 definition, the personality traits were not analyzed categorically (as before), but dimensionally. To relate the expression of currently recognized personality disorders to performance in CPS, we used correlation analyses between CPS performance and the 9 scale scores of the PSSI (verified by the SCID and MMPI-2) and also the 4 overall dimensions of the BIP. Given the high number of resulting correlations, p -values could be misleading because of the multiple testing. Accordingly, we identified relevant personality traits for CPS using (i) The Bonferonni-correction of p -values and (ii) an effect sizes cut-off of r > 0.25.

In a second step, we explored, which facets of the BIP contributed to the associations with CPS performance in order to get a more fine-grained picture of possible effects.

In sum, we sought to identify the strongest predictors of CPS performance using 3 multivariate regression models with the 9 clinical traits, controlling for gender in the 2nd model and additional game experience in the 3rd model.

Table 1 lists the experience with urban planning simulation games in the current sample. About 50% of the patients rated the game as “easy” or “rather easy,” 37.5% rated it as “not easy but also not difficult” and 12.6% responded that the game was “difficult” or “very difficult.”

Experience of the sample ( N = 242, N = 210 valid answers).

Correlation analyses show that CPS performance was negatively related to schizotypal ( r = −0.46), histrionic ( r = −0.44), and depressive ( r = −0.46) personality accentuations. The higher the expression in any of these areas, the higher the probability of failing in CPS. Effect sizes (: = r ) were > 0.40 for each of these traits (compare Table 2 ). Furthermore, CPS-performance was negatively correlated with the dependent ( r = −0.29) and paranoid ( r = −0.25) personality traits, but coefficients were much lower and therefore of less practical relevance as for schizotypical, histrionic, and depressive traits. Schizoid ( r = 0.04), borderline ( r = 0.17), anankastic ( r = −0.05), and anti-social ( r = −0.04) traits were not significantly associated with the CPS (see Table 3 ).

Correlations of CPS and personality disorders with work-related personality manifestations as assessed with the BIP.

Correlations surpassing an effect size of r = 0.25 are highlighted in bold font, italic numbers have a lower effect size but are still significant when taking only the Bonferroni Correction into account, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

Correlations between personality traits and CPS performance.

Correlations surpassing an effect size of r = 0.25 are highlighted in bold font, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05 but not significant when taking the Bonferroni Correction into account.

Regarding the work-related manifestations of the personality traits, CPS-performance was positively associated with the overall BIP dimensions of work orientation ( r = 0.27), professional orientation ( r = 0.34), and psychological constitution ( r = 0.25), but negatively with the overall BIP dimension social competencies ( r = −0.25). In order to explore these associations further, CPS performance and personality disorders were related to the sub-facet scores of the BIP (see Table 2 ).

Professional orientation was also negatively correlated with depressive traits ( r = −0.40), the psychological constitution was negatively correlated with borderline traits (−0.38), dependent traits (−0.31), and with depressive traits (−0.26).

The results demonstrate that particularly the facets resilience, action orientation, and motivation for creation were positively correlated with successful problem-solving, while sociability and CPS were significantly negatively correlated. The higher the resilience, action orientation and motivation for creation and the lower the sociability, the better was the CPS performance. When we take Bonferroni correction into account, also conscientiousness and motivation for leadership (italic in the table) were negatively correlated with the CPS performance.

Interestingly, the associations between personality disorders and work-related personality expressions were moderate. The strongest associations arose for resilience, which was negatively associated with several personality disorders, particularly, borderline, histrionic, and dependent traits. Focusing on the traits that showed the strongest impairment in CPS, schizotypal traits were associated with high sociability ( r = 0.36) and to a lesser extent with low-action orientation ( r = −0.22), which in turn related to low-CPS performance. Histrionic traits were related to low resilience ( r = −0.28), which in turn related to low-CPS performance. Depressive traits were related to low motivation for creation ( r = −0.25), and also low-leadership motivation ( r = −0.34) and to a lesser extent low-achievement motivation ( r = −0.21), low-action orientation ( r = −0.20), and low resilience ( r = −0.24), which in turn is related to low-CPS performance.

In a combined model with all 9 personality traits (adjusted R 2 = 36.7%), we confirmed that histrionic traits have the biggest negative impact on CPS performance (β = −0.351), followed by schizotypical (β = −0.312) and depressive traits (β = −0.303). Also, in the multiple regression model, dependent and paranoid traits are negatively related to CPS performance. If gender is the part of the model and held constant in a model containing the 9 traits, histrionic traits still have a significant and practical relevant impact of β′ = −0.325. (Condition Index = 24). The same holds true when also taking game experience into account (β ′ ′ = −0.319) see Table 4 .

Combined regression model, β′: controlling for gender, β ′ ′ controlling for gender and game experience.

Correlations surpassing an effect size of r = 0.25 are highlighted in bold font, **p < 0.001.

(Condition Index checking for possible multicollinearity is moderate with CI = 22, 36, so multicollinearity is moderately given, βs are, therefore, interpretable, p -values can be slightly biased, βs with 0.3 and higher found in this model for the 3 traits have for certain a significant and practically relevant impact).

The present study examined the influences of personality traits on the CPS performance in a clinical sample of individuals with a range of different psychiatric diagnoses. The aim of this empirical analysis was to extend previous research on individual differences in CPS to extreme personality traits as observed in personality disorders, and also their manifestation in work-related situations. We explored, which personality dimensions were most strongly associated with impairments in the CPS.

With regards to the clinical personality dimensions (i.e., dimensionally defined personality disorders), statistical analyses revealed that schizotypal, histrionic, dependent, and depressive personality traits were associated negatively with the participants’ performances in the given CPS task (consistent with, e.g., McMurran et al., 2007 ). Previous findings on these relationships were, therefore, further confirmed, specifically in showing that subjects with high levels of depressiveness and anxiety seemed to have more difficulties in finding and executing effective solutions to the given complex problems (e.g., see Marx et al., 1992 ; Lyubomirsky et al., 1999 ).

Unsurprisingly, no single clinical personality structure was associated with better problem-solving performances (as compared with the non-clinical trait levels). As personality disorders are generally linked with increased levels of neuroticism, which subsequently was consistently found to negatively influence problem-solving (e.g., McMurran et al., 2001 ; D’Zurilla et al., 2011 ), this result is also consistent with the general clinical intuition. But, contrary to the previous findings ( D’Zurilla et al., 2011 ), conscientiousness had no significant impact on CPS performance in this sample.

Further analyses gave deeper insights into relationships that were found in the first part of the data analyses. They are especially allowed to draw conclusions for the clinical patients. It was found that higher levels of resilience (consistent with, e.g., Garcia-Dia et al., 2013 ; Williamson et al., 2013 ; Crowther et al., 2016 , as cited in Li and Yang, 2009 ; Pinar et al., 2018 , as cited in Li et al., 2013 ), action orientation, and motivation for creation (e.g., see Eseryel et al., 2014 ) positively influenced the problem-solving performance as additional behavioral characteristics . This indicates that, even for high levels of usually negative personality traits, a person’s ability to successfully solve problems will not be impaired automatically if the person is also very resilient to the effects of negative events and highly action-oriented and motivated when facing problems. Hence, this interpretation is consistent with the conclusions of a study by Güss et al. (2017) , who found that more approach-oriented individuals outperformed avoidance-oriented participants in the complex problems. In this way, these positive traits act against the negative impact of otherwise impairing personality traits or even disorders. Interestingly, sociability was found to have a negative influence on the participants’ performances, while no significant influences on social skills, team orientation, or self-confidence were found. Therefore, it seems to be more comprehensible why some of us deal easily with complex problems and can manage things forward-looking while others do not succeed in making good decisions.

Data availability statement

Ethics statement.

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

UK was the main author, did all calculations, research to and wrote the article. SB did the programming of the microworlds and all technical support. MW did the review on the introduction and discussion part. WA and GS served as a consultant. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

We thank Martina Mathur and Belinda Pletzer for proofreading and translating.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

  • Arslan C. (2016). Interpersonal problem solving, self-compassion and personality traits in university students. Educ. Res. Rev. 11 474–481. 10.5897/ERR2015.2605 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Betsch T., Funke J., PLessner H. (2011). Denken - urteilen, entscheiden, problemlösen . Berlin: Springer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brehmer B., Dörner D. (1993). Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 9 171–184. 10.1016/0747-5632(93)90005-D [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butcher J. N., Dahlstrom W. G., Graham J. R., Tellegen A., Kaemmer B. (2000). Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory ® -2. Bern: Huber. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Costa P. T., McCrae R. R. (1986). Personality stability and its implications for clincal psychology. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 6 407–423. 10.1016/0272-7358(86)90029-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crowther S., Hunter B., McAra-Couper J., Warren L., Gilkison A., Hunter M., et al. (2016). Sustainability and resilience in midwifery: A discussion paper. Midwifery 40 40–48. 10.1016/j.midw.2016.06.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dörner D. (1980). On the difficulties people have in dealing with complexity . Simul. Games 11 , 87–106. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dörner D. (1986). Diagnostik der operativen intelligenz . Diagnostica 32 , 290–308. [ Google Scholar ]
  • D’Zurilla T. J., Maydeu-Olivares A., Gallardo-Pujol D. (2011). Predicting social problem solving using personality traits. Pers. Individ. Differ. 50 142–147. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.015 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • D’Zurilla T. J., Nezu A. M., Maydeu-Olivares A. (2002). The Social Problem-Solving Inventory-Revised (SPSI-R): Technical Manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems, Inc. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Danner D., Hagemann D., Holt D. V., Hager M., Schankin A., Wüstenberg S., et al. (2011a). Measuring performance in dynamic decision making. J. Individ. Differ. 32 225–233. 10.1027/1614-0001/a000055 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Danner D., Hagemann D., Schankin A., Hager M., Funke J. (2011b). Beyond IQ: A latent state-trait analysis of general intelligence, dynamic decision making, and implicit learning. Intelligence 39 323–334. 10.1016/j.intell.2011.06.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Kooter S. (2015). Cities Skylines Guide: Beginner Tips and Tricks Guide. Available Online at: https://www.gameplayinside.com/strategy/cities-skylines/cities-skylines-guide-beginner-tips-and-tricks-guide/ . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dörner D., Funke J. (2017). Complex problem solving: What it is and what it is not. Front. Psychol. 8 : 1153 . 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01153 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dörner D., Kreuzig H. W., Reither F., Stäudel T. (1983). Lohhausen: Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität. Soziologische Rev. 8 : 93 . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eseryel D., Law V., Ifenthaler D., Ge X., Miller R. (2014). An investigation of the interrelationships between motivation, engagement, and complex problem solving in game-based learning. Educ. Technol. Soc. 17 42–53. [ Google Scholar ]
  • First M., Williams J., Smith B., Spitzer R. (2019). SCID-5-PD. Göttingen: Hogrefe. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fletcher D., Sarkar M. (2013). A review of psychological resilience. Eur. Psychol. 18 12–23. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000124 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Funke J. (2001). Dynamic systems as tools for analysing human judgement. Think. Reason. 7 69–89. 10.1080/13546780042000046 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Funke J. (2003). Problemlösendes Denken. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Funke J. (2012). “ Complex Problem Solving ,” in Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning , ed. Seel N. M. (Boston, MA: Springer US; ), 682–685. 10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_685 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Garcia-Dia M. J., DiNapoli J. M., Garcia-Ona L., Jakubowski R., O’Flaherty D. (2013). Concept analysis: Resilience. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 27 264–270. 10.1016/j.apnu.2013.07.003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Güss C. D., Burger M. L., Dörner D. (2017). The role of motivation in complex problem solving. Front. Psychol. 8 : 851 . 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00851 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hossiep R., Paschen M. (2019). Bochumer Inventar zur berufsbezogenen Persönlichkeitsbeschreibung . Göttingern: Hogrefe. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hussy W. (1998). Denken und Problemlösen. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kipman U. (2020). Komplexes problemlösen . Wiesbaden: Springer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kuhl J., Kazen M. (2009). Persönlichkeits-Stil-und Störungs-Inventar . Göttingen: Hogrefe. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee H. H., Cranford J. A. (2008). Does resilience moderate the associations between parental problem drinking and adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing behaviors?: A study of Korean adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend. 96 213–221. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.007 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Li M. H., Yang Y. (2009). Determinants of problem solving, social support-seeking, and avoidance: A path analytic model. Int. J. Stress Manag. 16 155–176. 10.1037/a0016844 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Li M. H., Eschenauer R., Yang Y. (2013). Influence of efficacy and resilience on problem solving in the United States, Taiwan, and China. Multicult. Couns. Dev. 41 144–157. 10.1002/j.2161-1912.2013.00033.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lyubomirsky S., Tucker K. L., Caldwell N. D. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77 1041–1060. 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1041 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marx E. M., Williams J. M., Claridge G. C. (1992). Depression and social problem solving. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 101 78–86. 10.1037/0021-843X.101.1.78 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McMurran M., Duggan C., Christopher G., Huband N. (2007). The relationships between personality disorders and social problem solving in adults. Pers. Individ. Differ. 42 145–155. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McMurran M., Egan V., Blair M., Richardson C. (2001). The relationship between social problem-solving and personality in mentally disordered offenders. Pers. Individ. Differ. 30 517–524. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00050-7 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paradox Interactive (2015a). Cities: Skylines. Stockholm, SE: Paradox Interactive. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paradox Interactive (2015b). Cities: Skylines: User Manual. Stockholm, SE: Paradox Interactive. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pinar S. E., Yildirim G., Sayin N. (2018). Investigating the psychological resilience, self-confidence and problemsolving skills of midwife candidates. Nurse Educ. Today 64 144–149. 10.1016/j.nedt.2018.02.014 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rudolph J. W., Repenning N. P. (2002). Disaster dynamics: Understanding the role of quantity in organizational collapse. Adm. Sci. Q. 47 1–30. 10.2307/3094889 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rudolph J. W., Morrison J. B., Carroll J. S. (2009). The dynamics of action-oriented problem solving: Linking interpretation and choice. Acad. Manag. Rev. 34 733–756. 10.5465/AMR.2009.44886170 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schiepek G. (2009). Complexity and nonlinear dynamics in psychotherapy. Eur. Rev. 17 331–356. 10.1017/S1062798709000763 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schiepek G., Stöger-Schmidinger B., Aichhorn W., Schöller H., Aas B. (2016a). Systemic case formulation, individualized process monitoring, and state dynamics in a case of dissociative identity disorder. Front. Psychol. Clin. Settings 7 : 1545 . 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01545 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wagnild G. M., Young H. M. (1993). Development and psychometric evaluation of the resilience scale. J. Nurs. Meas. 1 165–178. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williamson G. R., Health V., Proctor-Childs T. (2013). Vocation, friendship and resilience: A study exploring nursing student and staff views on retention and attrition. Open J. Nurs. 7 149–156. 10.2174/1874434601307010149 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wittmann W. W., Hattrup K. (2004). The relationship between performance in dynamic systems and intelligence. Syst. Res. Behav. Sci. 21 393–409. 10.1002/sres.653 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

A Problem-Solving Style Can Be Described Using Dimensions Of​

Question 32

A problem-solving style can be described using dimensions of​

A) ​structure and consideration. B) structure and cooperation.​ C) dominance and sociability.​ D) assertiveness and cooperation.​

Correct Answer:

Q27: Assertive behavior​ A) ​tends to violate the rights

Q28: Which style of conflict management is a

Q29: A lose/lose conflict management strategy​ A) ​is never

Q30: A manager could apply a win/win strategy

Q31: Voluntary arbitration means​ A) ​the involvement of government

Q33: People who are trying to resolve conflict

Q34: Colleen frequently expresses her political views at

Q35: Candace owns a small business that is

Q36: Conflict in organizations​ A) ​can serve as an

Q37: Anne and Larry are discussing what restaurant

10+ million students use Quizplus to study and prepare for their homework, quizzes and exams through 20m+ questions in 300k quizzes.

Explore our library and get Management Homework Help with various study sets and a huge amount of quizzes and questions

Unlock this Answer For Free Now!

View this answer and more for free by performing one of the following actions

browser extension

Get instant 2 free unlocks once you install the browser extension

qr-code

Scan the QR code to install the App and get 2 free unlocks

upload documents

Unlock quizzes for free by uploading documents

IMAGES

  1. A Problem-solving Style Can Be Described Using Dimensions of

    a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

  2. What Is Problem-Solving? Steps, Processes, Exercises to do it Right

    a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

  3. Master Your Problem Solving and Decision Making Skills

    a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

  4. A Problem-solving Style Can Be Described Using Dimensions of

    a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

  5. 4 Steps Problem Solving Template

    a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

  6. 7 Steps to Improve Your Problem Solving Skills

    a problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

VIDEO

  1. Are you an implementer?

  2. Problem-Solving skills for UX Designers #uxdesign

  3. Are you a conceptualizer?

  4. Ex: Problem Solving

  5. Homogeneity of Physical Equations using Dimensions| Unit 1 Physics & Measurements

  6. 2.8 Dimensional Analysis Practice Problems

COMMENTS

  1. Quiz #3: Chapter 13

    A problem-solving style can be described using dimensions of When management-labor union disputes cannot be resolved, they may try other tactics, such as: A manager could apply a win/win strategy by

  2. Understanding individual problem-solving style: A key to learning and

    Assessing problem-solving style. Our model of problem-solving style can be operationally defined ... This 34-item instrument assesses the three dimensions and six styles in our problem-solving style model. Selby et al., 2007a, Selby et al., 2004 provided ... Study 1 showed that behaviors could be described with the CPS model. ...

  3. PDF Defining and Assessing Problem- Solving Style: Design and Development

    ABSTRACT. VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style (Selby, Treffinger, & Isaksen, 2002) is a new instrument for assessing problem-solving style, for use with individuals from ages 12 through adult. It measures three dimensions of style relating to creative problem solving and change management. In this article, we discuss the construction ...

  4. About Problem Solving Styles

    Problem-solving styles are consistent individual differences in the ways people prefer to deal with new ideas, manage change, and respond effectively to complex, open-ended opportunities and challenges. Knowledge of style is important in education in a number of ways. It contributes to adults' ability to work together effectively in teams and ...

  5. Understanding individual problem-solving style: A key to learning and

    That evolution also involves an emerging understanding of the construct of problem-solving style and the influence that construct holds on creative productivity. VIEW: An assessment of problem solving style is a new instrument for assessing problem-solving style. It is the result of more than five years of research and development.

  6. Problem-Solving Strategies and Obstacles

    Heuristics are shortcut strategies that people can use to solve a problem at hand. These "rule of thumb" approaches allow you to simplify complex problems, reducing the total number of possible solutions to a more manageable set. ... The social dimension of a solitary moment. In: The Systems Model of Creativity. 2015:73-98. doi:10.1007/978-94 ...

  7. 13 Dimensions of Problem-Solving, a Unique Approach to Conundrums

    11 - Revisit Dimension #3 (Determine the protocols). Adjust, fine-tune, calibrate, and manipulate. Do what is needed to ensure your process is still in alignment with the goal, and the vision of the problem when it is corrected. 12 - Examine the outcome. The results should be pending now, or perhaps even visible.

  8. Understanding individual problem-solving style: A key to learning and

    More than five decades of research and development have focused on making the Creative Problem Solving process and tools accessible across a wide range of ages and contexts. Recent evidence indicates that when individuals, in both school and corporate settings, understand their own style of problem solving, they are able to learn and apply process tools more effectively, and when teams ...

  9. PDF Problem Solving Style and Creative Productivity

    on the three dimensions of problem solving style (Orientation to Change, Manner of Processing, and Ways of Deciding). Students'presentations were scored for clarity of prompts used, for supporting details, and for use of ... After seeing the three clips, the multi-media project was then described. Students were to work together in their ...

  10. Solved A problem-solving style can be described using

    A problem - solving style can be described using dimensions of. Group of answer choices. dominance and sociability. assertiveness and cooperation. structure and consideration. structure and cooperation. Here's the best way to solve it. Powered by Chegg AI. Share Share.

  11. Defining and Assessing Problem‐Solving Style: Design and Development of

    VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style (Selby, Treffinger, & Isaksen, 2002) is a new instrument for assessing problem-solving style, for use with individuals from ages 12 through adult. It measures three dimensions of style relating to creative problem solving and change management. In this article, we discuss the construction of the instrument, the initial evidence supporting the ...

  12. Problem Solving Styles

    It outlines the nature of problem-solving style (based on extensive theory and research), explaining in clear, non-technical language what problem-solving styles are - and are not - and describes three problem-solving style dimensions and six styles. The book explains the important and unique personal characteristics and implications, benefits ...

  13. How can you describe your problem-solving style?

    1. Identify your style. Be the first to add your personal experience. 2. Highlight your strengths. Be the first to add your personal experience. 3. Acknowledge your weaknesses. Be the first to add ...

  14. PDF Problem Solving Style: A New Approach to Understanding and Using

    concluded that problem solving style is a very important dimension of creative ... three dimensions using Cronbach's coefficient Alpha. The coefficient Alpha ... The rotated factor structure indicates that VIEW's 34 items can be accurately described in a three-factor model, and the items for each of the three dimensions do load (r>.30) on ...

  15. Personality traits and complex problem solving: Personality disorders

    Individual differences in problem-solving have been described along a cognitive dimension, i.e., the problem-solving style, and an emotional-motivational dimension, i.e., the problem orientation ... These are designed as non-pathological analogs of the personality disorders described in the psychiatric diagnostic manuals DSM-IV and ICD-10 ...

  16. A Problem-Solving Style Can Be Described Using Dimensions Of

    View this answer and more for free by performing one of the following actions. A problem-solving style can be described using dimensions of A) structure and consideration. B) structure and cooperation. C) dominance and sociability. D) assertiveness and cooperation.

  17. PDF Problem Solving Style: A New Approach to Understanding and Using

    VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving StyleSM. This article will describe VIEW and its principal dimensions. VIEW seeks to represent and assess three dimensions of style preferences that are unique and important in understanding and guiding the efforts of individuals and groups to manage their creative problem solving or inventive efforts ...

  18. A problem solving style can be described using the dimensions of... a

    Final answer: Problem-solving styles can be described using various dimensions such as structure and consideration, structure and cooperation, dominance and sociability, assertiveness and cooperation.Each of these dimensions relates to a person's behavior in a group or organizational setting.Effectively, these dimensions help determine a person's approach to problem-solving.

  19. A problem solving style can be described using dimensions of

    AFinal answer:. A problem solving style can be described using dimensions such as analytical thinking, creativity, decision-making, problem-solving strategies, and communication skills.. Explanation: A problem solving style can be described using various dimensions that help identify and understand how individuals approach and solve problems. These dimensions include: