FREE SHIPPING FOR ORDERS P3,000 & UP | WE SHIP NATIONWIDE

10% OFF ON ALL BOOKS! Discount Code: HAPPY12

Paper Boox Logo

Item added to your cart

Online or offline reading: which is better for learning.

In today's digital world, many of us are turning to online sources for reading materials, but is it really the best way to learn? Is online or offline reading better for learning? In this blog post, we'll examine the pros and cons of both online and offline reading to see which one is better for learning. We'll also take a look at some of the research that has been done to better understand the differences between these two methods. Finally, we'll provide some tips on how to make the most of either approach so that you can maximize your learning potential.

Advantages of Online Reading

This blog post will focus on the advantages of reading online. By reading online, one can access a much larger library of information than is available through traditional offline sources like books and newspapers. Additionally, the internet provides access to a wealth of resources that are up-to-date and relevant to the topics you are studying. This can make it easier to stay current on the latest developments in your field.

Online reading also provides a more interactive learning experience than traditional offline reading. For example, you can use online forums and discussion boards to interact with experts in your field and ask questions that can help you better understand the material. Additionally, many online resources feature multimedia content like videos and audio recordings that can help enhance your understanding of a particular subject. Finally, some online reading materials feature interactive tools and activities that can help you practice and reinforce what you’ve learned. These tools can also be helpful for taking notes and keeping track of your progress.

Disadvantages of Online Reading

One of the major disadvantages of online reading is the lack of focus and concentration. When we read online, we’re often bombarded with ads, pop-ups, and other distractions. Studies have found that when people read on a screen they tend to skim more, which can lead to a less complete understanding of the material. Additionally, online readers tend to become easily distracted and lose focus if they are interrupted or need to switch tasks.

Furthermore, online readers may be prone to reading more quickly than they would offline. This is because they can scan the text and jump from one point to the next without taking the time to fully absorb the material. This can lead to a lack of comprehension and lead to misunderstandings of the material. Studies have also shown that when people read online, they often take shortcuts when searching for information and may not even read the entire text.

Finally, the quality of the material that you can access online is often questionable. With the prevalence of fake news and inaccurate information, it can be difficult to ascertain the reliability and accuracy of the content. Additionally, the quality of the visuals and graphics on a website can vary greatly, making it difficult to effectively learn from the material.

Advantages of Offline Reading

One of the biggest advantages of offline reading is that it is a more reliable and secure way to read. When you're reading offline, you don't need to worry about your connection dropping or your device experiencing a power outage. Plus, you don't need to worry about your data being stolen or hacked, as is possible with online reading.

Another advantage of offline reading is that it helps you to be more focused and engaged. When you're reading from a physical book or magazine, you can stay in the moment, undistracted by notifications or pop-ups from the internet. Furthermore, when you're reading from a physical book or magazine, you can take notes, highlight important passages, or even write down your own thoughts and insights.

Last but not least, offline reading can also help to reduce eye strain. When you read from a digital device, like a laptop or tablet, the blue light emitted by the screen can put a strain on your eyes. However, when you're reading from a physical book or magazine, you won't experience this same strain. In addition, offline reading can be a more pleasurable experience, as the feeling of flipping through a physical book or magazine can be calming.

Disadvantages of Offline Reading

One of the main disadvantages of offline reading is its lack of interactivity. Unlike online reading, which often has interactive elements like videos, quizzes, and games, offline reading is a much more passive activity. This can be a major limitation for learners who need more immediate feedback and reinforcement to fully understand and retain new information. Additionally, offline reading materials can quickly become outdated. As new information is published, reading material in books and magazines can quickly become obsolete, so readers must be sure to keep up with the latest information if they want to stay current.

Another disadvantage of offline reading is its limited scope. Because offline reading materials are often limited to one specific topic, it can be difficult for readers to get a broader understanding of a subject. Additionally, offline reading materials tend to be more expensive than online materials, so readers may be limited in the amount of content they can access. Finally, the material available in offline reading sources may be limited by the physical space, making it difficult to find the exact material needed.

Overall, while offline reading can be beneficial, its lack of interactivity, limited scope, and higher cost can make it less effective than online reading for many learners.

Research-Backed Insights on Online and Offline Reading

Online reading has become a popular way to access information in the digital age. It has made it easier and faster to find what you need quickly. Plus, reading online often offers more interactive elements such as videos, audio, and interactive quizzes. However, research has found that reading online is not always the best way to learn. Studies have shown that reading online can leave readers with shorter attention spans, are more easily distracted, and are likely to forget what they’ve read more quickly.

Offline reading has been around for centuries and remains an important way to learn. It has been proven to support more meaningful learning that sticks with readers longer. Physical books are more likely to be read in their entirety as well as be revisited at a later date. The tactile nature of physical books can also be more engaging and immersive.

Studies also show that the best way to learn is to use a combination of both online and offline reading. The combination of the two can be beneficial for information retention, comprehension, and overall learning. Using online sources to supplement physical books can help readers increase their knowledge and better understand the material. Additionally, online sources like videos and podcasts can provide a different perspective on the subject and can be used to further reinforce what has been learned.

Tips for Making the Most of Online and Offline Reading

Online reading has become increasingly popular in recent years, due to the vast amount of information available on the internet. However, the more traditional form of reading offline has been around for centuries, and both methods have their advantages and disadvantages when it comes to learning.

When reading online, the convenience and accessibility of the material is one major advantage. Moreover, the wealth of resources, multimedia content, and interactivity of the web make it easier to engage in the material and learn more effectively. On the other hand, offline reading can provide a more focused environment to read the material, and can also be easier on the eyes due to the lack of screens and computer monitors.

In order to make the most of either online or offline reading, it is important to create an effective learning environment. This means setting aside a quiet, distraction-free space where you can focus on the material, as well as setting a specific time each day for studying. Additionally, it can be helpful to break down the material into manageable chunks in order to ensure that you are comprehending the information and retaining it for later use. Finally, taking notes while reading can help you to easily review and recall the material at a later date.

Leave a comment

Please note, comments need to be approved before they are published.

  • Choosing a selection results in a full page refresh.

kindle unlimited

Is it better to read from books or online?

Online Reading or Offline? Which is Better for Learning? (2024)

Reading is undoubtedly the gateway to freedom. There has only been one avenue for reading for centuries: on paper. 

That said, the digital age has opened the floodgates to information via the internet, and with over 85.88% of the world’s population owning smartphones, online reading has become the new normal.

The recent pandemic also played a significant role in thrusting the education systems worldwide into the digital realm, and this move was made possible by digital publishing platforms like KITABOO .

Table of Contents:

I. Types of Reading – A Brief

II. Offline Reading

The Advantages of Offline Reading

The downsides of offline reading.

III. Online Reading

The Advantages of Reading Online

The downsides of online reading.

IV. Is it Better to Read from Books or Online?

Types of Reading - A Brief

Learning a language is the beginning of how we do almost everything in life. Even though reading comes third in terms of LSRW (listening, speaking, reading, and writing) skills when it comes to learning a new language, it gains the top spot in the field of education. 

Most people subconsciously approach reading in two primary ways depending on what they are reading, which are:

  • Extensive Reading : This technique is used when the reader is simply reading for pleasure. There is no aim or object here, and nor is the reader overly focused on the details. 
  • Scanning : This is a technique where one reads quickly through a large block of text while looking for specific keywords or pieces of information.  
  • Skimming : Skimming is a technique where you quickly glance through text to understand what the text is about. 
  • Critical Reading : Critical Reading is a technique used when a reader intends to understand the intricacies of what they are reading. This form of reading is all about analysis and is key to understanding complex information. 

With this in mind, let’s dive into the nitty-gritty of offline and online reading.

Offline Reading

Let’s start with the obvious. No matter how intertwined we get with technology, it can never replace offline reading, and this holds true, especially in the field of education.

Reading and writing are a critical part of education and will continue to be into the future. While the benefits of online reading do exceed offline reading, it still has a few aces up its sleeve.

Although considered old-fashioned, offline reading does boast numerous advantages, such as:

Well, It’s Offline

As the title suggests, good old-fashioned books do not require an internet connection. With this comes the added advantage of not having a device to read from that can be hacked, broken, or worry about running out of juice. 

Better Focus Due to Minimal Distractions

Offline reading allows for higher levels of focus due to minimal distractions.

With smart devices comes a barrage of notifications and the temptation to switch to a different app. When you sit with a book, you sit with the intention to read, and you automatically switch into an intensive reading mode. 

You Get a Break from Screens

Most of us spend a large chunk of our day staring at either a smartphone, a laptop, or a television. This can put a significant amount of strain on one’s eyes.

But when it comes to offline reading, as long as you are reading in a well-lit space, you don’t strain your eyes as much.

Offline reading does have its downsides as well, such as:

A Lack of Interactivity

Reading large chunks of text can get mundane over a period of time. Add to this the fact that not every one of us learns best from reading and you can see how its merits start to drop.

Statistics show that over 65% of the world’s population are visual learners. Relying only on offline learning leaves a large chunk of the student population at a disadvantage. 

Offline Reading is Limited in its Scope

Offline reading limits your scope of knowledge to the book or books you access to. Unless you have access to a library, getting more information solely from books can be an expensive endeavor. 

kitaboo webinar banner

Online Reading

Based on the current landscape, it is evident that digital content creation is going to continue into the foreseeable future.

Reading online has become the go-to option for getting the latest news, reading up on new topics, and, since the pandemic, a large part of the education system.

So, how does reading online fare in comparison to reading offline? Let’s find out.

Reading online has changed the very fabric of how we consume and interact with information. With it comes a host of advantages, such as:

Access to All the Information in the World

Online reading gives you access to infinite amounts of information, which, unlike offline reading materials, can be up to date with the latest information.

You no longer need to buy additional books or run to the library and spend additional time and effort looking for relevant sources of information. 

It is a Multimedia-Rich and Interactive Experience

Depending on the source material, online reading can be infused with multimedia-rich elements that are proven to make comprehension of complex topics easier and boost knowledge retention.

The interactive nature of these materials also leads to higher engagement levels in students. 

Improves Scanning and Skimming Reading Skills

Reading online is often visually cluttered, and readers scan and skim more often to find the necessary information.

As a result, readers tend to develop these skills quickly to identify the key information and ignore the less important details. 

Online Reading is More Accessible

Reading online transcends the limitation of offline reading when it comes to accessibility.

Whether a reader is visually impaired or hard of hearing, online reading has technology like Closed Captions and text-to-speech to overcome them.

Also Read: Best eBook Creation Softwares

As with everything, there is always a flip side, which also holds true for online reading. Here are the disadvantages of reading online: 

Too Many Distractions

Reading online does come with several distractions in the form of ads, pop-up notifications, and social media, to name a few, which can make it difficult to focus on the task at hand.

Due to this, studies have often found that readers then skim rather than critically read online. 

The Health Implications

Staring at a screen for extended periods puts a lot of strain on one’s eyes. You can also strain your neck or back if you do not maintain a proper posture while spending extended time on online devices.

Is it Better to Read from Books or Online?

Reading online does pull ahead of reading offline in terms of its benefits. However, there is no one correct answer to this question.

Instead of an either this or that approach, the best possible course of action is to maintain a careful balance of both, especially in the field of education. That way, readers reap both forms’ benefits while negating each other’s downsides. 

If you prefer reading online and want to help create or read eBooks, you should check out KITABOO. They are a cloud-based platform that supports various digital publishing needs. Explore now to know more.

Contact our expert team now and get started!

To know more, please write to us at [email protected] .

Suggested Reads:

  • Advantages of ePUB over PDF
  • Create a Custom eBook App to Reach a Wider Audience
  • What is eBook DRM and Why Do Publishers Need it?
  • Role of Technology in the Workplace
  • Best eBook Hosting Platforms for Digital Publishing
  • Create Effective Interactive Training Content
  • Evaluate Training Effectiveness and Impact

Discover How An Ebook Conversion, Publishing & Distribution Platform Can Help You

Kitaboo is a cloud-based content platform to create-publish & securely distribute interactive mobile-ready ebooks.

You May Also Like

interactive learning software

8 Top interactive learning software for online education (2024)

Digital Publishing , eBook solution / March 18, 2024

eLearning Training Solutions

Learning Management Systems: eLearning Training Solutions

Blog , Digital Publishing , eBook solution / March 10, 2024

editing epub

Epub Book: Best Practices in 2024

Blog , Digital Publishing , eBook solution , Education Technology / December 13, 2023

online reading vs offline reading essay

Mike Harman

Mike is the SVP Business Development at HurixDigital. He has over 30 years experience in achieving consistent top-line revenue growth and building mutually beneficial relationships

More Resources

  • Whitepapers
  • How To Guides
  • Product Videos
  • Infographics
  • Kitaboo FAQs

Request a Demo

An enterprise platform that 15 million users trust

Kitaboo Product Video

Recent Posts

How do I create an EPUB from a document?

How Do I Create an ePub From a Document?: A Step-by-Step Tutorial

Are digital textbooks worth it?

Are Digital Textbooks Worth It?

interactive content builder

Interactive Content Builder: Unleashing the Power of Engaging Content

epub-reader

The Best ePUB Readers for Windows (2024)

  • Digital Publishing
  • eBook solution
  • eBook Store
  • Education Technology
  • Employee Training
  • ePUB Conversion
  • Frankfuter Buchmesse
  • Nonprofit Organizations & Associations
  • Self-publishing
  • Uncategorized
  • XML Conversion

Get the latest posts delivered right to your email.

Sign up to Newsletter

Press & media.

  • Press Releases
  • News Section

Quick links

  • About Hurix Systems
  • KITABOO for K12 Publishers
  • KITABOO for Associations and Non-profit
  • KITABOO for Higher Education Publishers
  • Digital Content Solutions – HurixDigital
  • Terms and Conditions
  • Privacy Policy
  • Cookie Policy

Resources Links

  • Product videos
  • Kitaboo Partner Program

online reading vs offline reading essay

Kitaboo Reader

Hurix System' best in class interactive ebook reader application Kitaboo is now available on the Applie Itunes app store

  • Convert Fixed PDF / InDesign to Dynamic Content
  • Training Solutions
  • Online Reader
  • Android App
  • Windows Store Installer
  • Mac Store Installer
  • Kitaboo SDK
  • Case Studies
  • Request A Demo

Privacy Overview

It’s a wonderful world — and universe — out there.

Come explore with us!  

Science News Explores

Will you learn better from reading on screen or on paper.

One size doesn’t fit all situations. But for now, experts say, don’t throw away your books

a tween girl hand writes on notebook paper at a desk while reading a laptop screen

Computers are very much a part of education today. But books and paper are still a good way to learn information. Depending on the material, they can be the easiest way, studies find.

Carol Yepes/Moment/Getty Images Plus

Share this:

  • Google Classroom

By Avery Elizabeth Hurt

October 18, 2021 at 6:30 am

Want to know the current population of India? The internet is your best bet. Need a quick refresher on the phases of the moon ? Go ahead, read a story online (or two or three). But if you really need to learn something, you’re probably better off with print. Or at least that’s what a lot of research now suggests.

Many studies have shown that when people read on-screen, they don’t understand what they’ve read as well as when they read in print. Even worse, many don’t realize they’re not getting it. For example, researchers in Spain and Israel took a close look at 54 studies comparing digital and print reading. Their 2018 study involved more than 171,000 readers. Comprehension, they found, was better overall when people read print rather than digital texts. The researchers shared the results in Educational Research Review .

Patricia Alexander is a psychologist at the University of Maryland in College Park. She studies how we learn. Much of her research has delved into the differences between reading in print and on-screen. Alexander says students often think they learn more from reading online. When tested, though, it turns out that they actually learned less than when reading in print.

The question is: Why?

Educators and Parents, Sign Up for The Cheat Sheet

Weekly updates to help you use Science News Explores in the learning environment

Thank you for signing up!

There was a problem signing you up.

Reading is reading, right? Not exactly. Maryanne Wolf works at the University of California, Los Angeles. This neuroscientist specializes in how the brain reads. Reading is not natural, she explains. We learn to talk by listening to those around us. It’s pretty automatic. But learning to read takes real work. Wolf notes it’s because the brain has no special network of cells just for reading.

To understand text, the brain borrows networks that evolved to do other things. For example, the part that evolved to recognize faces is called into action to recognize letters. This is similar to how you might adapt a tool for some new use. For example, a coat hanger is great for putting your clothes in the closet. But if a blueberry rolls under the refrigerator, you might straighten out the coat hanger and use it to reach under the fridge and pull out the fruit. You’ve taken a tool made for one thing and adapted it for something new. That’s what the brain does when you read.

It’s great that the brain is so flexible. It’s one reason we can learn to do so many new things. But that flexibility can be a problem when it comes to reading different types of texts. When we read online, the brain creates a different set of connections between cells from the ones it uses for reading in print. It basically adapts the same tool again for the new task. This is like if you took a coat hanger and instead of straightening it out to fetch a blueberry, you twisted it into a hook to unclog a drain. Same original tool, two very different forms.

As a result, the brain might slip into skim mode when you’re reading on a screen. It may switch to deep-reading mode when you turn to print.

a teen girl scrolls down a smartphone screen

That doesn’t just depend on the device, however. It also depends on what you assume about the text. Naomi Baron calls this your mindset. Baron is a scientist who studies language and reading. She works at American University in Washington, D.C. Baron is the author of How We Read Now , a new book about digital reading and learning. She says one way mindset works is in anticipating how easy or hard we expect the reading to be. If we think it will be easy, we might not put in much effort.

Much of what we read on-screen tends to be text messages and social-media posts. They’re usually easy to understand. So, “when people read on-screen, they read faster,” says Alexander at the University of Maryland. “Their eyes scan the pages and the words faster than if they’re reading on a piece of paper.”

But when reading fast, we may not absorb all the ideas as well. That fast skimming, she says, can become a habit associated with reading on-screen. Imagine that you turn on your phone to read an assignment for school. Your brain might fire up the networks it uses for skimming quickly through TikTok posts. That’s not helpful if you’re trying to understand the themes in that classic book, To Kill a Mockingbird . It also won’t get you far if you’re preparing for a test on the periodic table .

Where was I?

Speed isn’t the only problem with reading on screens. There’s scrolling, too. When reading a printed page or even a whole book, you tend to know where you are. Not just where you are on some particular page, but which page — potentially out of many. You might, for instance, remember that the part in the story where the dog died was near the top of the page on the left side. You don’t have that sense of place when some enormously long page just scrolls past you. (Though some e-reading devices and apps do a pretty good job of simulating page turns.)

Why is a sense of page important? Researchers have shown that we tend to make mental maps when we learn something. Being able to “place” a fact somewhere on a mental map of the page helps us remember it.

It’s also a matter of mental effort. Scrolling down a page takes a lot more mental work than reading a page that’s not moving. Your eyes don’t just focus on the words. They also have to keep chasing the words as you scroll them down the page.

Mary Helen Immordino-Yang is a neuroscientist at the University of Southern California in Los Angeles. She studies how we read. When your mind has to keep up with scrolling down a page, she says, it doesn’t have a lot of resources left for understanding what you’re reading. This can be especially true if the passage you’re reading is long or complicated. While scrolling down a page, your brain has to continually account for the placement of words in your view. And this can make it harder for you to simultaneously understand the ideas those words should convey.

Alexander found that length matters, too. When passages are short, students understand just as much of what they read on-screen as do when reading in print. But once the passages are longer than 500 words, they learn more from print.

a boy lies on the couch while reading from a tablet screen

Even genre matters. Genre refers to what type of book or article you’re reading. The articles here on Science News for Students are nonfiction. News stories and articles about history are nonfiction. Stories invented by an author are fiction. The Harry Potter books are fiction, for example. So are Song for a Whale and A Wrinkle in Time .

In How We Read Now , Baron reviewed much of the research that’s been published about reading online. Most studies showed people understand nonfiction better when they read it in print. How it affects the understanding of fictional accounts is less clear.

Jenae Cohn works at California State University, Sacramento. Her work focuses on the use of technology in education. This past June, she published a book about digital reading: Skim, Dive, Surface . The biggest problem may not be the words on the screen, she finds. It’s the other things that pop up and get in the way of reading. It can be difficult to concentrate when something interrupts you every few minutes. She’s referring to pings and rings from texts or emails, pop-up advertisements and TikTok updates. All can quickly ruin concentration. Links and boxes that are meant to add to your understanding can be a problem, too. Even when they’re meant to be helpful, some can prove a distraction from what you’re reading.

Not all bad

If you want to do better in school (and who doesn’t?), it’s not quite as simple as turning off your tablet and picking up a book. There are plenty of good reasons to read on screens.

As the pandemic taught us, sometimes we have no choice. When libraries and bookstores close or it’s dangerous to visit them, digital reading can be a lifesaver. Expense is also an important factor. Digital books usually cost less than print ones. And, of course, you have to consider the environmental advantages of digital. It doesn’t take trees to make a digital book.

Digital reading has other advantages, too. In most cases, when you’re reading on-screen you can adjust the size of the letters. You also can change the background color and maybe the typeface. This is a huge help for people who don’t see well. It’s also useful for people with reading disabilities. People who have dyslexia, for instance, often find it easier to read material when it’s displayed in a typeface called Open Dyslexic . Computers, tablets and digital reading devices, such as Amazon’s Kindle, can offer this option. Many e-readers have apps that can be used on tablets, too. That makes it possible to get these advantages on a tablet or phone.

Reading online also allows editors to insert hyperlinks. These may help a reader dive deeper to understand a particular point or even just to learn the definition of a term that may be new or confusing.

a girl wearing overalls and glasses sits on a couch and reads on a tablet screen

Michelle Luhtala is a school librarian in New Canaan, Conn. She helps her school make the best use of digital material. She also trains teachers. Luhtala is not alarmed about digital reading. She points out that there are many ways to read on screens. Some e-textbooks and databases used in schools come with tools that make it easier, not harder, to learn, she says. Some e-books, for instance, let you highlight a passage. Then the computer will read it out loud. Other tools allow you to make notes about passages you’re reading and keep those notes after you’ve returned a book to the library. Most of these texts have pop-up definitions. Some link to maps, keywords and quizzes. Such tools can make digital material extremely useful, she argues.

Getting the most out of your digital reading

All experts agree on one thing: There’s no going back. Digital reading is here to stay. So it pays to make the most of it.

One obvious trick: Print anything that needs careful reading. You have this option when reading Science News for Students . (There’s a print icon at the top of every article.) But that may not be necessary. Other things can also make sure you retain the most from what you read on screens.

The most important thing, says Baron at American University, is to slow down. Again, this is about mindset. When you read something important, slow down and pay attention. “You can concentrate when you read digitally,” she says. But you have to make an effort. She suggests saying to yourself, “I’m going to take half an hour and just read. No text messages. No Instagram updates.” Turn off notifications on your phone or tablet. Only turn them back on when you’re done reading.

It’s also a good idea to do a little prep. Baron compares reading to sports or to playing music. “Watch a pianist or an athlete. Before they run the race or play the concerto, they get themselves in the zone,” she says. “It’s the same thing for reading. Before you read something you really want to focus on, get in the zone. Think about what you’ll be reading, and what you want to get from it.”

a boy sits at a bedroom desk with a book and laptop open in front of him

To really get the most from reading, Baron says, you have to engage with the words on the page. One great technique for this is making notes. You can write summaries of what you’ve read. You can make lists of key words. But one of the most useful ways to engage with what you’re reading is to ask questions. Argue with the author. If something doesn’t make sense, write down your question. You can look up the answer later. If you disagree, write down why. Make a good case for your point of view.

If you’re reading a print book, you can take notes on paper. If it’s a printout or if you own the book, you can write directly on the page. You can do this when you’re reading on your phone or tablet, too. Just keep a pad of paper handy while you read. Many apps also allow you to make virtual notes directly on a digital document, Luhtala points out. Some allow you to add virtual stickies. With some you can even write in the margins and turn down the corners of the virtual pages.

Like most things, what you get from reading on-screen depends on what you put into it. You don’t have to make a choice between print or digital. Alexander points out that when it comes to print versus digital, one is not better than the other. Both have their place. But they are different. So keep in mind that to learn well, how you interact with them may have to differ, too.

More Stories from Science News Explores on Tech

an illustration of a robot hand grabbing a small planet earth, the image has 1s and 0s throughout to illustrate an online/digital environment

How to design artificial intelligence that acts nice — and only nice

An illustration of a smiley face with a green monster with lots of tentacles and eyes behind it. The monster's fangs and tongue are visible at the bottom of the illustration.

‘Jailbreaks’ bring out the evil side of chatbots

an illustration of a robot hand holding a telephone reciever with a soundwave in the shape of a person coming out of the speaker

A new tool could guard against deepfake voice scams

an illustration of robotic hand using a human brain as a puppet on strings while inside a person's head

AI learned how to influence humans by watching a video game

a silver robotic hand with its finger open

Scientists Say: Bionic

a photo of Archax, a giant mecha

Could we build a mecha?

A person's hand is shown inserting a coin battery into a testing device.

Artificial intelligence helped design a new type of battery

a photo of a mahagony glider (a small fuzzy marsupial) clutching a branch

Analyze This: Marsupial gliders may avoid the ground to dodge predators

You can Choose category

The Difference Between Online Reading and Offline Reading

Introduction, the new literacies of online research and comprehension, developing students’ online reading comprehension, tpack framework, the use of tpac in planning lessons, my skill level in the tpac method.

With the integration of technology in education, there is a shift from offline reading to online reading. Based on the definition, offline reading occurs through printed texts, while online reading happens through screens and electronic texts, especially web-based reading materials (Dwyer, 2016). Unlike offline reading, where text is bound within the covers of a book, an online reader has the mandate of assembling potential text for reading. As a result, reading online requires the readers to deepen their skills to navigate, locate and synthesize the information presented through various websites. In addition, online readers must have computer skills to navigate through the internet and obtain the required texts (Dwyer, 2016). Therefore, contrary to offline reading, online reading depends entirely on information technology.

Compared with online reading, offline reading is better for focus and memory retention. Offline reading is free from distractions from online ads that may take the reader’s attention. It offers more fixity and solidity to the reader’s sense of the progress of the text (Dwyer, 2016). This is an indication that the reader has the ability to concentrate when reading. However, online reading exposes an individual to several distractions. A digital text reader can easily shift from reading to other web pages such as Facebook, YouTube, and others. With this distraction, it is challenging for an online reader to focus on the text. As a result, students have more focus when reading printed texts than when reading digital texts.

Teachers play an integral role in developing online reading comprehension and internet research skills. To achieve this, a teacher can use socially mediated experiences where they directly interact with students. This will assist a teacher in creating a positive relationship with learners. The second approach to teaching the concept is using an internet workshop (Zhang et al., 2011). The model is important because it introduces students to internet sites and develops crucial background knowledge. It involves activities such as locating a site, creating tasks requiring the students to use the site, assigning the tasks, and conducting a workshop. The third approach that a teacher can use is internet inquiry. In this case, students examine the information and prepare a report. Therefore, teachers should use models such as socially mediated, internet workshop, and internet inquiry to equip the students with online reading comprehension skills.

TPACK is an acronym for technological pedagogical content and knowledge. The framework provides a new model for integrating technology into education and how to offer the best educational experience for students. In addition, it was designed to illustrate a set of knowledge that educators need to teach their learners, teach efficiently and use technology (Koehler et al., 2013). The TPACK framework strives to enhance this study and scholarly heritage by incorporating technology into the types of knowledge that teachers must examine when teaching. The framework plays an important role in facilitating effective teaching. For example, with the advancement in technology, an educator is supposed to integrate technological concepts when teaching students (Koehler et al., 2013). As a result, the framework aims to aid in the development of improved tools for identifying and documenting how technology-related professional knowledge is implemented and instantiated in practice.

The three cores of TPACK are content, pedagogy, and technology. Content knowledge refers to the teacher’s knowledge of the subject matter. For example, this knowledge includes scientific facts, theories, and evidence-based reasoning. Pedagogy knowledge is an educator’s deep knowledge of the operations of teaching and learning (Koehler et al., 2013). For instance, it includes designing engaging classroom environments that allow maximum student activity and self-regulation. Technological knowledge is the educators’ knowledge of or ability to use different technical tools. For example, a teacher’s ability to use the internet or smart boards as tools for learning. Therefore, the main components of the model are content, pedagogy, and technological knowledge.

TPAC plays an integral role in developing a lesson plan because it provides the main aspects that should be included in the plan. The three primary components of the model; content, pedagogy, and technology, help design a teaching plan (Koehler et al., 2013). Firstly, an educator must create content that meets learners’ needs. The content is the core of the lesson plan because it shows the concepts that it explores. Secondly, the plan should incorporate the pedagogical knowledge of teaching (Koehler et al., 2013). For example, some of the methods used for new concepts are experiential and inquiry-based learning. Experiential learning allows a learner to explore and practice. Thirdly, the lesson plan should integrate technology, especially in the contemporary environment. For instance, an educator can consider using google meet and other technologies to conduct online learning.

My skill level in the TPAC model has increased tremendously after reading the texts. I discovered that TPAC is an important part of the education system because it incorporates the growing demand for technology, a good understanding of teaching practices, and the creation of effective content (Koehler et al., 2013). It forms the efficiency in the delivery of the lesson plan with the integration of technology. It is an application in all aspects of learning, which are integral in teaching and learning. I now understand that TPAC is a model that assists educators to contemplate on their learning domains interconnect to effectively teach and engage learners with technology (Koehler et al., 2013). It offers educators an opportunity to develop professional development and competency. For example, there is a need for educators to integrate technology into teaching. Therefore, I now have the ability to incorporate the TPAC model into learning.

I have strengths and weaknesses in using the TPAC model in teaching and learning. Based on my strength, I can apply the three components of the framework in developing an effective lesson plan. It allows teachers to analyze and reflect on their practices and how technology is integrated into the class. In addition, through the model, I now understand the intersection between content, pedagogy, and technology. It promotes contemporary learning as I recognize the importance of technology in teaching and learning. In terms of weaknesses, I feel that the meaning of various knowledge domains is inadequate. I believe that more can be done to describe the domains effectively. The model has strengths and weaknesses that should be considered during application.

To increase my command of the TPAC model, I will focus on increasing my knowledge. Firstly, I collect reliable and peer-reviewed sources that address the model from various sources. Then, I will read and analyze the contents and identify shortcomings. Secondly, I will conduct research to address the problems identified and questions derived from the reading. Based on this, I will be able to improve my understanding of the TPAC model.

Dwyer, B. (2016). Engaging all students in internet research and inquiry. The Reading Teacher , 69 (4), 383-389.

Koehler, M. J., Mishra, P., & Cain, W. (2013). What is technological pedagogical content knowledge (TPACK)? Journal of education , 193 (3), 13-19.

Zhang, S., Duke, N. K., & Jiménez, L. M. (2011). The WWWDOT approach to improving students’ critical evaluation of websites. The Reading Teacher , 65 (2), 150-158.

  • Submit A Post
  • EdTech Trainers and Consultants
  • Your Campus EdTech
  • Your EdTech Product
  • Your Feedback
  • Your Love for Us
  • EdTech Product Reviews

ETR Resources

  • Mission/Vision
  • Testimonials
  • Our Clients
  • Press Release

Online vs Offline Reading

Sudha

Language development starts with the ‘LSRW’ skill development. 

Reading, though is third in the row plays a vital role in learning.  It is not only required to concentrate on vocabulary development in languages but also for developing creative thinking.  Reading closes windows and opens doors of wisdom.  This activity is considered the base of revolutions around the world.  Ideas formed and expressed while writing, when transforms, influences other minds to become ‘ enlightened ’.

Reading took a new dimension during industrial revolution with the advent of ‘ printed books ’.  This was an activity which when cultivated among the intellects, changed the face of the world.  Today, reading has become more than a medium of expression.  Globalization has changed the very outlook of this skill as – knowledge, applicable and building a digitally aware citizen.  Reading is more online rather than offline.  Era of sending letters and postcards has changed to exchanging emails and online greeting cards.  Chat rooms, forum discussions, blogs are sources where people can upload and connect with the like-minded.

In this fast changing scenario, what change has happened in the field of Reading?

Online Reading has taken prominence, let’s take a quick look at the differences between online and offline reading.

Offline reading means carry the book on move.  It becomes difficult as it is both bulky and time consuming.  A book often does not give meanings of basal words for which it is not recommended.  Example: A book of chemistry, may carry the word ‘statistically’ but no emphasize of what statistics means would be provided.  The reader needs a dictionary to look up into the meaning and apply to the context, which is again time consuming.  Offline reading is directional and may have only one topic to move forward.  References provided need another book or article to be read to understand the base of the material.

Online reading can be called as multidimensional.  Words and phrases which are difficult can be searched either through hyperlinks provided by the author or in the very next tab opened besides the reading article.  Online reading is becoming more operative due to the very ease of carrying.  Space and storage has shrunk, and a library load of books can be accommodated in a mobile phone or a USB. 

The major advantage of online reading is in the availability of multitude of media plugins along the reading material.  Articles embedded with podcasts, videos give the reader an experience that helps them reinforce their reading.  Online reading facilitates ‘zoom’ option for better visual experience. 

All said the reader needs to understand the basic necessities of online reading.  The major problem countries are facing are due to cutting down of trees.  One of the prime purposes to cut down trees is to make paper.  Use of e-paper will help us build a sustainable future.  Schools have already started their focus in this direction by giving tablets to their students rather than text books.  Students can use mobile apps of text books which help them visualise the content of the material and allow them to look into diverse features of the same material.  Understanding the very fact that opening the internet needs to done ‘ with care ’, avoidance may not be the best resolution.  ‘ Kindling thought process ’ and ‘ making them aware ’ are the only options available, since we cannot stop the fast moving digital natives.

Student Scholarship LOreal India For Young Women in Science Scholarship 2015

Latest EdTech News To Your Inbox

Stay connected.

online reading vs offline reading essay

Sign in to your account

Username or Email Address

Remember Me

  • Literary Events
  • Recommendations
  • How To Publish

online reading vs offline reading essay

Online Reading Vs Traditional Reading In Today’s World

Online reading has been growing more popular each year. This trend has sparked a debate between online reading and traditional reading. For anyone who hasn’t picked a side yet, we are going to help you make your mind!

What is Online Reading?

Online reading is when you take information or text from a digital format. It is also called digital reading. PDF files, EBooks, audiobooks, blogs, etc. are all considered to be online reading, whether it’s through a PC or a phone. There are many advantages of online reading which include:

Advantages of Online Reading

1. it’s fast paced.

In today’s quick world, online reading makes it easy to look for books. You don’t need to plan a whole trip to the bookstore to stock up on your books. You can simply download your book. Even if you’re busy with work, you can save the book on a click!

2. It is More Versatile

Generally, if you download a book, it would be on your phone. And you take your phone everywhere. It doesn’t get easier than that. You can highlight important lines through the tools available on your laptop or even learn meanings of difficult words through the internet just in one click.

3. Easy To Understand

Sometimes while reading you get stuck on a word or a phrase you don’t really understand. Or sometimes you miss a point and get confused. Online reading provides the readers with hyperlinks which provides ease to the reader and reduces the chances of any confusion.

Disadvantages of Online Reading

Unfortunately nothing in the world comes with only advantages. There are certain disadvantages to online reading which include:

1. Attention is Diverted

While reading a book your eyes and mind are focused on the words and read them in the sequence they’re written. But while reading something online, your eyes keep wandering around the screen and you often skip over parts and details of the book.

2. Needs Power

It doesn’t matter if you read on your PC, phone or kindle, anything electronic will run out of power and you’ll need to charge it which can be annoying at times.

3. Loss of Details

Since you can’t give your full attention to the content on the screen, you’re more likely to loss information and details or wouldn’t be able to understand the ongoing situation in the book.

What Is Traditional Reading?

The process of getting information or text from a print format is called traditional reading . Reading books, magazines, newspapers, dictionaries, etc. is all considered to be traditional reading. Traditional readers find it hard to adap to newer ways of Online Reading. Book collectors and people who regularly visit traditional libraries also stick to their habits of reading printed books.

Advantages of Traditional Reading

1. linear reading.

When you’re reading something that is in your hand, it grasps your full attention and you read it in the sequence that it’s written. This gives you a better understanding of the book.

2. Does Not Need Power

You don’t need any power or charging for print formats. So if you’re reading the plot twist of the book and are gripped with suspense, you won’t have to worry about the battery dying.

3. Satisfaction

The satisfying feeling of buying your book, holding it, reading it, understanding it, sympathizing with the characters, going through the suspense and then finally finishing it can never be digitalized.

Disadvantages of Traditional Reading

1. time consuming.

Reading something physically requires at least a few hours to finish and sadly you can’t really multitask while reading either. It requires your full attention and time which is hard to give in today’s fast world.

2. Less Versatile

Books come in all sizes but mostly they aren’t small enough to carry around in a pocket. Carrying huge volumes of books can cause an inconvenience.

3. Take Up Space

Books usually take up a lot of space and can get damages if kept in an unfavourable environment.

Research shows that kids prefer reading online as it gives them more privacy. It also allows them to access more material online, reading in the dark and multitask.

But on the other hand, it has also impacted their patience. Children are getting more and more impatient. Online Reading has also decreased their attention span, since there are more distractions online than in reality. Online Reading has increased the number of eyestrains being caused by digital devices. Also, rather than reading from beginning to end, online readers increase their ability to skim through the pages and just search for keywords and phrases.

Traditional reading on the other hand encourages readers to do a more in-depth analysis. Research shows that since reading online makes us want to avoid long material and read just enough to understand.

So here was a list of advantages and disadvantages of both mediums. To be honest, both of them are already established in our daily lives to an extent. Both of them have certain advantages which the other doesn’t so it’s not an either or question, it more about how can we use both of them to the fullest to get the best out of them!

So head on to meraqissa and choose an eBook or order a printed copy today to vote for the medium you prefer!

  • #Onlinereading
  • #Publishing

' src=

Related Articles

Daastan published authors at pre-book conference at umt ila, leave a reply cancel reply.

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Top Book Publishers of Pakistan

Top 10 book publishing companies in 2024, stay in touch, follow our instagram.

online reading vs offline reading essay

  • Publish Books
  • Miscellaneous

Want to study better? Don’t ditch your books for online study materials

During the pandemic, many college professors abandoned assignments from printed textbooks and turned instead to digital texts or multimedia coursework.

As a professor of linguistics , I have been studying how electronic communication compares to traditional print when it comes to learning. Is comprehension the same whether a person reads a text onscreen or on paper? And are listening and viewing content as effective as reading the written word when covering the same material?

The answers to both questions are often “no,” as I discuss in my book “ How We Read Now ,” released in March 2021. The reasons relate to a variety of factors, including diminished concentration, an entertainment mindset and a tendency to multitask while consuming digital content.

Studies have shown that students do more mind-wandering when listening to audio than when reading. Source: Cindy Ord/Getty Images/AFP

Print versus digital reading

When reading texts of several hundred words or more, learning is generally more successful when it’s on paper than onscreen. A cascade of research confirms this finding.

The benefits of print particularly shine through when experimenters move from posing simple tasks – like identifying the main idea in a reading passage – to ones that require mental abstraction – such as drawing inferences from a text. Print reading also improves the likelihood of recalling details – like “What was the colour of the actor’s hair?” – and remembering where in a story events occurred – “Did the accident happen before or after the political coup?”

Studies show that both grade school students and college students assume they’ll get higher scores on a comprehension test if they have done the reading digitally. And yet, they actually score higher when they have read the material in print before being tested.

Educators need to be aware that the method used for standardised testing can affect results. Studies of Norwegian tenth graders and US third through eighth graders report higher scores when standardised tests were administered using paper. In the US study, the negative effects of digital testing were strongest among students with low reading achievement scores, English language learners and special education students.

My own research and that of colleagues approached the question differently. Rather than having students read and take a test, we asked how they perceived their overall learning when they used print or digital reading materials. Both high school and college students overwhelmingly judged reading on paper as better for concentration, learning and remembering than reading digitally.

The discrepancies between print and digital results are partly related to paper’s physical properties. With paper, there is a literal laying on of hands, along with the visual geography of distinct pages. People often link their memory of what they’ve read to how far into the book it was or where it was on the page.

But equally important is mental perspective, and what reading researchers call a “ shallowing hypothesis .” According to this theory, people approach digital texts with a mindset suited to casual social media, and devote less mental effort than when they are reading print.

Podcasts and online video

Given the increased use of flipped classrooms – where students listen to or view lecture content before coming to class – along with more publicly available podcasts and online video content, many school assignments that previously entailed reading have been replaced with listening or viewing. These substitutions have accelerated during the pandemic and moved to virtual learning.

Surveying US and Norwegian university faculty in 2019, University of Stavanger Professor Anne Mangen and I found that 32% of US faculty were now replacing texts with video materials, and 15% reported doing so with audio. The numbers were somewhat lower in Norway. But in both countries, 40% of respondents who had changed their course requirements over the past five to 10 years reported assigning less reading today.

A primary reason for the shift to audio and video is students refusing to do assigned reading. While the problem is hardly new , a 2015 study of more than 18,000 college seniors found only 21% usually completed all their assigned course reading.

Audio and video can feel more engaging than text, and so faculty increasingly resort to these technologies – say, assigning a TED talk instead of an article by the same person.

Maximising mental focus

Psychologists have demonstrated that when adults read news stories or transcripts of fiction , they remember more of the content than if they listen to identical pieces.

Researchers found similar results with university students reading an article versus listening to a podcast of the text. A related study confirms that students do more mind-wandering when listening to audio than when reading.

Results with younger students are similar, but with a twist. A study in Cyprus concluded that the relationship between listening and reading skills flips as children become more fluent readers. While second graders had better comprehension with listening, eighth graders showed better comprehension when reading.

Research on learning from video versus text echoes what we see with audio. For example, researchers in Spain found that fourth through sixth graders who read texts showed far more mental integration of the material than those watching videos. The authors suspect that students “read” the videos more superficially because they associate video with entertainment, not learning.

The collective research shows that digital media have common features and user practices that can constrain learning. These include diminished concentration, an entertainment mindset, a propensity to multitask, lack of a fixed physical reference point, reduced use of annotation and less frequent reviewing of what has been read, heard or viewed.

Digital texts, audio and video all have educational roles, especially when providing resources not available in print. However, for maximising learning where mental focus and reflection are called for, educators – and parents – shouldn’t assume all media are the same, even when they contain identical words.

Naomi S. Baron , Professor of Linguistics Emerita, American University

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article .

Popular stories

The most prestigious canadian universities in 2024, you’re doing resumes the hard way: 10 best resume-maker apps that are free, fast and easy to use, no coding skills, no problem: these high-paying jobs in ai welcome everyone, the most affordable canadian universities in 2024 that won’t break the bank, can bionic reading make you a speed reader not so fast, why are certain genres of music more effective in helping you study than others, important lessons from 'atomic habits' students can draw from.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Online and Offline Reading Comprehension

Profile image of Carlo Soberano

Modern perspectives on the innovation of teaching pedagogies, language assessments, theories and practice were introduced. However, limited attention was given to ESL users' modality of reading comprehension and preference. This empirical study explores the difference between Online and Offline Reading Comprehension of Filipino Grade Eleven students based on their raw scores. The study sought to answer the question: Is there a significant difference between Online and Offline Reading Comprehension of participants? Mixed methods were employed to strengthen the validity and reliability of the study. Statistical analysis was initially conducted followed by observation and focus group discussion. New Literacies and online-based language assessments are primary concerns that opens for further researches.

Related Papers

International Journal of Applied Linguistics & English Literature

Chingyi Tien

online reading vs offline reading essay

English Language Teaching

Abdul Rashid Mohamed

The purpose of the study is to have a relook at the ESL reading comprehension assessment system for Malaysian Year Five students. Traditionally, the ESL teachers have been assessing and reporting on their primary year’s students by merely giving a composite grade with some vague remarks. This process has been used and is still being employed in spite of the numerous advances and progress that have been made in the realm of education. To gauge the students’ reading ability there is a need to take a serious look into the way teachers assess the students. In this ESL reading comprehension assessment system, a set of standardised generic reading comprehension test, a reading matrix and reading performance descriptors were developed. The findings revealed that Year Five respondents at reading performance Band 1, Band 2, Band 3, Band 4 and Band 5 have acquired the literal, reorganization and inferential reading sub-skills to a certain extent. The results obtained were found to be consiste...

This study was conducted to systematically track and benchmark upper primary school students " ESL reading comprehension ability and subsequently generate data at the micro and macro levels according to individual achievement, school location, gender and ethnicity at the school, district, state and national levels. The main intention of this initiative was to provide information to assist ESL teachers about their students " reading ability and to determine students' reading comprehension performance standards. The auto generated data is expected to facilitate classroom instructional process without necessitating teachers to prepare test materials or manage data of their students " reading comprehension track records. The respondents were 1,514 Year 5 students from urban and rural schools from a district in northern Malaysia. The idea was conceptualised through a series of tests and development of the Reading Evaluation and Decoding System (READS) for Primary Schools. The findings indicated that majority of the respondents were " below standard " and " at academic warning ". We believe the generated data can assist the Ministry of Education to develop better quality instructional processes that are evidence based with a more focused reading instruction and reading material to tailor to the needs of students.

Fabio Arismendi

Few studies in Colombia have explored and compared students’ reading comprehension processes in EFL, in different modalities of instruction. This article reports on some findings of a larger study in which two groups of graduate Law students took a reading comprehension course in English, delivered in two different modalities of instruction: face-to-face and web-based. Both courses were served by an English teacher from the School of Languages at Universidad de Antioquia. The data gathered from class observations, in-depth interviews, questionnaires, tests, the teacher’s journal and data records in the platform provided insights about the students’ use of reading and language learning strategies in both modalities. Findings suggest that students applied the reading strategies explicitly taught during the courses and some language learning strategies for which they did not receive any instruction.

International Journal of Social Science Research

Andang Suhendi

Reading is an important language skill for second-language learners. Effective reading is essential for undergraduates to understand academic texts and become critical thinkers in identifying and evaluating opinions and arguments. This study examines students' achievement in reading tests conducted in three different modes: physical face-to-face, online and blended learning platforms. The selection of respondents was made using purposive sampling. The data for this study consist of secondary data obtained from 392 participants from three different faculties in a local university. The data were analyzed using the SPSS version 20 tool. The results indicate significant differences in teaching and learning modes and students' achievement in reading tests. The findings show that physical face-to-face mode produced the highest test scores, while blended learning produced the lowest test scores. Next, the findings also showed that girls performed better in reading tests than boys. ...

Hamid Mojarrad

Julie Coiro

This mixed-method study investigated the extent to which new reading comprehension proficiencies may be required on the Internet. It also explored the nature of online reading comprehension among adolescent readers who read online at different levels of proficiency. Results of a hierarchical regression analysis of data from a stratified random sample of 109 seventh graders indicated performance on one measure of online reading comprehension ability accounted for a significant amount of the variance in performance on a second measure of online reading comprehension ability over and above offline reading comprehension ability and a measure of topic-specific prior knowledge. Further, a developmental, contrastive case study analysis of retrospective think-aloud protocol data obtained from three purposefully selected focal students revealed a developmental progression of online reading skills and strategies that appeared to distinguish three diverse readers’ performance within six observed phases of online reading comprehension.

UAD TEFL International Conference

Indah Fajar

This study focused on reading comprehension ability of the English Department students in answering READS, MCQ questions. READS is an alternative standardized assessment system that is capable of measuring reading comprehension ability. This is a descriptive study based on Barrett's Taxonomy levels, such as literal comprehension, reorganization, and inferential comprehension. This study consisted of 122 students of the English Department academic year 2015 through 2018 at one of the private universities in Yogyakarta, Indonesia. This study analysis utilized the three Barrett's taxonomy scales to identify whether the students were at meet the standard, below standard, above standard or at academic warning. Finding of the research indicated that 27.8 % students categorized into academic warning (Band 2 and 3), 46.7% students categorized into below standard (Band 4) and 25.5% students categorized into meet standard (Band 5). Consequently, READS as an alternative assessment will...

The 6th International Conference on Multidisciplinary Research (ICMR) 2023

Septhia Irnanda

This study aims to identify both linguistic and non-linguistic reading comprehension problems during online learning. This qualitative study used a descriptive design focusing on 18 of the fifth and seventh semester students from the English Education Department in the Teacher Training and Education Faculty in Serambi Mekah University. The instrument used in this study was a questionnaire. The data was analyzed by using qualitative analysis to explore the students' reading comprehension difficulties during online learning. The results indicated that the students experienced the most reading problems in the areas of facilities (69.46%). This means the participants considered it as the most frequent problems that interfered with their reading comprehension during online learning

Psychology and Education: A Multidisciplinary Journal

Psychology and Education , Shara Jeane D. Niala

The purpose of this study was to find out whether Project e-Localized (eBooks and Printed localized reading materials) will enhance the level of reading comprehension in Filipino among Grade 3 pupils section Masunurin of Villa Perez Elementary School. This study utilized the quantitative method of research using a quasiexperimental design. The researcher statistically analyzed the pre-test and a post-test result to determine the significant difference in the level of reading comprehension in Filipino before and after the utilization of the Project e-Localized. Purposive sampling was used by the researcher composing of thirty-eight (38) Grade 3 pupils as the respondents. The statistical tools used were Mean Percentage Score and T-test. The study showed that after implementing Project e-Localized, the pretest MPS 57.37% with the descriptive analysis of "good" increased from the posttest resulted to 84.61%, with the descriptive analysis of "excellent" given the difference of 27.24 % and the value of the t-test is 22.9236 which is greater than t-Critical value of 2.0262, with 37 degrees of freedom at 0.05 level of significance. It is evident that there is a significant difference; therefore, the null hypothesis is rejected. Thus, the utilization of Project e-Localized wherein the researcher used electronic books and printed localized materials greatly enhanced the level of comprehension in Filipino of Grade Three pupils. Teachers must keep in mind that enhancing reading comprehension takes time and effort. It is educators' duties and responsibilities to enhance the level of comprehension of every pupil.

RELATED PAPERS

Gerardo Dominguez

Thierry Van Hees

Dr. Bhupendra Prajapati

Marcello Sarini

Forestry in the Midst of Global Changes

Adrina C . Bardekjian

Plant Physiology

S. Somerville

Mongolian Journal of Agricultural Sciences

Davaasuren Nergui

Revista médica de Chile

Alejandro de Marinis

Shyam Singh

dimas doyie

Michael Smart

Metabolites

Rosa Perestrelo

Nature Biotechnology

NAIMUL ISLAM

SQUALEN, Bulletin of Marine and Fisheries Postharvest and Biotechnology

Gemilang Rahmadara

Ensayos sobre Política Económica

Rolando Martinez

International Journal of Hyperthermia

ananda kumar

Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología

Francesc Formiga

Journal of Surgical Technique and Case Report

NASIRU ISMAIL

Springer eBooks

Corinne Young

Behavioral and Neural Biology

Lawrence Wichlinski

UCLA文凭证书 加州大学洛杉矶分校文凭证书 klhjkgh

Jurnal Komunikasi

Urip Tri Wijayanti

Applied Surface Science

Cristián Mauricio Covarrubias

The Handbook of Environmental Chemistry

Antoine Bret

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Psicol Reflex Crit
  • v.35; 2022 Dec

Reading digital- versus print-easy texts: a study with university students who prefer digital sources

Noemí bresó-grancha.

1 Escuela de Doctorado, Universidad Católica de Valencia San Vicente Mártir, San Agustín 3, Esc. A, Entresuelo 1, 46002 València, Spain

María José Jorques-Infante

2 MEB lab, Faculty of Psychology|, Universidad Católica de Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Avenida de la Ilustración, 4, Burjassot, Valencia, 46100 Spain

Carmen Moret-Tatay

3 Dipartimento di Neuroscienze Salute Mentale e Organi di Senso (NESMOS), La Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy

The transition from on-paper to on-screen reading seems to make it necessary to raise some considerations, as a greater attentional effort has been claimed for print texts than digital ones. Not surprisingly, most university students prefer this digital medium. This research aims to examine reading times by contextualizing this phenomenon into two processes: namely, word recognition and reading comprehension task on paper and on screen. Thus, two different tasks—counterbalanced into digital and print mediums—were carried out per each participant with a preference for a digital medium: a reading comprehension task (RCT) and a lexical decision task (LDT) after reading a specific story. Participants were slower reading print texts and no statistically significant differences were found in RCT accuracy. This result suggests that the task required more cognitive resources under the print medium for those with a worse comprehension performance in reading, and a more conservative pattern in digital RCT for those with a better performance.

Introduction

Reading has different effects on the brain, is considered as an activity that can reduce stress (Corazon et al., 2010 ), improves people’s memories (Peng et al., 2018 ), and enhances empathic skills (Gabay, Dundas, Plaut, & Behrmann, 2017 ; Kuzmičová, Mangen, Støle, & Begnum, 2017 ; Mangen, 2016 ), among others. This process has also been linked to longer life spans (Chang, Wu, & Hsiung, 2020 ; Peng et al., 2018 ), but, surprisingly for most, it is not innate. It must be learned through specific exercises for this purpose, usually in our childhood. In this way, networks of connections are developed through an architecture which is already used for recognizing visual patterns and understanding spoken language (Vogel et al., 2013 ). This architecture has been mainly addressed by studying response times (RTs) (Luce, 1986 ) and the specificity of reading-related regions through fMRI (Vogel et al., 2013 ).

When describing the reading process, an abstraction is developed in the fusiform gyrus that allows our brain to recognize strings of letters in milliseconds. This occurs even if stimuli are presented in different typographies, sizes, or even upper or lower cases, among others (Perea, Moret-Tatay, & Panadero, 2011 ). The changing nature and circumstances of reading, as digitization is growing, is a subject of debate that might influence our reading process. Literature has suggested a greater attentional effort for print texts than for digital texts (Mangen & Kuiken, 2014 ). While some studies stipulate higher reading comprehension on paper (Kim & Kim, 2013 ; Mangen, Walgermo, & Brønnick, 2013 ), others find no such differences between mediums (Porion, Aparicio, Megalakaki, Robert, & Baccino, 2016 ; Rockinson- Szapkiw, Courduff, Carter, & Bennett, 2013 ). These differences on results might be explained through the effect of modulating variables (Delgado, Vargas, Ackerman, & Salmerón, 2018 ) such as time constraints, genre or type of text, and temporal moment. Moreover, a piece of research (Mangen, Olivier, & Velay, 2019 ) stands out by assessing this variable in Kindle DX, as a digital medium, and print. Participants were assessed in their reconstruction of the story from both mediums. The authors concluded that kinesthetic feedback was less informative in a Kindle. Another study comparing print, e-reader, and a tablet computer by combining EEG and eye-tracking measures showed shorter mean fixation durations and lower EEG theta band voltage density in the digital medium for older participants (Kretzschmar et al., 2013 ). However, comprehension accuracy did not differ between mediums.

Comparisons in this front have also been addressed from fields such as ergonomics and perception (Benedetto, Drai-Zerbib, Pedrotti, Tissier, & Baccino, 2013 , 2014 ). It should be noted that the physical interaction that occurs while reading on paper or on screen is significantly different. Actions like turning a page or feeling the paper of a book produce a multisensory experience that increases the cognitive, affective, and emotional insertion in the subject matter (Jacobs, 2015 ; Kuzmičová et al., 2017 ; Mangen et al., 2019 ). In this context, a shallowing hypothesis is theorized, which tries to explain how recent media technologies might lead to a decline in reflective thought and an increase in superficial learning, as an immediate reward is expected (Annisette & Lafreniere, 2017 ). Given this change in pattern, could decoding be different from one medium to the other, being more superficial in digital, and thus affecting comprehension?

Classical authors such as Eldredge ( 1988 ) claimed that repeated exposure to frequently words in print mediums is likely to improve learners’ visual recognition of those words, which in turn is also likely to improve reading comprehension. Not surprisingly, this is an accepted strategy in fluency interventions (Brown et al., 2018 ). One of the main classical models on text comprehension defines this process as the result of the interaction of text features and readers' knowledge, involving variables such as propositional representation and readers' prior knowledge (Hsu, Clariana, Schloss, & Li, 2019 ; McNamara & Kintsch, 1996 ). In this way, familiarity with the medium is a variable of interest, and differences can be stipulated between samples that are more familiar with the digital versus print environment. On the other hand, considering the simple view of reading (Hoover & Gough, 1990 ) as one of the most influential approaches which has been developed to address early reading comprehension, reading must be addressed from a cognitive perspective. More precisely, this cognitive model of reading comprehension (RC) stipulates that RC is a consequence of the interaction between decoding and linguistic comprehension, where word recognition can be considered part of the decoding process (de Oliveira, da Silva, Dias, Seabra, & Macedo, 2014 ; Kirby & Savage, 2008 ).

According to the literature, the most commonly employed cognitive tasks involved in printed word identification have been lexical decision (LDT) and naming (Imbir et al., 2020 ; Katz et al., 2012 ; Navarro-Pardo, Navarro-Prados, Gamermann, & Moret-Tatay, 2013 ), bearing in mind that in both tasks RTs and accuracy are the main dependent variables. Moreover, one of the dependent variables that RC and lexical decision tasks have in common is the analysis of reaction time or response latency. Within this field, processing speed has been described as an indicator of reading performance in groups such as participants with dyslexia (Norton & Wolf, 2012 ), and is considered a reflection of brain architecture (Luce, 1986 ; Moret-Tatay, Gamermann, Navarro-Pardo, de Córdoba, & Castellá, 2018 ). Some studies seem to indicate that readers in digital media spend less time than in printed texts; however, their understanding may also be affected (Ackerman & Lauterman, 2012 ). Therefore, the literature has stipulated that readers may overestimate their understanding of digital texts.

According to the literature, a decline of long-form reading in higher education is diminishing (Baron & Mangen, 2021 ). In this way, university students seem to prefer, or more frequently used, digital sources than print ones for short times of reading (Terra, 2015 ). Differences between mediums, which were previously described, might be of interest for to be addressed, particularly for short texts. For this reason, this research aims to examine the differences between digital and print easy-texts reading in University students who prefer digital sources. We hypothesize that university students with a preference for digital texts have lower reading latency for simple digital texts than print ones. In addition, we expect, in this profile of participants, shorter lexical decision latencies in digital versus print easy-texts. If comprehension is not compromised in easy texts, previous literature might reflect a cost optimization, which becomes more controversial in long texts, as previous literature has shown. A RC and word recognition across mediums were selected for this research question. Moreover, if word recognition is considered directly linked to RC, it is expected a similar pattern for both processes in terms of speed processing. Lastly, a Bayesian approach was considered as an alternative strategy to support traditional analysis as described in prior literature (Nuzzo, 2014 ; Puga, Krzywinski, & Altman, 2015 ).

Materials and methods

Participants.

A sample of 40 university students (25 women and 15 men with an average age of 18.90 years and SD= 1.51), with no history or evidence of neurological or psychiatric disease, volunteered to participate. All participants were Spanish native speakers, without any neurological disorder reported and normal or corrected vision, and a preference for digital texts than print ones. All participants indicated a higher use of digital media than print, although specific usage was not quantified. They were randomly divided into two counterbalanced groups to perform the experiment tasks. In this way, one subgroup first performed the digital task and after that the print one, while the other way around was employed for the second subgroup. The tasks were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University ethical committee. The ethical code is UCV/2020-2021/030. Participants gave written consent to participate in the study.

A total of two tasks were carried out per participant: a reading comprehension task (RCT) and a lexical decision task (LDT).

For the reading comprehension task, two stories published in previous literature (Perea, Panadero, Moret-Tatay, & Gómez, 2012 ) were selected with its correspondent reading comprehension questions. These two stories have been successfully employed with children and employed in a counterbalanced design as the one carried out in the original research. In other words, the participants were divided into two groups, one read one of the stories in digital and the other one in print, while conversely in the other group. In terms of presentation, the whole text was presented in a single screen or in a single piece of paper. One text has a total of 153 Spanish words and the second one 162, with the title “The Wind” and “The Snowman” (see Perea et al., 2012 ). Afterwards, participants had to write in a piece of paper the answer to five questions related to each text, which were the same questions employed in the original study.

The second part of the session was dedicated to carry out the LDT. The stimuli consisted of a set of 120 five-letter words in Spanish and 120 pseudowords. Words were divided into two lists (to enable the comparison between print and digital in a counterbalanced way) and matched in terms of frequency and orthographic neighbors, as in the original publication (Moret-Tatay & Perea, 2011 ) 1 . It should be noted that this material was also originally developed for children and published in previous research work, but it has been employed in other experiments with other Spanish groups that differ in age, such as university and senior students (Navarro-Pardo et al., 2013 ; Perea, Devis, Marcet, & Gomez, 2016 ). By using these materials employed in children and older adults, the reading of simple texts is to be examined. Moreover, current results might allow future comparisons in different age groups.

All stimuli were presented with a fixed text that was the same for the digital and printed versions, in lowercase 14 pt Times New Roman. The viewing distance was optional for the participants, who were free to approach the text as closely as they wished, but with a maximum distance of 30 cm. For ecological validity reasons, the chosen presentation of texts was carried out as follows: the digital texts were presented on the computer screen, while the printed ones were presented on a sheet of paper on the table.

As previous mentioned, participants had to perform a LDT and a RCT. Thus, two lists of words and two versions for each story, one for a digital and one for a print medium, were prepared and randomly counterbalanced across participants. Half of the participants were initially presented with one text and word list, (e.g., digital), and then the other text and word list (e.g., on paper). The participants were tested in a quiet room at the University structure in small groups. The presentation of the stimuli and recorded response times were controlled by computers through the Windows software DMDX (Forster and Forster, 2003 ).

The presentation of the stories was counterbalanced across digital and print media, as shown in Fig. ​ Fig.1. 1 . It should be noted that for the RCT there was no maximum time in both print and digital mediums, the time employed for each participant was recorded. Participants were encouraged to employ the time they needed to properly read the text. They must press a key in the keyboard to indicate the starting and the same bottom to indicate the reading end in both mediums, similar than the LDT procedure. After the reading phase, they were asked to answer five questions out of time in a paper medium. Therefore, the dependent variables used were the unrestricted reading times (paper versus digital) and accuracy was measured.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41155_2022_212_Fig1_HTML.jpg

RCT task. The task was counterbalanced in the order of presentation of reading material according to media type: digital or print (left versus right). Finally, written comprehension questions were asked in written form

For the LDT digital version, after pressing a starting bottom, a fixation point (+) was presented for 500 ms in the center of the screen. Then the target stimulus was presented until the participant’s response, with a maximum of 2500 ms. Participants were instructed to press a button (labeled “yes”) if the stimulus corresponded to an existing word in Spanish, and press another button (labeled “no”) if it did not. As depicted in Fig. ​ Fig.2, 2 , for the paper-based adaptation of the TDL, randomized lists of words were generated on printed paper, and participants were challenged to cross out the words against the pseudo-words.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41155_2022_212_Fig2_HTML.jpg

The classical LDT. The left-hand page is the paper adaptation for the LDT where participants crossed the words from pseudowords

In this case, participants had to press a button in DMDX to start and finish the task, and their timing was compared with the digital version. For the whole experiment, participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible, maintaining a reasonable level of accuracy. The stimuli were randomly presented to each participant. Each session lasted about 20 minutes. Since reaction time might vary slightly between words and pseudo-words into each medium (due to obvious differences in media manipulation), only the accuracy between digital and print word recognition was taken as the dependent variable.

Data analysis

Sample size was estimated with G*Power software (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007 ). A non-parametric approach was carried out, as some assumptions, such as normality, were not accomplished on the RCT times and LDT accuracy of the participants. Correlation was used to examine how performance in RCT and word recognition were related to each other. Furthermore, a Bayesian inference was carried out using the Bayes factor notation (BF10), as a strategy that offers several benefits from frequentist statistics in terms of Interval confidence for parameters, as well as evidential trajectory in favor of H 1 over H 0 (Marsman & Wagenmakers, 2017 ). This approach also allows to quantify uncertainty about effect sizes more easily (Moret-Tatay, Wester, & Gamermann, 2020 ). It should be noted that that measure of interest, the RTs, can be non-normally distributed, so other approaches might be of interest to shed light on the barriers to reach assumptions such as data normality. In this way, the Bayes factor notation (BF10) was employed to support H 1 over H 0 where, according to medium theories, word recognition and reading times predict comprehension, and differences might appear depending on the format. Data analysis was performed by using JASP (Version 0.12.2) [Computer software].

Descriptive analyses on the variables of interest are depicted in Table ​ Table1. 1 . It should be noted that reading time in the RCT was remarkably longer for on-paper reading than for on-screen reading. The same pattern was identified in reading comprehension and word recognition tasks. A test of normality (Shapiro-Wilks) was conducted on the variables under study. Statistically significant results (all p <.01) suggested that these variables were not normally distributed. For this reason, a non-parametric approach was carried out.

Descriptive analysis on the three variables of interest: comprehension (over 5 points), word recognition accuracy (over 1 point), and reading time in milliseconds

SD standard deviation

A Wilcoxon signed-ranks test indicated that reading time in a print medium was higher than in a digital one, and the rank-biserial correlation ( r ) between both measures was employed as a measure for effect size in this non-parametric approach: Z =− 5.05; p <.001; r =.91; 95% CI [.84, .96]. This difference was also statistically significant in LDT accuracy; however, the confidence interval did not support this difference: Z =− 3.84; p <.001; r = − .04; 95% CI [− .37, .30]. RCT accuracy also reached the statistical significance level: Z = − 1.16; p <.001; r = − .50; 95% CI [− .72, -.20]. Table ​ Table2 2 depicts the Spearman correlations across variables, showing a strong correlation between RCT accuracy of digital and print media (rho=0.608; p <0.01). Moreover, RCT time in digital media correlated with RCT accuracy in digital in a positive way (rho=0.39; p <0.05), but not in print media. RCT time in print inversely correlated with accuracy in both digital (rho= − 0.339; p <0.05) and print mediums (rho= − 0.263; p <0.01). Finally, digital LDT only correlated with RCT time (rho=0.334; p <0.05), while print LDT correlated with both print (rho=0.319; p <0.05) and digital reading time (rho=0.335; p <0.05).

Spearman’s rho correlations on the three variables of interest: comprehension (over 5 points), word recognition accuracy (over 1 point), and reading time in milliseconds

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

A Bayesian paired procedure was also carried out across mediums. Figures ​ Figures3, 3 , ​ ,4, 4 , and ​ and5 5 illustrate the evidential trajectory in favor of H 1 over H 0 . It must be pointed out that the evidence for the alternative hypothesis is relatively stable for reading times and word recognition, thereby suggesting that the analysis is robust. However, the evidence in favor of H1 in RC is not anecdotical.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41155_2022_212_Fig3_HTML.jpg

Bayes factor robustness check for differences between RCT time on screen and paper format

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41155_2022_212_Fig4_HTML.jpg

Bayes factor robustness check for differences between LDT accuracy on digital and paper support

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41155_2022_212_Fig5_HTML.jpg

Bayes factor robustness check for differences between RCT accuracy on digital and paper support

The digital proliferation has attracted the scientific community’s interest towards the reading process in the last decade (Clinton, 2019 ). A switch towards superficial from deep reading processes has been described in digital mediums (Mangen & Kuiken, 2014 ), also suggesting that those who often read in print texts are less likely to perform multiple tasks during reading than those who often read in digital screens. Not surprisingly, it has been claimed that this difference is related to encouraging divided attention in this last medium (Sanbonmatsu, Strayer, Medeiros-Ward, & Watson, 2013 ). Even if detrimental effects have been reported regarding digital texts, university students seem to prefer digital sources (Terra, 2015 ). Even if mixed results have been found in the literature on long texts, what are the differences between reading digital- versus print-easy texts in university students who prefer the digital medium? Do these differences influence individual word recognition or only general comprehension?

The current results show a longer reading time for printed texts compared to digital texts. From the Bayesian approach, it is interesting to note that the most robust variables on the differences between print and digital texts were RCT time and LDT accuracy, but not for RCT accuracy. A feasible explanation related to RCT time would be related to the fact that printed texts allow readers to see and feel the spatial extent and physical dimensions of the text. In this way, the number of stimuli in printed texts would be enlarged, ultimately providing a greater number of physical, tactile, and spatio-temporal cues for reading, even for easy texts. However, comprehension seems not to be affected under such a level of demand. This might also explain higher attentional levels. However, the current manipulation does not allow to claim this explanation, as attentional levels were not measured, but are of interest for future lines of research. What these results did attempt to measure was the level of comprehension, which although slightly higher in the printed format, it was not conclusive. This result could support that young people would adapt to this form of reading as opposed to the older adult groups described in previous literature with techniques such as EEG (Kretzschmar et al., 2013 ). However, although the results seem promising, replications with other age groups are necessary to establish such e-generation claim.

The second main aim of the study was to address the relationship between RC and LDT accuracy in both mediums under study. An inverse relationship is found between RCT time and accuracy for print. However, this pattern changes for digital texts. This could reflect a familiarization cost from one medium to another. On the other hand, the direct relationship between RCT accuracy and time in digital texts might reflect a more conservative pattern of response in this medium that is more familiar to participants. Finally, a relationship was found between RCT and LDT accuracy per each medium. RCT accuracy in digital was also related to LDT accuracy in print, but not inversely. This would mark differences in individual patterns, as marked in previous literature regarding how text is integrated between mediums (Latini, Bråten, & Salmerón, 2020 ). Nevertheless, it must be considered that the results were not direct, suggesting that more variables underlie this process. One variable of interest—and it was not controlled in the current study—is the motivation during the task. This is one of the main limitations of this study. Moreover, the task employed here could be very easy for university students. This material was employed to avoid any “floor effect” and allows future comparison with other age groups.

It should be noted that being faster in the reading task has been also linked to the search for immediate reward in previous literature (Ackerman & Lauterman, 2012 ; Delgado et al., 2018 ). If so, that might explain shorter RTs under the digital, which in this case, might not affect comprehension in an easier task as the one under study. Despite the well-known preference for digital texts, regardless of the task and the benefits offered by the medium (for example, portability and speed), there are certain controversies regarding them, particularly considering groups of readers with dyslexia, who showed a more effective and comprehensive reading in digital texts (Chen & Keong, 2017 ; Schneps, Thomson, Chen, Sonnert, & Pomplun, 2013 ), but in these cases, the difficulty of the text was not controlled.

Finally, we would like to highlight the extra value of an integrative strategy such as the Bayesian approach in this field (Moret-Tatay, Beneyto-Arrojo, Laborde-Bois, Martínez-Rubio, & Senent-Capuz, 2016 ; Nuzzo, 2014 ; Ruiz-Ruano, López-Puga, & Delgado-Morán, 2019 ), by offering additional evidence to support differences between digital and print materials. In this regard, the Bayesian correlation depicted not particularly strong H 1 in some cases. To sum up, the results of this study seem not to ensure the superiority of electronic texts over print ones in the prediction of reading time, as latency is not a single indicator, and as superficial pattern seems to be depicted even for easier texts.

Conclusions

The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between RCT accuracy, RCT time, and LDT accuracy in print and digital mediums in university students with a preference for digital texts. For this reason, two experiments were carried out into a counterbalanced order across digital and print environments. Results can be described as follows: (i) latencies for reading on digital texts were shorter in digital than print mediums; (ii) Word recognition and Comprehension were slightly higher for print than digital texts, but not conclusive (iii) Reading time on digital and print text strongly correlated to each other; (iv) A relationship regarding LDT accuracy in print and RCT accuracy in print and digital texts was found, but this is not the case with LDT accuracy in digital.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all participants involved in this study, and Professor Gracia Prats Arolas for their support on participants’ recruitment and comments, as well as the Universidad Católica de Valencia San Vicente Mártir for the support and funding.

Authors’ contributions

Conceptualization: N.B.-G. and C. M.-T.; methodology and formal analysis: C.M.-T.; data curation: N.B.-G., M-J.I., and C.M.-T.; writing—review and editing: N.B.-G., M.J.-I., and C.M.-T. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Declarations

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

1 As in the original research by Moret-Tatay and Perea et al. ( 2011 ) the same 120 words from this research were employed, 60 were of high frequency (mean = 146.7 per million, range = 30.9–675.6) and 60 low frequency (mean = 10.2 per million, range = 0.7–23.2). Moreover, and as previously mentioned, the same two lists of stimuli than in the original research were employed, with 60 words and 60 nonwords being randomly assigned to List 1 and the other 60 words and 60 nonwords being assigned to List 2. In this way, those participants who were initially were presented with List 1 in Block 1, the second block would be List 2 and vice versa.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Noemí Bresó-Grancha, Email: [email protected] .

Carmen Moret-Tatay, Email: moc.liamg@teromnemrac , Email: [email protected] .

  • Ackerman R, Lauterman T. Taking reading comprehension exams on screen or on paper? A metacognitive analysis of learning texts under time pressure. Computers in Human Behavior. 2012; 28 (5):1816–1828. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.023. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Annisette LE, Lafreniere KD. Social media, texting, and personality: A test of the shallowing hypothesis. Personality and Individual Differences. 2017; 115 :154–158. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.043. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baron NS, Mangen A. Doing the Reading: The Decline of Long Long-Form Reading in Higher Education. Poetics Today. 2021; 42 (2):253–279. doi: 10.1215/03335372-8883248. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Benedetto S, Carbone A, Drai-Zerbib V, Pedrotti M, Baccino T. Effects of luminance and illuminance on visual fatigue and arousal during digital reading. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 41 :112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.023. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Benedetto S, Drai-Zerbib V, Pedrotti M, Tissier G, Baccino T. E-Readers and Visual Fatigue. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8 (12):e83676. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083676. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown, L. T., Mohr, K. A., Wilcox, B. R., & Barrett, T. S. (2018). The effects of dyad reading and text difficulty on third-graders’ reading achievement. The Journal of Educational Research , 111 (5), 541–553.
  • Chang, Y.-H., Wu, I.-C., & Hsiung, C. A. (2020). Reading activity prevents long-term decline in cognitive function in older people: Evidence from a 14-year longitudinal study. International Psychogeriatrics , 1–12 . 10.1017/S1041610220000812. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
  • Chen CJ, Keong MWY. Affording inclusive dyslexia-friendly online text reading. Universal Access in the Information Society. 2017; 16 (4):951–965. doi: 10.1007/s10209-016-0501-0. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clinton V. Reading from paper compared to screens: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Reading. 2019; 42 (2):288–325. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12269. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corazon SS, Stigsdotter UK, Ekholm O, Pedersen PV, Scopelliti M, Giuliani MV. Activities to Alleviate Stress and the Association with Leisure Time Activities, Socioeconomic Status, and General Health: Activities to alleviate stress. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research. 2010; 15 (4):161–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2011.00059.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Oliveira, D. G., da Silva, P. B., Dias, N. M., Seabra, A. G., & Macedo, E. C. (2014). Reading component skills in dyslexia: Word recognition, comprehension and processing speed. Frontiers in Psychology , 5 . 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01339. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
  • Delgado P, Vargas C, Ackerman R, Salmerón L. Don’t throw away your printed books: A meta-analysis on the effects of reading media on reading comprehension. Educational Research Review. 2018; 25 :23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.09.003. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eldredge, J. L. (1988). Improving the reading comprehension skills of poor readers. Reading Horizons: A Journal of Literacy and Language Arts , 29 (1), 4.
  • Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007; 39 (2):175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers , 35 (1), 116–124. [ PubMed ]
  • Gabay Y, Dundas E, Plaut D, Behrmann M. Atypical perceptual processing of faces in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language. 2017; 173 :41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.06.004. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoover WA, Gough PB. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 1990; 2 (2):127–160. doi: 10.1007/BF00401799. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hsu C-T, Clariana R, Schloss B, Li P. Neurocognitive Signatures of Naturalistic Reading of Scientific Texts: A Fixation-Related fMRI Study. Scientific Reports. 2019; 9 (1):10678. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47176-7. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Imbir KK, Duda-Goławska J, Pastwa M, Jankowska M, Modzelewska A, Sobieszek A, Żygierewicz J. Electrophysiological and Behavioral Correlates of Valence, Arousal and Subjective Significance in the Lexical Decision Task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2020; 14 :567220. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.567220. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jacobs, A. M. (2015). Neurocognitive poetics: Methods and models for investigating the neuronal and cognitive-affective bases of literature reception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience , 9 . 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00186. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
  • Katz L, Brancazio L, Irwin J, Katz S, Magnuson J, Whalen DH. What lexical decision and naming tell us about reading. Reading and Writing. 2012; 25 (6):1259–1282. doi: 10.1007/s11145-011-9316-9. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim, H. J., & Kim, J. (2013). Reading from an LCD monitor versus paper: Teenagers’ reading performance. International Journal of Research Studies . Educational Technology , 2 (1). 10.5861/ijrset.2012.170.
  • Kirby JR, Savage RS. Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy. 2008; 42 (2):75–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kretzschmar F, Pleimling D, Hosemann J, Füssel S, Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I, Schlesewsky M. Subjective Impressions Do Not Mirror Online Reading Effort: Concurrent EEG-Eyetracking Evidence from the Reading of Books and Digital Media. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8 (2):e56178. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056178. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kuzmičová A, Mangen A, Støle H, Begnum AC. Literature and readers’ empathy: A qualitative text manipulation study. Language and Literature: International Journal of Stylistics. 2017; 26 (2):137–152. doi: 10.1177/0963947017704729. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latini, N., Bråten, I., & Salmerón, L. (2020). Does reading medium affect processing and integration of textual and pictorial information? A multimedia eye-tracking study. Contemporary Educational Psychology , 62 , 101870.
  • Luce RD. Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization . Oxford: (Oxford University Press on Demand; 1986. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mangen A. What Hands May Tell Us about Reading and Writing: What Hands May Tell Us about Reading and Writing. Educational Theory. 2016; 66 (4):457–477. doi: 10.1111/edth.12183. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mangen A, Kuiken D. Lost in an iPad: Narrative engagement on paper and tablet. Scientific Study of Literature. 2014; 4 (2):150–177. doi: 10.1075/ssol.4.2.02man. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mangen A, Olivier G, Velay J-L. Comparing Comprehension of a Long Text Read in Print Book and on Kindle: Where in the Text and When in the Story? Frontiers in Psychology. 2019; 10 :38. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00038. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mangen A, Walgermo BR, Brønnick K. Reading linear texts on paper versus computer screen: Effects on reading comprehension. International Journal of Educational Research. 2013; 58 :61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2012.12.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marsman M, Wagenmakers E-J. Bayesian benefits with JASP. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2017; 14 (5):545–555. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2016.1259614. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNamara DS, Kintsch W. Learning from texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes. 1996; 22 (3):247–288. doi: 10.1080/01638539609544975. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moret-Tatay C, Beneyto-Arrojo MJ, Laborde-Bois SC, Martínez-Rubio D, Senent-Capuz N. Gender, Coping, and Mental Health: A Bayesian Network Model Analysis. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal. 2016; 44 (5):827–835. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2016.44.5.827. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moret-Tatay C, Gamermann D, Navarro-Pardo E, de Córdoba F, Castellá P. ExGUtils: A Python Package for Statistical Analysis With the ex-Gaussian Probability Density. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018; 9 :612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00612. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moret-Tatay C, Perea M. Is the go/no-go lexical decision task preferable to the yes/no task with developing readers? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 110 (1):125–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.005. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moret-Tatay C, Wester AG, Gamermann D. To Google or Not: Differences on How Online Searches Predict Names and Faces. Mathematics. 2020; 8 (11):1964. doi: 10.3390/math8111964. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Navarro-Pardo E, Navarro-Prados AB, Gamermann D, Moret-Tatay C. Differences Between Young and Old University Students on a Lexical Decision Task: Evidence Through an Ex-Gaussian Approach. The Journal of General Psychology. 2013; 140 (4):251–268. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2013.817964. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norton ES, Wolf M. Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and Reading Fluency: Implications for Understanding and Treatment of Reading Disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology. 2012; 63 (1):427–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nuzzo R. Scientific method: Statistical errors. Nature. 2014; 506 (7487):150–152. doi: 10.1038/506150a. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peng P, Barnes M, Wang C, Wang W, Li S, Swanson HL, Dardick W, Tao S. A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin. 2018; 144 (1):48–76. doi: 10.1037/bul0000124. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perea M, Devis E, Marcet A, Gomez P. Are go/no-go tasks preferable to two-choice tasks in response time experiments with older adults? Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2016; 28 (2):147–158. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1107077. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perea M, Moret-Tatay C, Panadero V. Suppression of mirror generalization for reversible letters: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Memory and Language. 2011; 65 (3):237–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.04.005. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perea M, Panadero V, Moret-Tatay C, Gómez P. The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction. 2012; 22 (6):420–430. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.04.001. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Porion A, Aparicio X, Megalakaki O, Robert A, Baccino T. The impact of paper-based versus computerized presentation on text comprehension and memorization. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016; 54 :569–576. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Puga JL, Krzywinski M, Altman N. Bayesian networks. Nature Methods. 2015; 12 (9):799–800. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3550. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rockinson- Szapkiw AJ, Courduff J, Carter K, Bennett D. Electronic versus traditional print textbooks: A comparison study on the influence of university students’ learning. Computers & Education. 2013; 63 :259–266. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.11.022. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruiz-Ruano A-M, López-Puga J, Delgado-Morán J-J. El componente social de la amenaza híbrida y su detección con modelos bayesianos [The Social Component of the Hybrid Threat and its Detection with Bayesian Models]. URVIO . Revista Latinoamericana de Estudios de Seguridad. 2019; 25 :57–69. doi: 10.17141/urvio.25.2019.3997. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sanbonmatsu DM, Strayer DL, Medeiros-Ward N, Watson JM. Who Multi-Tasks and Why? Multi-Tasking Ability, Perceived Multi-Tasking Ability, Impulsivity, and Sensation Seeking. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8 (1):e54402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054402. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schneps MH, Thomson JM, Chen C, Sonnert G, Pomplun M. E-Readers Are More Effective than Paper for Some with Dyslexia. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8 (9):e75634. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075634. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Terra AL. Students’ Reading Behavior: Digital vs. Print Preferences in Portuguese Context. In: Kurbanoglu S, Boustany J, Špiranec S, Grassian E, Mizrachi D, Roy L, editors. Information Literacy: Moving Toward Sustainability . Springer International Publishing; 2015. pp. 436–445. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vogel AC, Church JA, Power JD, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Functional network architecture of reading-related regions across development. Brain and Language. 2013; 125 (2):231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.12.016. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

IGI Global

  • Get IGI Global News

US Flag

  • All Products
  • Book Chapters
  • Journal Articles
  • Video Lessons
  • Teaching Cases
  • Recommend to Librarian
  • Recommend to Colleague
  • Fair Use Policy

Copyright Clearance Center

  • Access on Platform

Export Reference

Mendeley

  • e-Journal Collection
  • e-Book Collection Select
  • Social Sciences Knowledge Solutions e-Journal Collection
  • Education Knowledge Solutions e-Journal Collection

Reading Online and Offline: Language Teachers’ Perspectives

Reading Online and Offline: Language Teachers’ Perspectives

Literature review.

A number of studies have been conducted on online and off-line reading, particularly with a focus on online reading strategies of English as a second language (ESL) and/or English as a foreign language (EFL) learners. For example, Anderson (2003) examined metacognitive online reading strategies of ESL readers in the US and EFL readers in Costa Rica. He used an online survey of reading strategies (OSORS) adapted from the survey of reading strategies (SORS) (Sheorey & Mokhtari, 2001; Mokhtari & Sheorey, 2002, cited in Anderson, 2003) with three categories of reading strategies: global reading strategies, problem solving strategies and support strategies. The results of his study showed that the online readers used problem solving strategies (e.g., adjusting reading rate, rereading difficult text, pausing to think about what one is reading) most frequently and the EFL readers reported using the problem solving strategies more frequently than the ESL readers. There were significant differences in the use of problem solving strategies between the ESL readers and the EFL readers whereas no differences were found in the use of global reading strategies and support reading strategies between the two groups in the study. Based on these results, Anderson argues that metacognitive online reading strategies play an important role in L2 reading and the distinction between EFL and ESL in online reading is fading.

In a different context, Huang, Chern and Lin (2009) investigated online reading strategies of EFL learners in Taiwan and the effects of strategy use on the learners’ reading comprehension. They created an online reading program which offers four strategy types of fifteen function buttons: global strategies, problem-solving strategies, support strategies and socio-affective strategies. They invited their university students to use the reading program to read four articles online and collected data on the frequency of the students’ use of each of the strategy support buttons. The results of their study showed that support strategies (e.g., using online dictionaries, online grammar resources, an online translation mechanism, highlighting tools and electronic notebooks) were used by the students most dominantly and contributed to their comprehension gains while global strategies (e.g., using previews, keywords and outlines of the texts, making predictions) contributed to better comprehension, particularly for low proficiency students. Huang, Chern and Lin assert that L2 learners’ personal learning styles and needs need to be considered for better reading performance.

Complete Article List

COMMENTS

  1. Reading online vs offline: what's best for learning?

    However, online reading can be very distracting. While it is easy to get information from online sources, the brain processes digital reading differently than it does when reading offline from paper. Shallower Processing. Online means constant exposure to fast-paced, and rapidly changing information. Digital media trains the brain to process ...

  2. Online or Offline Reading: Which is Better for Learning?

    When reading online, the convenience and accessibility of the material is one major advantage. Moreover, the wealth of resources, multimedia content, and interactivity of the web make it easier to engage in the material and learn more effectively. On the other hand, offline reading can provide a more focused environment to read the material ...

  3. Stay Updated With the Trend: Online vs Offline Reading

    Here are the benefits of reading online, Higher accessibility - The best thing related to reading digitally, is that you can read novels anywhere, across all devices. This makes online reading an efficient source of reading. Cost-effective - Since you can download the books online, it makes you save a fortune over the years.

  4. Online Reading or Offline? Which is Better for Learning? (2024)

    Online Reading is More Accessible. Reading online transcends the limitation of offline reading when it comes to accessibility. Whether a reader is visually impaired or hard of hearing, online reading has technology like Closed Captions and text-to-speech to overcome them. Also Read: Best eBook Creation Softwares.

  5. Reading on-screen vs reading in print: What's the difference for

    In Baron's own research between 2013 and 2015 of more than 400 university students from five countries, 86% preferred reading longer texts in print and 78% when reading for pleasure, with 92% saying it was easiest to concentrate when reading print. 85% of the US students were more likely to multitask in an online environment and only 26% when ...

  6. PDF How Digital Reading Differs from Traditional Reading: An Action ...

    In other studies done about online reading dispositions, it is observed that there is a signifi cant relationship between online reading disposition and online reading skill (Coiro, 2008) and that since students find the internet to be practical, valuable , and attractive, they are willing to endure the hardships of online reading (Coiro, 2009).

  7. Will you learn better from reading on screen or on paper?

    Much of what we read on-screen tends to be text messages and social-media posts. They're usually easy to understand. So, "when people read on-screen, they read faster," says Alexander at the University of Maryland. "Their eyes scan the pages and the words faster than if they're reading on a piece of paper.".

  8. Predicting Reading Comprehension on the Internet:

    These relatively large positive correlations between online and offline reading comprehension are contrary to findings from a previous study (Leu et al., 2005) that found no correlation between scores on the same measure of offline reading comprehension (e.g., standardized reading achievement scores) and online reading comprehension assessed ...

  9. Focusing Effects from Online and Offline Reading Tasks

    reading experiences. What is considered important emerges as a function of. instructional tasks, reader goals, and the nature of the text content. To date, studies of reader focus have examined ...

  10. The Difference Between Online Reading and Offline Reading

    Based on the definition, offline reading occurs through printed texts, while online reading happens through screens and electronic texts, especially web-based reading materials (Dwyer, 2016). Unlike offline reading, where text is bound within the covers of a book, an online reader has the mandate of assembling potential text for reading.

  11. PDF ONLINE VS OFFLINE READING CLASSES: THE EFL UNIVERSITY ...

    (20 23), 6 (1): 242-250 244 P-ISSN 2623-0356 E-ISSN 2654-5586 whether to conduct online or offline reading classes based on students' perspectives. This study is also intended to serve as a ...

  12. (PDF) Online Vs Offline Reading Classes

    Whereas, the advantages are students have more reading sources, have time and reading topics flexibility, and are cost-effective. Moreover, EFL university students' problems or difficulties in ...

  13. Online vs Offline Reading

    Online Reading has taken prominence, let's take a quick look at the differences between online and offline reading. Offline reading means carry the book on move. It becomes difficult as it is both bulky and time consuming. A book often does not give meanings of basal words for which it is not recommended. Example: A book of chemistry, may ...

  14. Effects of offline vs. online digital storytelling on the development

    The present study investigated the effects of offline vs. online digital storytelling on the development of EFL learners' literacy skills (reading and writing). Forty-two lower intermediate language learners participated in the study as the experimental (n = 21) and control groups (n = 21). The Reading-Writing section of the Key English Test ...

  15. Online Reading Vs Traditional Reading In Today's World

    Advantages of Traditional Reading. 1. Linear Reading. When you're reading something that is in your hand, it grasps your full attention and you read it in the sequence that it's written. This gives you a better understanding of the book. 2. Does Not Need Power.

  16. The effect of reading strategy use on online reading comprehension

    Using online reading strategies in the classroom can increase students' active participation in the learning process [ 18 ]. These strategies have therefore become important for reading comprehension achievement [ 10 ]. They are recommended for use in reading online texts to help motivate interest in reading [ 19 ].

  17. Examining the online reading behavior and performance of fifth-graders

    Unlike traditional offline reading, online reading provides a "hypertextual" form of reading. The hypertext on the Internet is a type of text that uses a system of nodes and hyperlinks. The node is the basic unit of a hypertext system and often represents a concept or an idea. The nodes are connected by hyperlinks, and every node can be ...

  18. Is print reading better than learning onscreen?

    When reading texts of several hundred words or more, learning is generally more successful when it's on paper than onscreen. A cascade of research confirms this finding. The benefits of print particularly shine through when experimenters move from posing simple tasks - like identifying the main idea in a reading passage - to ones that ...

  19. Online and Offline Reading Comprehension

    Download Free PDF. View PDF. Online and Offline Reading Comprehension Carlo Soberano De La Salle University, Manila Abstract Modern perspectives on the innovation of teaching pedagogies, language assessments, theories and practice were introduced. However, limited attention was given to ESL users' modality of reading comprehension and preference.

  20. Reading digital- versus print-easy texts: a study with university

    Eldredge, J. L. (1988). Improving the reading comprehension skills of poor readers. Reading Horizons: A Journal of Literacy and Language Arts, 29(1), 4. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods.

  21. Reading Online and Offline:: Language Teachers' Perspectives

    and investigates the teachers' perspectives on. and experiences with online reading. In this. article, reading electronic texts on all types of. computers and mobile devices is considered as ...

  22. Reading Online and Offline: Language Teachers' Perspectives

    This article presents data from the discussion forums and investigates the teachers' perspectives on online reading, while exploring the differences between online reading and offline reading and the advantages and disadvantages of the two forms of reading. Findings indicate that many teachers liked to download and print study materials for ...

  23. Essay on online vs offline education

    Offline Education. Offline Education or previously known as traditional and face-to-face education is the most common method of giving out education. Online classes are great for lower expenses and are easier to get but Offline classes are far more beneficial as a whole. Students can ask about problems the teacher during the lecture or ask ...