• Browse All Articles
  • Newsletter Sign-Up

DecisionMaking →

No results found in working knowledge.

  • Were any results found in one of the other content buckets on the left?
  • Try removing some search filters.
  • Use different search filters.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals

Decision making articles from across Nature Portfolio

Latest research and reviews.

decision making research topics

Dynamics of collective cooperation under personalised strategy updates

Collective cooperation is found across many social and biological systems. Here, the authors find that infrequent hub updates promote the emergence of collective cooperation and develop an algorithm that optimises collective cooperation with update rates.

  • Sean P. Cornelius

decision making research topics

Experiencing nature leads to healthier food choices

Across five studies of 3,726 participants, walking in nature (study 1) and viewing images of nature (studies 2-5) led to significantly more healthy food choices and fewer unhealthy food choices compared to urban settings.

  • Maria Langlois
  • Pierre Chandon

decision making research topics

Contributions of human cultures to biodiversity and ecosystem conservation

This Review identifies and describes interactions and feedbacks between biodiversity and diversity of Indigenous Peoples and Local Communities, and uses case studies from South America to illustrate the conservation and human benefits that can arise from protecting both biological and cultural diversity.

  • Carolina Levis
  • Bernardo M. Flores
  • Charles R. Clement

decision making research topics

Field testing the transferability of behavioural science knowledge on promoting vaccinations

The authors field test the transferability of behavioural science knowledge on promoting COVID-19 booster uptake. Interventions effective in past field work improve uptake, but those deemed effective in surveys measuring intentions or predictions do not.

  • Silvia Saccardo
  • Hengchen Dai
  • Jeffrey Fujimoto

decision making research topics

Aligning renewable energy expansion with climate-driven range shifts

The authors conduct a systematic literature review on renewable energy expansion and biodiversity. Comparing renewable energy siting maps with the ranges of two threatened species under future climates, they highlight the potential conflict and need for consideration of climate-change-driven range shifts.

  • Uzma Ashraf
  • Toni Lyn Morelli
  • Rebecca R. Hernandez

decision making research topics

Public perceptions and support of climate intervention technologies across the Global North and Global South

This article establishes a global baseline of public perceptions of climate-intervention technologies. Publics across the global South are more favorable and supportive but concerned about impacts on mitigation and unequal burdens of risks on poor countries.

  • Chad M. Baum
  • Livia Fritz
  • Benjamin K. Sovacool

Advertisement

News and Comment

decision making research topics

Transformative localization to accelerate the 2030 Agenda

To get the Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) back on track we need to reshape our approaches to implementation, including localization. Localization done differently involves progressing beyond symbolic piecemeal efforts, prioritizing the SDGs with the greatest gains, and pluralizing interpretations and pathways for actions.

  • Shirin Malekpour
  • Brett Bryan

decision making research topics

FAO’s 1.5 °C roadmap for food systems falls short

The first instalment of the FAO food systems roadmap is a key step in identifying pathways to achieve zero hunger without breaching the 1.5 °C climate change threshold. But future instalments should be more methodologically transparent, emphasize the need to reduce animal-sourced food consumption and align with a holistic One Health approach.

  • Cleo Verkuijl
  • Jan Dutkiewicz
  • Matthew Hayek

decision making research topics

Judgements of bias vary with observers’ political ideology and targets’ characteristics

Whether conservatives or liberals have higher sensitivity towards underrepresentation depends on the target of the judgement: conservatives are shown to have higher thresholds than liberals for indicating bias against traditionally nondominant groups, whereas liberals have higher bias thresholds regarding dominant groups. However, such relationships weaken when the targets of bias are unknown or ideologically irrelevant to the observer, which emphasizes the context-dependency of such bias judgements.

Emerging local energy communities

  • Silvana Lakeman

decision making research topics

Adaptation requires attuning to shifting temporal patterns

Communal life is characterized by the shared timing of human and environmental events. Climate change is disrupting these timings, creating mismatches in these coordinated temporal patterns and requiring adaptive governance.

  • Scott Bremer
  • Nicole Klenk
  • Danielle Kwan-Lafond

Unpacking symbolic policy-making for the first Global Stocktake under the Paris Agreement

The first ever Global Stocktake is scheduled to conclude during COP28 and aims to assess progress on climate change mitigation, adaptation, and the means of implementation. However, the Global Stocktake runs the risk of overestimating progress by overlooking the symbolic dimensions of climate change adaptation policy.

  • Robbert Biesbroek
  • Alexandra Lesnikowski

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

decision making research topics

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

National Research Council (US) Committee on Risk and Decision Making. Risk and Decision Making: Perspectives and Research. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 1982.

Cover of Risk and Decision Making: Perspectives and Research

Risk and Decision Making: Perspectives and Research.

  • Hardcopy Version at National Academies Press

RESEARCH ON RISK AND DECISION MAKING

People in a large number of professions and disciplines are currently engaged in research related to risk and decision making:

  • Toxicologists devise laboratory experiments to identify potential carcinogens, mutagens, and other toxic substances;
  • Climatologists build models to predict the effects of atmospheric CO 2 concentrations on global weather patterns;
  • Epidemiologists use large data bases to isolate statistical associations between various risk factors (e.g., pollution and diet) and various indices of morbidity and mortality;
  • Physicists, chemists, and biologists study fundamental physical processes to facilitate the identification and assessment of risks;
  • Ecologists investigate the tensions between the needs of humans and the needs of other organisms in an ecosystem;
  • Economists explore the effects of regulation on inflation, employment, innovation, competition, and productivity;
  • Legal scholars assess how various liability doctrines affect both the compensation of victims and the incentives for injurors to engage in risk-generating conduct;
  • Psychologists develop and test theories about how people form perceptions about risks and about how people make personal decisions about risks in their daily lives;
  • Communication specialists examine the potential effects of educational media campaigns on self-hazardous life-styles;
  • Market researchers assess the consequences of advertising on the consumption of hazardous products and substances;
  • Sociologists study the influences of peer pressure on teenage smoking and drinking habits;
  • Political scientists describe and evaluate how different political and economic systems generate and cope with risks;
  • Philosophers and political theorists study the value trade-offs and ethical considerations in risk and decision making;
  • Demographers and biostatisticians compile and analyze risk indices to identify crucial trends in risks over time;
  • Defense analysts weigh the deterrent effects of weapon systems against the risks of escalation in armed conflict;
  • Classical and Bayesian statisticians study how inferences about uncertainties should be made, how new information about risks should be incorporated into old beliefs, and how information about risks from disparate sources should be combined in a formal decision analysis;
  • Organizational theorists study how the incentives and rewards faced by employees in business firms and public agencies cause people to generate and cope with risks;
  • Engineers design safer consumer products and cleaner production processes; they worry about the cost and complexity of safety devices versus the risks of accidents;
  • Geographers study techniques for managing natural hazards and natural disasters; and
  • Decision and management scientists develop methods for formalizing value trade-offs in decisions about risks.

This list, although incomplete, conveys the range and diversity of expertise now involved in research involving consideration of risk and resulting decisions. It also makes plain the near impossibility of establishing priorities for research. Accordingly, the agenda for research offered below is not a ranking but rather a sampling of research that is generally applicable to the improvement of risk analyses in individual disciplines.

A message also carried by the agenda is that performance of risk analysis calls not only for knowledge of its methods but also for disciplinary understanding of the particular problems at issue, be they certifying airplanes or assessing the mutagenic effects of a new chemical; it also demands an awareness of economic and social implications of a given risk and various options for dealing with it. The practice of risk analyses requires the knowledge of many fields; it is in fact an undertaking of multiple disciplines melded together in an interdisciplinary analysis.

Obviously, with the diversity of research actors, coordination of research and communication of its results is a necessity if risk analyses are to have available the best tools and expertise. To be more specific, research on particular issues in risk analysis that coordinates, suggests, and links insights from different disciplines may be especially effective. Research that coordinates the techniques of one field with the issues facing another does not now have a home within universities or research institutions. Consequently this work, falling at the interface between disciplines, is often neglected; yet it is the essence of risk analysis. Moreover, the coordinating function, to be truly effective, must be supported by effective communication of results to the involved parties.

Our nation needs vigorous and coordinated programs of research on risk and decision making. Risk in its many forms is such a pervasive problem, is subject to so many unknowns and uncertainties, and is such a significant source of social concern that it demands the attention of the nation's research communities and research sponsors. Research is needed not only on the identification, assessment, and quantification of various risks, but also on improved methods of analysis for decision making and on improved market, legislative, regulatory, and judicial decision-making processes. Such research should draw on experiences within the United States as well as those in other countries.

There will not be, of course, any scientific resolution to many of the controversies about risk and decision making. Scientific research can narrow the range of disagreement about the magnitudes of certain risks, but even in those cases the result may trigger more explicit and heated controversy about value trade-offs and ethical considerations. At the same time, the whole field of conflict resolution--so integral to risk and decision making--is a fertile and valuable research domain for behavioral, social, and management scientists. Research and experimentation with improved techniques for mediation and bargaining could be especially useful in resolving disputes about environmental policies that affect health, safety, and the environment.

There is also a need for expansion of interdisciplinary research projects on risk and decision making. The relative neglect of interdisciplinary research is understandable, given the inherent difficulties with such efforts: differences in problem definition, language barriers between participants, problems in finding sponsors who are sympathetic to joint projects, the complexities in establishing appropriate peer review mechanisms for interdisciplinary work, and the extra time and expense associated with combating all of these difficulties. Despite these problems, the committee believes that an expanded interdisciplinary research program on risk and decision making should be undertaken.

We identify numerous specific research topics that are both critically important and that require the joint efforts of multiple disciplines. There are several general rationales for giving some priority to these kinds of projects:

  • The fact that most risk problems require knowledge of specialists from a variety of natural, social, and management sciences;
  • The desire of many decision makers to have an effective synthesis of scientific inputs into decision making, a difficult yet appropriate demand that certainly requires multiple talents;
  • The ability of research teams with diverse members to communicate findings to decision makers who lack scientific training and to the general public; and
  • The tendency of research projects within a single discipline to become preoccupied with technical intricacies that may not be central to the needs of decision makers who are faced with immediate problems involving risks.

Several billion dollars are spent every year on research that is related to society's efforts to cope with health, safety, and environmental risks. Nearly all of this money is devoted to natural science and engineering research, especially biomedical research and, to a lesser extent, safety engineering. Relatively little attention has been focused on social science research, on research to develop better analytical methods for risk assessment, risk evaluation, and decision making. We list social science and analytical research topics that might be explored.

Research on risk analysis is not within the scope of any single agency, but coordination and the communication of results may be. Alternative mechanisms may be an interagency committee or a unit separate from the governmental structure, perhaps housed within a university. Within that framework the committee proposes an agenda for research, one that is not comprehensive but rather selective and indicative. The agenda follows in its outline that of the first section of this report and takes up in order research on: actual and perceived risks ; risk generating and risk coping processes; and approaches to and methods of risk analysis.

  • ACTUAL AND PERCEIVED RISKS

A risk analysis, while asking distinct questions and applying specific knowledge, still must be provided with a perspective--the overall health of society as measured by indicators of the population's health, safety, the state of the environment, or the economy. This is particularly true when the analysis must consider not only the risk itself but perceptions of it. Whether the subject of the analysis is the effects of changing the upper limits on nitrogen oxide emissions from cars, the effects of low-level radiation from nuclear power plants, or putting a new airport at a particular location, the common need is a yardstick against which to measure the relative risk. No such yardstick is now available; even the very extensive data collected by the committee was incomplete, especially for measures of morbidity and environmental quality. A source book of health, safety, and environmental indicators, regularly revised, would be extremely valuable; it certainly would have helped the work of the committee. An analogous volume is Social Indicators , a set of “selected data on social conditions and trends in the United States” with some international comparisons, which is assembled and periodically updated by the social indicators staff of the Center for Demographic Studies in the Bureau of the Census in collaboration with the Interagency Committee on Social Indicators. Many of the indices in this compilation of statistics are relevant to concerns about risk, but many more appropriate indices about risks are missing. A compilation of risk indicators could include data about public perceptions of risks as well as data on actual risk indices.

In addition to a regularly published comprehensive collection of existing health, safety, and environmental indicators, serious thought should be given to how to collect, analyze, evaluate, and disseminate additional information about trends and patterns in mortality, morbidity, and environmental quality. Deficiencies in available data should be assessed, including the likelihood of misinterpretations from such data.

As noted above, data on morbidity and on environmental quality are sparse. Furthermore, data on global conditions and on comparisons of conditions in different countries are very spotty. A more complete, current, and accurate data base would be very useful. It is an ambitious task, but a beginning could be made by outlining the scope of the volume, the data not now collected that should be, and the data that are currently incomplete or insufficiently validated. The forms in which current data are now provided should be critically examined. Are the caveats in the data clearly stated?

Societies are not static and neither is their health. In looking at existing and prospective risks, the direction of change must be considered as well as the current state. For that reason, an effort to encourage the relevant disciplines, such as epidemiology, demography, actuarial analysis, psychology, and statistics, to assess likely trends in known data and their underlying forces would be invaluable in improving the analysis of risks. A dynamic analysis of this sort would, among other things, better tell how relevant current data are to possible future risks. As examples, the following are important questions for risk analysts to discuss:

  • Why have cardiovascular death rates declined so dramatically over the last two decades?
  • How much are cancer rates likely to increase or decrease in the future?
  • What are the causes of higher mortality among blacks compared with whites, males compared with females, Americans compared with Swedes, etc.?

Risk Perceptions and Behavior

Two research communities are concerned with perceptions of risk and choices among them. Behavioral or descriptive decision analysts are concerned with how both lay people and experts actually perceive risks--how they learn about risks, how they behave, and how they explain or rationalize their behavior. This community includes cognitive psychologists, psychometricians, learning theorists, and some economists and operations researchers. Normative or prescriptive decision analysts are concerned with how people should behave or might want to behave if they were consciously made aware of underlying desiderata for reasoned behavior. The field has a theoretical and an applied side. Theory asserts how idealized people should behave to satisfy certain rational desiderata, while the applied side is concerned with guiding people to behave more “rationally.” Prescriptive decision analysis includes in its community many (but not all) theoretical economists, decision analysts, operations researchers, and management scientists.

Behavioral decision analysts have demonstrated in the laboratory and through surveys and clinical studies not only that individuals do not behave according to the rationality assumptions of the prescriptive theories,but also that many individuals continue to behave the same way even after they are made aware of their so-called inconsistencies. “Behavior” as used in this context has three facets: how people perceive risks and think about uncertainties; how with new information they modify these perceptions, that is, how they learn; and how they choose among alternatives when uncertainties are present.

There is a need to foster a closer link between these two communities. Prescriptive theories that are designed to guide behavior should be modified to account for real psychological concerns. In dealing with issues of risk, many people do not think probabilistically, even when they know how to, and the heuristics they use to guide their behavior often are clearly inappropriate. How can some of the insights from prescriptive decision analysis be introduced into the general consciousness of people in ways that will produce more reasonable heuristics for behavior? The aim is to teach people how to think about risks more clearly but not to indoctrinate them into narrow channels that eliminate real psychological concerns.

Further Descriptive Research on Perceptions

It may be appropriate to extend in several directions the research being done on the ways in which people think about risks and about such risk-taking behavior as choice of insurance coverage, choice of occupation, life-style habits, life-saving precautions, and choice of investment. Several general questions are worth pursuing:

  • In perceptions about risks and in risk-taking behavior, what are the differences (if any) between cultures--between the United States and other countries; and, within the United States, between the sexes, races, religious affiliations, rural and city dwellers, socioeconomic groups, people with different levels of education, etc.? What accounts for these differences?
  • How do children think and learn to think about risks? Do competitive sports have an influence on risk attitudes? What are the influences of early education, number of siblings, etc.?
  • How do perceptions of risk relate to beliefs in astrology and how do attitudes toward risk relate to other attitudes, e.g., internal versus external control, fatalism? Is there a relationship between those beliefs and attitudes and the perceptions of risk?
  • What kinds of people are more concerned about what kinds of risks? For example, to what extent and why do risk perceptions differ, for example, between business leaders and governmental officials or between technical experts and lay people?
  • What are the attributes of a risk that people are particularly concerned about (e.g., controllability, catastrophic potential, etc.)?
  • What is the role of the media in shaping and responding to people's perceptions about risks?
  • To what extent are an individual's attitudes toward entrepreneurial risks related to his or her attitudes toward health, safety, and environmental risks?
  • What are the historical trends in people's attitudes toward value trade-offs--between dollars and health, economic growth and environment, and so forth?
  • THE GENERATION OF RISKS

As we have argued, the responses to risks, the coping mechanisms, depend on the class of risks and in turn on how a particular risk is generated. In some instances, such as hang gliding, the policy response is to do nothing; in others, such as child abuse, the response is vigorous.

The design of coping mechanisms and the selection of alternatives is aided by an understanding not only of how risks are generated but also of the milieus in which people tolerate some risks but not others, deliberately expose themselves to risks or with vigor or considerable cost avoid them. Both political and economic milieus may affect attitudes toward taking risks.

Thus, in understanding responses to risk or to its avoidance, it would help to more precisely tabulate the different health, safety, and environmental problems in nations with different political systems (democracies versus autocracies), different economic systems (capitalist versus socialist), or different national mores (e.g., West German versus American). The corollary question is whether some political or economic systems are better able to cope with certain risks than with others. If so, which risks and why? The essential purpose of the research, then, is to clarify the different responses to risks in different political, economic, or national systems and also to analyze the nature of the risks in those countries. What are considered to be serious risks in the U.S.S.R., for example, that are accepted in the United States? Or which risks that concern policy makers in the United States are given a gloss in France?

What is the pattern of causal relationships that link wealth and health? Does “richer mean safer” in countries as wealthy as the United States? How important are the various correlates of high standards of living, including education levels, leisure, and patterns of employment, in determining levels of health? To what extent would a change in the rate of economic growth affect the rate of progress against mortality and morbidity?

  • COPING WITH RISKS

There are many ways to cope with risk, of which Figure 2 lists only a few. Some are used routinely, such as insuring against a fire or car accident; others, such as effluent fees, are intensely discussed in the academic literature but have yet to be used in the United States; others that are applied to one sort of risk may be applicable to others. A critical function of research should be to explore the dimensions of both existing and untried coping strategies, determining the type of risks they may best fit, the implications for decision makers, and associated benefits and costs.

Social Experiments

It is, of course, very difficult to predict with reasonable accuracy the consequences of innovative coping strategies, such as the sale of pollution licenses and changes in tort law. Consequently, research is needed on ways in which new coping strategies can be experimentally tested, especially those for possible new environmental threats, such as acid precipitation, changing climates due to rising levels of carbon dioxide, the erosion of ozone by manufactured chemicals, or indoor pollution. The design of a social experiment in risk-coping should include procedures for monitoring and evaluating the effects of innovations over time with adaptive feedback controls.

International Comparisons

The same research agenda for comparing the attitudes toward and generation of risks in nations differing in their politics, economics, and mores can be applied to an international survey of coping strategies. Certainly in developing a richer array of coping strategies, as suggested above, the United States would benefit from the experience of other industrialized countries; as with the generation of risks, however, little effort to date has been made to compare, contrast, and critically evaluate the tactics in different countries for coping with risks.

Some coping strategies that are employed extensively in other nations, such as effluent fees for pollution control and legal restrictions on self-hazardous behavior, are not used extensively in the United States. What factors explain the difference in usage, what can be learned from foreign experience, and how can the successful strategies from abroad be implemented in the United States?

A Particular Strategy: Monitoring Chemicals

The society is exposed to tens of thousands of chemicals and each year thousands of new chemicals are introduced. Some of these chemicals are noxious without important benefits and clearly have been removed or have not been introduced. Others are clearly benign with copious benefits. Others present problems: Are their uncertain risks and costs worth their uncertain benefits? One question is what to do about a given chemical. A larger and more important question is how to design an entire system to review old and new chemicals--a system that must exploit and perhaps modify given institutional structures to ensure that trivial risks do not overload the system and that proper attention is paid to large risks, whether they are historical (often known risks) or are more uncertain (see Weinstein, 1979, 1981).

That is simply a recognition of the nature of the problem: a huge industry is in place, the national economy depends in part on both the health of the industry and the availability of its products, and new chemicals are constantly introduced and others removed. The issue thus is more than testing a particular chemical; it involves dealing with an industry that is an economic pillar in a manner that strengthens the economy and protects the population. The coping tactics applied to monitoring chemicals are necessarily iterative ones, a constant seeking of an equilibrium among economic, health, safety, and environmental concerns.

Such a strategy means setting priorities, accumulating more experimental evidence, rearranging priorities, and so on. Such a strategy must involve a host of actors, including government agencies, government research facilites, business firms and their research facilities, universities, and industry-sponsored research facilities. It must be designed so that the incentives of the marketplace work, on balance, for the common good. The problem can be characterized as a sequential design for the interaction of experimentation and action in a decentralized environment. Science, assessment, experimentation, evaluation, action, monitoring--all must interact within the confines of our institutional structures.

Is this a researchable topic? Certainly what is now done can be described. The specific procedures of the current strategy can be critiqued and ameliorative, incremental changes can be recommended. What we are doing can be contrasted with what other countries are doing. While their institutions differ, cross-national epidemiological data are still relevant. One might also simplify the problem by ignoring institutional constraints and investigate how a control system might work in a more abstract setting--the hope being that some of the analysis would generate realistic heuristic insights. One might even suggest experimental modifications of the current strategy and carefully monitor and evaluate the performance of the modified system.

Underlying what has been said so far is our society's capacity to deal with aggregated risk: that is, an entire industry rather than any one of its products. New strategies may be needed to deal with that difficulty. Research is needed to consider what these strategies might be, the suitability of our present institutional structures for applying them effectively, their applicability to a particular problem involving aggregate risks, and their probable costs and benefits.

One might also foreshadow the discussion below on the assessment and evaluation stages of risk analysis by noting that the separation of the two stages is quite natural in considering a single chemical, but not at all in coping with a risk analysis en bloc for hundreds or even thousands of chemicals. Also, examination of such “macro risks” demands extensive interdisciplinary interactions.

Taxation Strategies

Economists have published research on the theoretical advantages of tax schemes for coping with various risks (e.g., effluent charges for pollution control and consumption fees for hazardous products). The issue now is whether and how this theoretical work will actually be applicable within political constraints. Can taxation strategies be used effectively to cope with the emerging problem of hazardous wastes? What are the political or institutional impediments to more widespread use of these approaches in the United States, and how can they be made more politically feasible?

Mediation and Arbitration

Power plants, refineries, dumps for toxic chemicals, airports, and incinerators frequently have to be built despite the fact that increased risks may be imposed on the nearby residents. Vociferous arguments occur about the siting of facilities that are deemed on balance beneficial to the majority but unfair to a minority. Ultimately such decisions must be made through a political and judicial process, but analysis may be of assistance in several ways. In particular, is it possible to elicit reliable opinions about costs, benefits, equities, and values from those having a stake in the result? This is a growing research area among economists and policy analysts.

The choice of the site and the type and the size of a facility pose difficult technical problems. If there were no conflicts between interest groups, then optimization techniques, such as operations research methods, could be used. There is a tendency for conflict to drive out analysis, but a case could be made for the reverse: Adding conflict to an already technically difficult choice makes the problem still harder, and analysis might be useful in designing compromise solutions. This is, however, rarely done in practice. In recent years, several environmental disputes have been mediated successfully because the mediator has been astute enough to suggest a new compromise alternative that was not on the table in front of the disputants (see for example Center for Environmental Conflict Resolution, 1978). Part of the mediators' task is to turn a win-lose confrontation mentality into a win-win joint problem-solving task; very rarely has formal analysis been used in this type of joint problem solving.

If towns or regions were monolithic, it might be possible to compensate a locality that accepts a locally noxious facility. Auctions, for example, could be used to determine the amount of financial compensation to the locality accepting the facility. Compensation can come in many guises. One of them is to couple alternatives that raise risks with those that lower them so that the net effect is lessened risk for the locality that accepts a noxious facility. Applied research talent is needed to exploit these ideas, to show the practicality of such interventions in settling disputes.

Innovation, Productivity, and Competition

Political debates about regulatory reform frequently contain assertions about the effects of federal regulation on industrial research and development, productivity, and competition. Reliable economic research does not currently exist to refute or establish most of these claims. Since repeal or modification of many regulations may occur during the tenure of the Reagan administration, a unique opportunity exists to assess the consequences of regulatory reform. Of course, any macroeconomic effects of regulatory changes may be lost in “measurement noise.” However, microeconomic studies of particular firms and industries can bolster cause-and-effect hypotheses. Some issues deserving special attention are: the effects of regulatory changes on small firms and industry market shares; the effects of regulatory uncertainty on research, development, and capital investment decisions; and the positive and negative effects of regulation on measures of economic productivity.

  • RISK ANALYSIS

While, as emphasized in the first part of this report, analysis has a limited role in decision making, it still can be a powerful tool for categorizing risks, assessing them, evaluating a given set of alternative coping strategies, and devising new alternatives. The actual role of risk analysis in particular situations is variable, as are the methods used. There is also the fact that in some decision making on risk, analysis has no role; in other situations analysis is useful only if it is designed so that it provides information that the decision maker cannot obtain. Research that is based largely on retrospective looks at analyses that have been done and their uses would be helpful to both analysts and to decision makers in determining when formal analyses may be useful, operating under what criteria, and involving what sort of dialogue between the analysts and those using their advice.

Such research would inform the committee's belief that formal analyses can often be useful to decision makers at various stages of interaction, either by providing critical information or by helping decision makers to structure their thinking.

As part of learning how to do better risk analyses, existing studies could be reviewed, compared, evaluated, and criticized--and then perhaps redone. Comparisons of studies within a fairly narrow field could be used to discover and elucidate valuable features elements to be included, good methods, etc., versus omissions, pitfalls, inappropriate methods, etc. Comparisons of relatively good studies across fields could be used to discover, elucidate, and illustrate the common features as well as the reasons for differences in emphasis, method, and so on. The goal of studies that redo a risk analysis would be to exploit improvements in methods, better data collection and problem formation, the use of experts, and the communication of results; the purpose is to show how improvements can be made by explicitly building on the partial successes and failures of the past. The studies that have been done of risk analyses are often regarded, justifiably or not, as tainted by adversial biases; that is, they were done by those unhappy with the conclusions of the original work that triggered the follow-up study.

It may be useful to expend considerable effort on analyses of a few specific risks to advance the state of the art of risk analysis. The purpose is to produce analyses of such high quality that they become models in the field. Specific problems should be chosen to illustrate various important features of analysis and with a view toward the transferability of approach, technique, style, organization, and process. There are now very few calculations of risk in the open literature and still fewer with detailed commentaries.

Some of these prototypical analyses might concentrate on risk assessments (i.e., on estimating the magnitudes of health, safety, and environmental hazards); other analyses might concentrate on separating and linking assessment with those evaluations that consider policy alternatives, value trade-offs, political constraints, implementation concerns, and so on; other analyses might concentrate on cases in which the separation between assessment and evaluation is inappropriate or not achievable without crippling distortions. Some “models” might illustrate the usefulness of simple, back-of-the-envelope quick analysis, whereas others might focus on more elaborate time-consuming studies.

At the core of risk assessment and evaluation are experts--generating facts, expressing judgments, influencing values. As expertise becomes more important in social decision making, research is needed about what it is, what its systematic biases are, how it interacts with other sources of knowledge or opinion (such as intuition, common sense, adversarial presentation, etc.), how it is acquired and lost, how experts communicate with lay persons and with other experts, and whether expertise can be isolated from other ingredients of opinion so that others can evaluate those ingredients separately by the different criteria appropriate to each.

Some of the ethical issues that arise in coping with risks, alluded to earlier, include:

  • Distributional equity with respect to the potential costs and benefits of policies that generate or mitigate risks or allow risks to remain unabated;
  • Distributional equity between the interests of people living today and members of future generations with respect to potential costs and benefits of policies that generate or mitigate risks or allow risks to remain unabated;
  • The imposition of risks that will fall on a statistically chosen few for the good of many;
  • Proper balancing between incommensurables like expenditures and the saving of anonymous lives; and
  • The balance between individual freedom of choice and social interventions.

Surveys have asked Americans how they feel about such issues, but the questions usually are vague and ineffective in probing deep ethical values. Can a better job be done in understanding how people really feel about these issues? Can simple questions be devised whose responses correlate well with the results of deeper, more probing, more time-consuming studies? A specific research agenda in this area might include the comparison of the results from standard surveys and in-depth analysis of the same issue. The outcome may aid in determining the most efficient and economical methods of probing the distribution of ethical values in society. If so, will they lead to a better understanding of the distribution of ethical values in society through survey techniques or through studies of small groups of individuals?

  • SYNTHESIZING RISK ASSESSMENTS

Separating assessment from evaluation in risk analyses is difficult but also more commonly done than supposed. It may be done poorly. Nevertheless, the effort is generally made to separate from evaluation those components that are a part of risk assessment, such as the identification of hazards, the review of scientific theories and facts, and the compilation of experimental and epidemiological data. Such separation is attempted because it aids systematic and rational attempts to deal with complex issues.

The synthesis of a risk assessment into a form that is coherent and useful to the evaluator and to the policy maker is, as also noted earlier, invariably more difficult than the original separation, given the disputed interpretations, conflicting or incomplete data, and disparate methodologies that typify many assessments. Accordingly, research into understanding and ameliorating those difficulties is needed.

A clear presentation of indisputable facts dealing with a given uncertainty or uncertainties might suffice for decision and policy purposes. In some specific circumstance, it may be clear that a given chemical is severely carcinogenic and that appropriate, inexpensive substitutes exist; no profound analysis may be necessary to conclude that the chemical should not be used. In other circumstances the evidence may indicate clearly that the chemical has earned an impeccably clean bill of health. In some circumstances, however, the evidence may pull in different directions: theory may pull one way; in vitro tests another way; animal studies of different qualities and credibilities may pull in several contradictory ways; human controlled experiments may be sparse and only indirectly relevant; epidemiological studies may be marred by a host of extraneous complicating factors that make cause and effect almost impossible to sort out. In short, there are cases in which the facts do not speak for themselves. Policy makers may want to know how the experts interpret the data; they may want all the theory and data relating to the uncertainties in question to be synthesized in a way that will be suitable as an input to the policy-evaluation process.

Statisticians are trained to work with other scientists to tease out the inferential meaning of experimental evidence. There are even textbook cases of well-structured problems for which there are formal ways of combining evidence from different sources. But there are a host of real problems that plague risk assessors, for which a reasoned syntheses of the data is extremely difficult, even for the most sophisticated statistician. Synthesis, if it is to be done at all, must incorporate the judgments of experts who have studied the empirical facts and who are knowledgeable about the science involved. The skills of the scientist, the statistician, and the detective have to be combined--creatively and artistically. How can this task be done better? Can the art, if not the science, of synthesis be taught? At the very least, there is need for documentation of case studies of good and bad syntheses. Can people even recognize or agree on good and bad cases of synthesis?

Considerable research has been done in the area of eliciting judgments about uncertainties from a single individual. Some research has been done in statistically validating individual judgments about uncertainties with reality (e.g., how well do meteorologists or stock-market analysts calibrate over time); some research has examined how independent judgments from several experts can be combined and how well these composite assessments calibrate with reality (e.g., pooling betting odds of experts on horse races or other sporting events); some research examines formal iterative techniques that drive experts toward agreement (e.g., Delphi techniques); a great deal of sociological research has examined small group behavior. Despite all this research on component aspects of the problems faced by risk-assessment committees, there are very few case studies of committee processes and deliberations that can be used as appropriate models. There has been considerable research on the Delphi technique, but the reviews are mixed at best and few substitutes have appeared on the scene. The deliberation of experts in committees is a pervasive issue in risk assessment, and much more research should be addressed to the process itself.

In what form, for example, should substantive disagreements among committee members be reported to the evaluator? How can the significance of such disagreements to the central policy questions be stated? Should, in fact, an assessment committee consider the importance of its disagreements to the resolution of policy issues?

Research also is needed on techniques for separating assessment from evaluation, both the effectiveness of current techniques and the development of alternatives. Finally, studies are needed of situations in which separation is impossible or inappropriate. What are the most effective ways to conduct analyses for which decomposition is not done? What are examples of such studies done well? Or done poorly? How can such studies be structured to derive optimum gain from peer review?

  • Cite this Page National Research Council (US) Committee on Risk and Decision Making. Risk and Decision Making: Perspectives and Research. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 1982. RESEARCH ON RISK AND DECISION MAKING.

In this Page

Recent activity.

  • RESEARCH ON RISK AND DECISION MAKING - Risk and Decision Making: Perspectives an... RESEARCH ON RISK AND DECISION MAKING - Risk and Decision Making: Perspectives and Research

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

Decision-Making

Reviewed by Psychology Today Staff

Chocolate or strawberry? Life or death? We make some choices quickly and automatically, relying on mental shortcuts our brains have developed over the years to guide us in the best course of action. Understanding strategies such as maximizing vs. satisficing , fast versus slow thinking, and factors such as risk tolerance and choice overload, can lead to better outcomes.

  • The Art of Decision-Making
  • How to Make Good Decisions
  • Avoiding Bad Decisions

TDallas/Shutterstock

When making a decision, we form opinions and choose actions via mental processes which are influenced by biases, reason, emotions, and memories. The simple act of deciding supports the notion that we have free will . We weigh the benefits and costs of our choice, and then we cope with the consequences. Factors that limit the ability to make good decisions include missing or incomplete information, urgent deadlines, and limited physical or emotional resources.

When people are put in a familiar situation, their decisions are often fast and automatic , based on longtime experience with what works and what doesn’t. However, when encountering a situation they’ve never been in before, they have to take time to weigh the potential benefits and risks when choosing a course of action. They are more likely to make mistakes and face negative consequences.

The ability to think critically is key to making good decisions without succumbing to common errors or bias . This means not just going with your gut, but rather figuring out what knowledge you lack and obtaining it. When you look at all possible sources of information with an open mind, you can make an informed decision based on facts rather than intuition .

A satisficing approach to making decisions involves settling for a good-enough outcome, even if it’s flawed. A maximizing approach, on the other hand, waits for conditions to be as perfect as possible to minimize potential risks. People who make good decisions know when it’s important to act immediately, and when there’s time to wait and gather more facts before making their choice.

Leszek Glasner/Shutterstock

How do we choose between two or more options that seem equally appealing on the surface? Decision-making usually involves a mixture of intuition and rational thinking; critical factors, including personal biases and blind spots, are often unconscious , which makes decision-making hard to fully operationalize, or get a handle on.

However, there are steps to ensure that people make consistently excellent choices, including gathering as much information as possible, considering all the possible alternatives, as well as their attendant benefits and costs, and taking the time to sleep on weightier decisions.

In life, there is often no “right” decision. When surrounded by an abundance of options, it’s easy to experience decision paralysis or feel less satisfied with your decisions. You may even blame yourself when really you are going through “choice overload.” The key is to find ways to simplify your decision and not ruminate over the many roads not taken.

Decision-making can be stressful , and follow-through is essential. You may need to accept that panic , fear , and lack of self-confidence are often part of the decision-making process. It’s crucial to get enough sleep, so you can think clearly. Try to keep your priorities straight. Carefully weigh the trade-offs, commit to a decision, and then follow through on it.

Slow down the decision-making process to prevent impulsive choices. Be aware of common sales strategies like nudges and the decoy effect, which introduces a trick option to get individuals to make a certain decision. Gather as much information as you can, and don’t allow the desires of others to dictate your decision.

baranq/Shutterstock

The field of behavioral economics demonstrated that people are not always rational when it comes to decision making. Fortunately, most personal and professional choices have few or no long-term, negative consequences. However, sometimes a person has to make a decision that will have a profound impact on their future—from who they marry to where they live to how they manage their professional career . In these cases, it’s important to avoid the common pitfalls that can lead to poor decision-making. These can include doing too little or too much research, mistaking opinions for facts, decision fatigue, a failure to learn from past errors, and more.

Don’t try to make the decision you would make, or railroad them into simply acting quickly if they are vacillating about an important matter. Rather, help them cultivate qualities of mind that will serve beyond just this moment, and encourage them to think through their options by simply and respectfully asking questions.

There are two types of rationalization that people commonly engage in: prospective and retrospective. Prospective rationalizing refers to rationalizing a decision before making it, whereas retrospective rationalizing refers to rationalizing a decision after the fact.

In the 2000s, Barry Schwarz coined the phrase the paradox of choice to describe the fact that American consumers have so many choices from which to choose that they often waste time and mind-space second-guessing themselves and comparing trivial differences.

When a large number of people are involved in making a decision, the process can be usurped by groupthink . Groupthink is when well-intentioned individuals make poor or irrational choices out of a desire to conform or avoid dissent. As a result, group members may feel pressured to ignore ethical considerations and refrain from expressing natural doubts and concerns.

decision making research topics

To achieve success, it is important to think for a minute, assess the situation, consider alternatives, and then follow through.

decision making research topics

Do people in some countries generally cheat more often than people in other countries? Two international teams of researchers think not.

decision making research topics

With challenges in our lives also come lessons. The key to successfully running your life is knowing your vulnerabilities. Here's where to start.

decision making research topics

Your aging brain knows how to match the function of the other bits of your body. We just have to alter how we do things to match the conditions across the lifespan.

decision making research topics

Every time you browse the internet or social media, you’re vulnerable to influence, but there are ways to protect yourself.

decision making research topics

Temu has become one of consumers’ most popular apps. Its secrets are unbelievably low prices and psychology hacks that get consumers to keep shopping.

decision making research topics

It's important to have individual boundaries that help you be your best. But boundaries as a couple that protect your relationship against the outside world are also essential.

decision making research topics

How can you find clarity amidst career chaos? Learn practical steps to regain focus and direction so you know your purpose and have a clear path forward.

Psychologist, therapy and woman talking in consultation for mental health in office.

Here are steps to take to advocate for your health and transform your medical experiences.

decision making research topics

A deep dive into pharmaceutical advertising and what a new FDA ruling could mean for the industry and the consumer.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Teletherapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Therapy Center NEW
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

March 2024 magazine cover

Understanding what emotional intelligence looks like and the steps needed to improve it could light a path to a more emotionally adept world.

  • Coronavirus Disease 2019
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Cart

  • SUGGESTED TOPICS
  • The Magazine
  • Newsletters
  • Managing Yourself
  • Managing Teams
  • Work-life Balance
  • The Big Idea
  • Data & Visuals
  • Reading Lists
  • Case Selections
  • HBR Learning
  • Topic Feeds
  • Account Settings
  • Email Preferences

The Art of Asking Smarter Questions

  • Arnaud Chevallier,
  • Frédéric Dalsace,
  • Jean-Louis Barsoux

decision making research topics

With organizations of all sorts facing increased urgency and unpredictability, being able to ask smart questions has become key. But unlike lawyers, doctors, and psychologists, business professionals are not formally trained on what kinds of questions to ask when approaching a problem. They must learn as they go. In their research and consulting, the authors have seen that certain kinds of questions have gained resonance across the business world. In a three-year project they asked executives to brainstorm about the decisions they’ve faced and the kinds of inquiry they’ve pursued. In this article they share what they’ve learned and offer a practical framework for the five types of questions to ask during strategic decision-making: investigative, speculative, productive, interpretive, and subjective. By attending to each, leaders and teams can become more likely to cover all the areas that need to be explored, and they’ll surface information and options they might otherwise have missed.

These five techniques can drive great strategic decision-making.

Idea in Brief

The situation.

With organizations of all sorts facing increased urgency and uncertainty, the ability to ask smart questions has become key. But business professionals aren’t formally trained in that skill.

Why It’s So Challenging

Managers’ expertise often blinds them to new ideas. And the flow of questions can be hard to process in real time, so certain concerns and insights may never be raised.

Strategic questions can be grouped into five domains: investigative, speculative, productive, interpretive, and subjective. By attending to each, leaders and teams are more likely to cover all the areas that need to be explored—and they’ll surface information and options they might otherwise have missed.

As a cofounder and the CEO of the U.S. chipmaker Nvidia, Jensen Huang operates in a high-velocity industry requiring agile, innovative thinking. Reflecting on how his leadership style has evolved, he told the New York Times, “I probably give fewer answers and I ask a lot more questions….It’s almost possible now for me to go through a day and do nothing but ask questions.” He continued, “Through probing, I help [my management team]…explore ideas that they didn’t realize needed to be explored.”

  • Arnaud Chevallier is a professor of strategy at IMD Business School.
  • Frédéric Dalsace is a professor of marketing and strategy at IMD.
  • Jean-Louis Barsoux is a term research professor at IMD and a coauthor of ALIEN Thinking: The Unconventional Path to Breakthrough Ideas (PublicAffairs, 2021).

Partner Center

A theoretical framework to guide AI ethical decision making

  • Theory/Conceptual
  • Published: 13 April 2024

Cite this article

  • O. C. Ferrell   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-2841-7022 1 ,
  • Dana E. Harrison 2 ,
  • Linda K. Ferrell 1 ,
  • Haya Ajjan 3 &
  • Bryan W. Hochstein 4  

Artificial Intelligence (AI) ethics is needed to address the risks that are outpacing efforts to protect consumers and society. AI is becoming human-competitive with the ability to perform tasks, that without controls, can result in harmful or destructive actions. Principles are currently the most discussed ethical approach for pervasive boundaries for algorithmic rule-based intelligence. Principles, values, norms, and rules should be the foundation of an ethical corporate culture with all participants aware of and involved in developing AI ethics. To address these concerns, a theory-based decision framework is presented to incorporate ethical considerations into AI applications. With limited discussion on frameworks to manage AI ethics, we provide a modification of the Hunt–Vitell (H–V) ethical decision model to provide a supportive theoretical framework. This model considers the cultural, industry, organizational, and legal standards that shape AI ethical decision making. The model is based on individual decision making and parallels the decision process in autonomous AI system decision making. Topics for additional research are advanced to create expanded knowledge on this topic.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

decision making research topics

Access, N. (2018, November). Human rights in the age of artificial intelligence. https://www.accessnow.org/cms/assets/uploads/2018/11/AI-and-Human-Rights.pdf .

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behavior. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 173–221.), Erlbaum.

Ansari, M. P., & Singh, D. K. (2023). Identifying human activities in megastores through postural data to monitor shoplifting events. Neural Computing and Applications , 35 , 6515–6528.

Article   Google Scholar  

Beijing, A. I. P. (2019, May 25). Beijing Academy of Artificial Intelligence. JournalismAI. https://journalismai.com/2019/05/25/beijing-ai-principles-beijing-academy-of-artificial-intelligence-2019 .

Bock, D. E., Wolter, J., & Ferrell, O. C. (2020). Artificial intelligence: Disrupting what we know about services. Journal of Services Marketing , 34 (3), 317–334.

Boose, M., & Dean, F. (2000). Analyzing ethical decision-making: Applying the Hunt-Vitell model in insurance courses. Risk and Management Insurance Review , 3 (2), 237–249. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6296.2000.tb00032.x .

Bubeck, S., Chandrasekaran, V., Eldan, R., Gehrke, J., Horvitz, E., Kamar, E., Lee, P., Lee, Y. T., Li, Y., Lundberg, S., Nori, H., Palangi, H., Ribeiro, M. T., & Zhang, Y. (2023). Sparks of artificial general intelligence: Early experiments with GPT-4. arXiv preprint arXiv:2303.12712.

Buhmann, A., Paßmann, J., & Fieseler, C. (2019). Managing algorithmic accountability: Balancing reputational concerns, engagement strategies, and the potential of rational discourse. Journal of Business Ethics , 163 , 265–280.

Burström, T., Parida, V., Lahti, T., & Wincent, J. (2021). AI-enabled business-model innovation and transformation in industrial ecosystems: A framework, model and outline for further research. Journal of Business Research , 127 , 85–95.

Casali, G. L., & Perano, M. (2021). Forty years of research on factors influencing ethical decision making: Establishing a future research agenda. Journal of Business Research , 132 , 614–630.

Cerullo, M. (2023, March 22). Cybercriminals are using AI voice cloning tools to dupe victims. CBS News. https://www.cbsnews.com/philadelphia/news/ai-scam-voice-cloning-rising-ftc/ .

Chen, A. (2018). July 26). IBM’s Watson gave unsafe recommendations for treating cancer. The Verge . https://www.theverge.com/2018/7/26/17619382/ibms-watson-cancer-ai-healthcare-science .

Cui, Y. G., van Esch, P., & Jain, S. P. (2021). Just walk out: The effect of AI-enabled checkouts . European Journal of Marketing.

Davenport, T. (2019, March 2). What we talk about when we talk about AI. https://www.tomdavenport.com/what-we-talk-about-when-we-talk-about-ai .

Davenport, T. H., & Kirby, J. (2016). Just how smart are smart machines? MIT Sloan Management Review , 57 (3), 21.

Google Scholar  

Davenport, T. H., & Ronanki, R. (2018). Artificial intelligence for the real world. Harvard Business Review , 96 (1), 108–116.

Davenport, T., Guha, A., Grewal, D., & Bressgott, T. (2020). How artificial intelligence will change the future of marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science , 48 (1), 24–42.

de Vries, T., Misra, I., Wang, C., & van der Maaten, L. (2019). Does object recognition work for everyone? Proceedings of the IEEE/CVF Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops , 52 , 59.

Deloitte (2023). Ethical technology: Everyone’s responsibility. https://www2.deloitte.com/us/en/pages/about-deloitte/articles/technology-trust-ethics-annual-report.html .

Dholakia, N., & Firat, A. F. (2019). Markets, consumers and society in the age of heteromation. European Journal of Marketing , 53 (8), 1504–1520.

Du, S., & Xie, C. (2021). Paradoxes of artificial intelligence in consumer markets: Ethical challenges and opportunities. Journal of Business Research , 129 , 961–974.

Etzioni, A., & Etzioni, O. (2016). AI assisted ethics. Ethics Information Technology , 18 , 149–156.

Ferrell, O. C., & Ferrell, L. (2021). Applying the Hunt Vitell ethics model to artificial intelligence ethics. Journal of Global Scholars of Marketing Science , 31 (2), 178–188.

Ferrell, O. C., Fraedrich, J., & Ferrell, L. (2022). Business ethics: Ethical decision making and cases . Cengage.

Fjeld, J., Achten, N., Hilligoss, H., Nagy, A., & Srikumar, M. (2020). Principled artificial intelligence: Mapping consensus in ethical and rights-based approaches to principles for AI. Berkman Klein Center Research Publication , (2020-1) , https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3518482 .

Ghallab, M. (2019). Responsible A.I.: Requirements and challenges. AI Perspectives , 1 (1), 1–7.

Grant, N., & Weise, K. (2023, April 7). In A.I. race, Microsoft and Google choose speed over caution: Technology companies were once leery of what some artificial intelligence could do. Now the priority is winning control of the industry’s next big thing. The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2023/04/07/technology/ai-chatbots-google-microsoft.html .

Greene, D., Hoffmann, A. L., & Stark, L. (2019). Better, nicer, clearer, fairer: A critical assessment of the movement for ethical artificial intelligence and machine learning. Proceedings of the 52nd Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences .

Hagendorff, T. (2020). The ethics of AI ethics: An evaluation of guidelines. Minds and Machines , 30 , 99–120.

Hao, K. (2019, April 5). Facebook’s ad-serving algorithm discriminates by gender and race. Technology Review . https://www.technologyreview.com/2019/04/05/1175/facebook-algorithm-discriminates-ai-bias/ .

Harrison, D., Ajjan, H., Ferrell, O., Hochstein, B., & Ferrell, L. (2023). The disconnect of AI principles and decision making: A path forward, working paper.

Hermann, E. (2021). Leveraging artificial intelligence in marketing for social good—An ethical perspective. Journal of Business Ethics , 179 , 1–19.

Huang, M. H. (2017). Technology in the frontline: From dumb to thinking to feeling. Journal of Service Research , 20 (1), 93–95.

Huang, M. H., & Rust, R. T. (2018). Artificial intelligence in service. Journal of Service Research , 21 (2), 155–172.

Hunt, S. D., & Laverie, D. A. (2004). Experiential learning and the Hunt-Vitell theory of ethics: Teaching marketing ethics by integrating theory and practice. Marketing Education Review , 14 (3), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/10528008.2004.11488874 .

Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. (1986). A general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of Macromarketing , 6 (1), 5–16.

Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. M. (2005). The general theory of marketing ethics: A revision and three questions. Journal of Macromarketing , 26 , 143–153.

Hustedt, C., & Fetic, L. (2020, April 2). From principles to practice: How can we make AI ethics measurable? Ethics of Algorithms. https://ethicsofalgorithms.org/2020/04/02/from-principles-to-practice-how-can-we-make-ai-ethics-measurable/ .

IBM (2022). Everyday ethics for artificial intelligence. https://www.ibm.com/watson/assets/duo/pdf/everydayethics.pdf .

Jobin, A., Ienca, M., & Vayena, E. (2019). The global landscape of AI ethics guidelines. Nature Machine Intelligence , 1 , 389–399. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42256-019-0088-2 .

Kaplan, A., & Haenlein, M. (2019). Siri, Siri, in my hand: Who’s the fairest in the land? On the interpretations, illustrations, and implications of artificial intelligence. Business Horizons , 62 (1), 15–25.

Kaplan, A., & Haenlein, M. (2020). Rulers of the world, unite! The challenges and opportunities of artificial intelligence. Business Horizons , 63 (1), 37–50.

Kaplan, R. S., & Mikes, A. (2012). Managing risks: A new framework. Harvard Business Review , 90 , 48–60.

Kelley, S. (2022). Employee perceptions of the effective adoption of AI principles. Journal of Business Ethics , 178 , 871–893. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-022-05051-y .

Khan, L. (2023, May 3). FTC chair says agency is watching for ‘unfair or deceptive use of AI use.’ The Hill. https://thehill.com/policy/technology/3985771-ftc-chair-says-agency-watching-for-unfair-or-deceptive-ai-use/ .

Koshiyama, A., Kazim, E., Treleaven, P., Rai, P., Szpruch, L., Pavey, G., Ahamat, G., Leutner, F., Goebe, R., Knight, A., Adams, J., Hitrova, C., Barnett, J., Nachev, P., Barber, D., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Klemmer, K., Gregorovic, M., Khan, S., & Lomas, E. (2021). Towards algorithm auditing: A survey on managing legal, ethical and technological risks of AI, ML and associated algorithms. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3778998 .

Krafft, T., Hauer, M., Fetic, L., Kaminski, A., Puntschuh, M., Otto, P., Hubig, C., Fleischer, T., Grünke, P., Hillerbrand, R., Hustedt, C., & Hallensleben, S. (2020). From principles to practice: An interdisciplinary framework to operationalise AI ethics. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/340378463_From_Principles_to_Practice_-_An_interdisciplinary_framework_to_operationalise_AI_ethics .

Laurance, B. (2019, September 16). AI: Ethics must be the starting point – OECD. London Business School . https://www.london.edu/think/iie-ai-significant-potential-but-ethics-must-be-the-starting-point-says-oecd .

Lee, L. W., Dabirian, A., McCarthy, I. P., & Kietzmann, J. (2020). Making sense of text: AI-enabled content analysis. European Journal of Marketing , 54 (3), 615–644.

Leone, D., Schiavone, F., Appio, F. P., & Chiao, B. (2021). How does artificial intelligence enable and enhance value co-creation in industrial markets? An exploratory case study in the healthcare ecosystem. Journal of Business Research , 129 , 849–859.

Li, M. (2020, October 26). To build less-biased AI, hire a more diverse team. Harvard Business Review. https://hbr.org/2020/10/to-build-less-biased-ai-hire-a-more-diverse-team .

Lobschat, L., Mueller, B., Eggers, F., Brandimarte, L., Diefenbach, S., Kroschke, M., & Wirtz, J. (2021). Corporate digital responsibility. Journal of Business Research , 122 , 875–888.

Loureiro, S. M. C., Guerreiro, J., & Tussyadiah, I. (2020). Artificial intelligence in business: State of the art and future research agenda. Journal of Business Research , 129 , 911–926.

Lurie, Y. (2004). Humanizing business through emotions: On the role of emotions in ethics. Journal of Business Ethics , 49 , 1–11.

Makarius, E. E., Mukherjee, D., Fox, J. D., & Fox, A. K. (2020). Rising with the machines: A sociotechnical framework for bringing artificial intelligence into the organization. Journal of Business Research , 120 , 262–273.

Manis, K. T., & Madhavaram, S. (2023). AI-Enabled marketing capabilities and the hierarchy of capabilities: Conceptualization, proposition development, and research avenues. Journal of Business Research , 157 , 113485.

Markus, M. L. (2017). Datification, organizational strategy, and IS research: What’s the score? Journal of Strategic Information Systems , 26 (3), 233–241.

Martin, K. (2018). Ethical implications and accountability of algorithms. Journal of Business Ethics , 160 (4), 835–850.

Martin, K. E., & Freeman, R. E. (2004). The separation of technology and ethics in business ethics. Journal of Business Ethics , 53 (4), 353–364.

Martin, K., Shilton, K., & Smith, J. (2019). Business and the ethical implications of technology: Introduction to the symposium. Journal of Business Ethics , 160 (2), 307–317.

Mayo, M. A., & Marks, L. J. (1990). An empirical investigation of a general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science , 18 (2), 163–171.

McNamara, A., Smith, J., & Murphy-Hill, E. (2018). Does ACM’s code of ethics change ethical decision-making in software development? Proceedings of the 2018 26th ACM Joint Meeting on European Software Engineering Conference and Symposium on the Foundations of Software Engineering , 729–733.

Metz, C., & Schmidt, G. (2023, March 29). Elon Musk and others call for pause on A.I., citing ‘profound risks to society.’ The New York Times , https://www.nytimes.com/2023/03/29/technology/ai-artificial-intelligence-musk-risks.html .

Miller, A. P., & Hosanagar, K. (2019). November 8). How targeted ads and dynamic pricing can perpetuate bias. Harvard Business Review . https://hbr.org/2019/11/how-targeted-ads-and-dynamic-pricing-can-perpetuate-bias .

Morley, J., Floridi, L., Kinsey, L., & Elhalal, A. (2020). From what to how: An initial review of publicly available AI ethics tools, methods and research to translate principles into practices. Science and Engineering Ethics , 26 (4), 2141–2168.

Morris, D. (2020, January 31). How marketers are increasingly using A.I. to persuade you to buy. Fortune . https://fortune.com/2020/01/31/ai-marketing-persuade/ .

Morse, L., Teodorescu, M. H. M., Awwad, Y., & Kane, G. C. (2021). Do the ends justify the means? Variation in the distributive and procedural fairness of machine learning algorithms. Journal of Business Ethics , 181 (4), 1–13.

Mustak, M., Salminen, J., Plé, L., & Wirtz, J. (2021). Artificial intelligence in marketing: Topic modeling, scientometric analysis, and research agenda. Journal of Business Research , 124 , 389–404.

OpenAI (2023). GPT-4 technical report. https://cdn.openai.com/papers/gpt-4.pdf .

Perez-Vega, R., Kaartemo, V., Lages, C. R., Razavi, N. B., & Männistö, J. (2021). Reshaping the contexts of online customer engagement behavior via artificial intelligence: A conceptual framework. Journal of Business Research , 129 , 902–910.

Pichai, S. (2018, June 7). AI at Google: Our principles. Google Blog. https://www.blog.google/technology/ai/ai-principles/ .

Prabbakaron, V., Qadri, R., & Hutchinson, B. (2022). Incongruencies in artificial intelligence. First Workshop on AI/AI in Culture . https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2211.13069 .

PwC Analysis (2017). Sizing the prize: PwC’s global artificial intelligence study: Exploiting the AI revolution. https://www.pwc.com/gx/en/issues/data-and-analytics/publications/artificial-intelligence-study.html .

Rainie, L., Anderson, J., & Vogels, E. A. (2021). Experts doubt ethical AI design will be broadly adopted as the norm within the next decade. Pew Research Center . https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2021/06/16/experts-doubt-ethical-ai-design-will-be-broadly-adopted-as-the-norm-within-the-next-decade/ .

Roose, K. (2023, February 16). A conversation with Bing’s chatbot left me deeply unsettled: A very strange conversation with the chatbot built into Microsoft’s search engine led to it declaring its love for me. The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2023/02/16/technology/bing-chatbot-microsoft-chatgpt.html .

Shankar, V. (2018). How artificial intelligence (AI) is reshaping retailing. Journal of Retailing , 94 (4), 6–11.

Shrestha, Y. R., Krishna, V., & von Krogh, G. (2021). Augmenting organizational decision-making with deep learning algorithms: Principles, promises, and challenges. Journal of Business Research , 123 , 588–603.

Shumanov, M., Copper, H., & Ewing, M. (2021). Using AI predicted personality to enhance advertising effectiveness. European Journal of Marketing , 1–20.

Singer, N. (2018, July 26). Amazon’s facial recognition wrongly identifies 28 lawmakers, A.C.L.U. says. The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2018/07/26/technology/amazon-aclu-facial-recognition-congress.html .

Singh, J., Flaherty, K., Sohi, R. S., Deeter-Schmelz, D., Habel, J., Le Meunier-FitzHugh, K., Malshe, A., Mullins, R., & Onyemah, V. (2019). Sales profession and professionals in the age of digitization and artificial intelligence technologies: Concepts, priorities, and questions (pp. 2–22). Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management.

Syam, N., & Sharma, A. (2018). Waiting for a sales renaissance in the fourth industrial revolution: Machine learning and artificial intelligence in sales research and practice. Industrial Marketing Management , 69 , 135–146.

Toti, J. F., Diallo, M. F., & Huaman-Ramirez, R. (2021). Ethical sensitivity in consumers’ decision-making: The mediating and moderating role of internal locus of control. Journal of Business Research , 131 , 168–182.

University of Montreal (2018). Montreal Declaration. https://www.montrealdeclaration-responsibleai.com/the-declaration .

Vermillion, L. J., Lassar, W. M., & Winsor, R. D. (2002). The Hunt–Vitell general theory of marketing ethics: Can it enhance our understanding of principal-agent relationships in channels of distribution? Journal of Business Ethics , 41 (3), 267–285.

Vincent, J. (2019, June 11). AI is worse at identifying household items from lower-income countries. The Verge. https://www.theverge.com/2019/6/11/18661128/ai-object-recognition-algorithms-bias-worse-household-items-lower-income-countries .

Virzi, A. M., & Yeager, B. (2018). Marketing technology survey 2018: Martech adoption surges as brands pursue personalization, measurement and advertising. Gartner. https://www.gartner.com/en/documents/3891975/marketing-technology-survey-2018-martech-adoption-surges .

Vitell, S. J. (2021). Shelby D. Hunt: Contributions of a pioneer in marketing ethics and micromarketing. Journal of Global Scholars of Marketing Science , 31 (2), 151–161.

Vlačić, B., Corbo, L., Silva, S. C., & Dabić, M. (2021). The evolving role of artificial intelligence in marketing: A review and research agenda. Journal of Business Research , 128 , 187–203.

Watson, H. J., & Nations, C. (2019). Addressing the growing need for algorithmic transparency. Communications of the Association for Information Systems , 45 (1), 26.

Wirtz, J., Patterson, P., Kunz, W., Gruber, T., Lu, V., Paluch, S., & Martins, A. (2018). Brave new world: Service robots in the frontline. Journal of Service Management , 29 (5), 907–931.

Wirtz, J., Kunz, W., Hartley, N., & Tarbit, J. (2023). Corporate digital responsibility in service firms and their ecosystems. Journal of Service Research , 26 (2), 173–190.

Yang, J. R. (2020). Three ways AI can discriminate in hiring and three ways forward. Urban Institute. https://www.urban.org/urban-wire/three-ways-ai-can-discriminate-hiring-and-three-ways-forward .

Zuboff, S. (2019). The age of surveillance capitalism: The fight for a human future at the new frontier of power . Hachette Book Group.

Download references

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose. All authors certify that they have no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest or non-financial interest in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript. The authors have no financial or proprietary interests in any material discussed in this article

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Harbert College of Business, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, 36849, USA

O. C. Ferrell & Linda K. Ferrell

College of Business and Technology, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN, 37614, USA

Dana E. Harrison

Martha and Spencer Love School of Business, Elon University, Elon, NC, 27244, USA

Culverhouse College of Commerce, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, AL, 35487, USA

Bryan W. Hochstein

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to O. C. Ferrell .

Ethics declarations

Coflict of interests.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Ferrell, O.C., Harrison, D.E., Ferrell, L.K. et al. A theoretical framework to guide AI ethical decision making. AMS Rev (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13162-024-00275-9

Download citation

Received : 30 May 2023

Accepted : 19 March 2024

Published : 13 April 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s13162-024-00275-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Artificial intelligence
  • Hunt–Vitell model
  • AI ethical decision making
  • AI principles
  • Ethics principles
  • Business ethics
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Publications

Decision making for patients with severe dementia versus normal cognition near the end of life

Innovation in Aging August 1, 2023

Read the full article

Research Areas

  • Communication & Decision Making

PAIR Center Research Team

Headshot of Scott Halpern

Scott Halpern

  • Advance Care Planning

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: The clinical progression of severe dementia frequently leads to situations where surrogate decision makers must quickly make choices about potentially burdensome treatments that offer limited clinical benefit. We examined whether the number of decision makers and their access to advance directives were related to treatment choice for patients with severe dementia in comparison to those with normal cognition.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS: We retrospectively linked survey responses about end-of-life treatment decisions to Medicare claims for Health and Retirement Study respondents dying between 2002 and 2015 whose next-of-kin reported a need for surrogate decision making. We estimated multivariable logistic regression models to study measures of aggressive care in the last 6 months of life; in-hospital death, burdensome transfers, and burdensome treatments.

RESULTS: Compared to patients who were cognitively normal near the end of life (n = 1 198), patients with severe dementia (n = 722) were less likely to experience burdensome treatments (18% [95% confidence interval {CI} 14–21] vs 32% [95% CI 29–35]), burdensome transfers (20% [95% CI 17–24] vs 30% [95% CI 27–33]), and in-hospital death (24% [95% CI 20–28] vs 30% [95% CI 26–33]) when surrogates were involved. Rates of burdensome treatments, transfers, or in-hospital death for decedents with severe dementia did not vary with single versus multiple decision makers or when decision makers were informed by advance directives. However, among decedents with normal cognition, a single decision maker informed by an advance directive was associated with the lowest rates of burdensome treatments and in-hospital death.

DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS: Surrogate decision makers made similar choices around end-of-life care for patients with severe dementia regardless of the number of decision makers and availability of advance directives. However, both advance directives and single decision makers were associated with less aggressive care for cognitively normal decedents.

National Institute on Aging

Lauren Hersch Nicholas, Scott D Halpern, David R Weir, Micah Y Baum, Marie Nolan, Joseph Gallo, Kenneth M Langa

Related Publications

Moving “ourcarewishes” to the bedside: a step-wedge pragmatic trial, accuracy of expected symptoms and subsequent quality of life measures among adults with copd.

  • Alzheimer's disease & dementia
  • Arthritis & Rheumatism
  • Attention deficit disorders
  • Autism spectrum disorders
  • Biomedical technology
  • Diseases, Conditions, Syndromes
  • Endocrinology & Metabolism
  • Gastroenterology
  • Gerontology & Geriatrics
  • Health informatics
  • Inflammatory disorders
  • Medical economics
  • Medical research
  • Medications
  • Neuroscience
  • Obstetrics & gynaecology
  • Oncology & Cancer
  • Ophthalmology
  • Overweight & Obesity
  • Parkinson's & Movement disorders
  • Psychology & Psychiatry
  • Radiology & Imaging
  • Sleep disorders
  • Sports medicine & Kinesiology
  • Vaccination
  • Breast cancer
  • Cardiovascular disease
  • Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease
  • Colon cancer
  • Coronary artery disease
  • Heart attack
  • Heart disease
  • High blood pressure
  • Kidney disease
  • Lung cancer
  • Multiple sclerosis
  • Myocardial infarction
  • Ovarian cancer
  • Post traumatic stress disorder
  • Rheumatoid arthritis
  • Schizophrenia
  • Skin cancer
  • Type 2 diabetes
  • Full List »

share this!

April 10, 2024

This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies . Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

AI will provide heart transplant surgeons with new decision-making data, say researchers

by International Society for Heart and Lung Transplantation

patient in gurney

Artificial intelligence will significantly impact the heart transplantation process by helping physicians better assess the complex factors impacting patient outcomes, according to researchers at today's Annual Meeting and Scientific Sessions of the International Society of Heart and Lung Transplantation ( ISHLT ) in Prague.

"Until now, we've assessed the likelihood of transplant success based on individual risk factors," said Eileen Hsich, medical director of the Heart Transplant Program at the Cleveland Clinic. "I think our guidelines will change because we'll be able to look at combinations of weighted risk factors and how they interplay.

"That work cannot be done manually," she said. "Machine learning can provide data we've never had before, and it will make a big difference."

Johan Nilsson, MD, Ph.D., senior consultant cardiothoracic surgeon at Skane University Hospital and professor at Lund University in Lund, Sweden, is developing a decision-support tool for transplant surgeons using a modeling technique called "digital twinning."

"This technique enables us to create a digital picture of each recipient that can help physicians predict future patient outcomes based on specific combinations of data," he said.

Dr. Nilsson's team has built a database with multiple data points, including clinical information and test results, on all 600 heart recipients and donors treated at his institution since the inception of its transplantation program. They are also in the process of sequencing the whole genome of recipients and their respective donors to add to the database.

"Patients added to an organ transplant waiting list provide a lot of health data and laboratory samples," he said. "The same information is also gathered from the donor."

Dr. Nilsson plans to continue to follow heart recipients after their procedure, adding information such as heart rate , blood oxygen levels, and biopsy results to the database.

"The advantage of algorithms is they provide an unbiased decision-support system that can help physicians determine the 3 Rs: the Right donor at the Right time with the Right patient," he said. "If you get a donor in the middle of the night and you have three potential recipients, AI can offer an independent system to help determine the best match."

Dr. Nilsson said AI could help improve organ allocation systems by helping physicians better predict outcomes at each step of the transplantation process.

Explore further

Feedback to editors

decision making research topics

Carbon beads help restore healthy gut microbiome and reduce liver disease progression, researchers find

4 hours ago

decision making research topics

Untangling dreams and our waking lives: Latest findings in cognitive neuroscience

10 hours ago

decision making research topics

Researchers demonstrate miniature brain stimulator in humans

Apr 13, 2024

decision making research topics

Study reveals potential to reverse lung fibrosis using the body's own healing technique

Apr 12, 2024

decision making research topics

Researchers discover cell 'crosstalk' that triggers cancer cachexia

decision making research topics

Study improves understanding of effects of household air pollution during pregnancy

decision making research topics

Wearable sensors for Parkinson's can improve with machine learning, data from healthy adults

decision making research topics

New insights on B cells: Researchers explore building better antibodies and curbing autoimmune diseases

decision making research topics

Grieving pet owners comforted by 'supernatural' interactions

decision making research topics

Study shows AI improves accuracy of skin cancer diagnoses

Related stories.

decision making research topics

Older organs found to accelerate aging in transplant recipients in preclinical models

Dec 5, 2023

Five ways AI promises to transform organ transplants

Mar 21, 2023

decision making research topics

Video: How innovation is transforming heart transplants

Dec 1, 2023

decision making research topics

Heart transplant doctors could help more people by accepting donations from the obese

Jul 24, 2019

decision making research topics

Transplant of organs from SARS-CoV-2-positive donors safe, finds study

Feb 27, 2023

decision making research topics

New method of donor-lung distribution expected to decrease deaths among those on transplant waiting list

Jan 5, 2023

Recommended for you

decision making research topics

New AI method captures uncertainty in medical images

Apr 11, 2024

decision making research topics

Infections after surgery are more likely due to bacteria already on your skin than from microbes in the hospital: Study

decision making research topics

Scientists use wearable technology to detect stress levels during sleep

decision making research topics

Decoding spontaneous thoughts from the brain via machine learning

decision making research topics

COVID-19 vaccine effectiveness: Results from Norway demonstrate the reproducibility of federated analytics

Let us know if there is a problem with our content.

Use this form if you have come across a typo, inaccuracy or would like to send an edit request for the content on this page. For general inquiries, please use our contact form . For general feedback, use the public comments section below (please adhere to guidelines ).

Please select the most appropriate category to facilitate processing of your request

Thank you for taking time to provide your feedback to the editors.

Your feedback is important to us. However, we do not guarantee individual replies due to the high volume of messages.

E-mail the story

Your email address is used only to let the recipient know who sent the email. Neither your address nor the recipient's address will be used for any other purpose. The information you enter will appear in your e-mail message and is not retained by Medical Xpress in any form.

Newsletter sign up

Get weekly and/or daily updates delivered to your inbox. You can unsubscribe at any time and we'll never share your details to third parties.

More information Privacy policy

Donate and enjoy an ad-free experience

We keep our content available to everyone. Consider supporting Science X's mission by getting a premium account.

E-mail newsletter

  • Frontiers in High Performance Computing
  • Cloud Computing
  • Research Topics

Software-driven Synergy between Internet of Things and High Performance Computing.

Total Downloads

Total Views and Downloads

About this Research Topic

The Internet of Things (IoT) has revolutionized the way we interact with the world, connecting billions of devices and enabling seamless communication and data exchange. With IoT, objects such as sensors, actuators, and everyday devices can collect and transmit vast amounts of data, creating a massive influx of information. High Performance Computing (HPC), characterized by its immense processing power via super-computing, fast-network, vast-storage, and substantial-memory capabilities, enables the execution of complex computations and data-intensive tasks at unprecedented speeds. Given the shared interests in big data analytics, it has been identified that HPC can help address the requirements and challenges of IoT in terms of efficiently processing/analyzing data and distilling insights, while IoT brings new chances as well as evolution directions to HPC. By combining the capabilities of IoT and HPC, researchers and practitioners can unlock more opportunities for data-driven knowledge creation, real-time decision-making, and advanced automation. The synergy between IoT and HPC also presents several challenges that this Research Topic aims to address. Firstly, there is a need for efficient data management and processing techniques to handle the enormous volumes of data generated by IoT devices. This includes developing scalable algorithms, optimizing data storage, and implementing real-time analytics to extract valuable information from the data deluge. Additionally, the integration of IoT and HPC raises concerns regarding security, privacy, and reliability. As IoT devices become more pervasive, the vulnerability to cyber threats increases. The research topic will explore novel approaches to secure IoT-HPC systems, safeguard sensitive data, and ensure the integrity and availability of the infrastructure. While the hardware aspects of IoT and HPC have received considerable attention, the role of software in enabling their integration and maximizing their combined potential remains relatively unexplored. By emphasizing software frameworks, architectures, and algorithms, this topic offers a comprehensive understanding of how to effectively leverage the synergy between these two domains. This Research Topic fosters collaboration and knowledge exchange among leading researchers, bringing together experts from diverse backgrounds, including IoT, HPC, cloud computing, data analytics, and cybersecurity, to collectively address the challenges and push the boundaries of this exciting research area. Topics of interest include, but are not limited to: • Exploring the integration of IoT and HPC • Case studies on the pivotal role of software in harnessing the combined potential of IoT and HPC • Hybridization of distributed and centralized data processing • Modular and scalable high-performance computing • HPC problems and applications in the IoT context • Device-to-cloud integration platform • Device-to-cloud data processing pipeline • Supporting software/middleware for fusing heterogeneous systems • IoT-native software engineering • Architectural patterns and tactics for privacy preservation in IoT • Application benchmarks and workloads for IoT-friendly HPC

Keywords : internet of Things, high performance computing, software, optimization, data processing.

Important Note : All contributions to this Research Topic must be within the scope of the section and journal to which they are submitted, as defined in their mission statements. Frontiers reserves the right to guide an out-of-scope manuscript to a more suitable section or journal at any stage of peer review.

Topic Editors

Topic coordinators, submission deadlines, participating journals.

Manuscripts can be submitted to this Research Topic via the following journals:

total views

  • Demographics

No records found

total views article views downloads topic views

Top countries

Top referring sites, about frontiers research topics.

With their unique mixes of varied contributions from Original Research to Review Articles, Research Topics unify the most influential researchers, the latest key findings and historical advances in a hot research area! Find out more on how to host your own Frontiers Research Topic or contribute to one as an author.

IMAGES

  1. 5 Steps To Making Great Decisions Using Decision Tree Analysis

    decision making research topics

  2. 15+ Decision Tree Infographics for Decision Making

    decision making research topics

  3. How to Master the Decision-Making Process in 6 Steps (2022)

    decision making research topics

  4. Problem-Solving and Decision Making

    decision making research topics

  5. Figure A.1: The four elements of evidence-informed decision making

    decision making research topics

  6. 6 Elements of Effective Decision Making

    decision making research topics

VIDEO

  1. OM 226

  2. Choosing the Best Research Topic

  3. Khan Sir Ne Bataya Decision Kaise 🤯👀 || ft.@khangsresearchcentre1685

  4. Vasojen merkkaus perinteisellä tavalla eli korviin leikattavalla poromerkillä

  5. How To Get The Best Advice #shorts

  6. How to do research

COMMENTS

  1. Decision Making: Articles, Research, & Case Studies on Decision Making

    Lessons in Decision-Making: Confident People Aren't Always Correct (Except When They Are) A study of 70,000 decisions by Thomas Graeber and Benjamin Enke finds that self-assurance doesn't necessarily reflect skill. Shrewd decision-making often comes down to how well a person understands the limits of their knowledge.

  2. Decision Making Research Topics

    Decision Making Definition Decision making refers to the act of evaluating (i.e., forming opinions of) several alternatives and choosing the one most likely to achieve one or more goals. Common examples include deciding for whom to vote, what to eat or buy, and which college to attend. Decision making plays a key role in many… Continue reading Decision Making Research Topics

  3. Decision making

    decision making, process and logic through which individuals arrive at a decision. Different models of decision making lead to dramatically different analyses and predictions. Decision-making theories range from objective rational decision making, which assumes that individuals will make the same decisions given the same information and preferences, to the more subjective logic of ...

  4. Decision Making: a Theoretical Review

    Decision-making is a crucial skill that has a central role in everyday life and is necessary for adaptation to the environment and autonomy. It is the ability to choose between two or more options, and it has been studied through several theoretical approaches and by different disciplines. In this overview article, we contend a theoretical review regarding most theorizing and research on ...

  5. Judgment and Decision Making

    Prelude: Classical Economics. The advent of research in judgment and decision making in psychology was directly related to how these topics were studied in the field of economics (see Becker & McClintock, 1967, for a review).Economic theory proposed to identify the best possible solution to a problem given the decision maker's values and preferences (for reviews, see Baron, 2012; Fischhoff ...

  6. Decision

    Decision is a multidisciplinary research journal focused on a theoretical understanding of neural, cognitive, social, and economic aspects of human judgment and decision-making behavior.. Decision publishes articles on all areas related to judgment and decision-making research including probabilistic inference, prediction, evaluation, choice, decisions under risk or uncertainty, and economic ...

  7. Decision making

    Experiencing nature leads to healthier food choices. Across five studies of 3,726 participants, walking in nature (study 1) and viewing images of nature (studies 2-5) led to significantly more ...

  8. Decision-making competence: From decision structuring to choice

    In particular, the Research Topic will encourage the following types of contribution: (1) theoretical/opinion papers reviewing and/or critically discussing frameworks and theories of decision-making competence in relation to empirical evidence; (2) theoretical or research papers on measurement issues and instruments; (3) research papers ...

  9. (PDF) Decision-Making

    The chapter also portrays decision making in a human light: explaining how defining a rational choice and the decision process are constrained by human biology and behavior. The steps required to ...

  10. Decisionmaking

    Generating Evidence Using the Delphi Method. The Delphi method, developed at RAND in the 1950s, is a group-based process for eliciting and aggregating opinion on a topic with a goal of exploring the existence of consensus among a diverse group of experts. It's become best practice for quantifying the results of group elicitation processes.

  11. Decision making and problem solving

    Decision making and problem solving Magazine Article. Steven C. Wheelwright. Robert H. Hayes. The past several years have witnessed a growing awareness among American managers of the central ...

  12. Decision Research

    Recent decision research has highlighted the role of different types of processes involved in risky decisions. Some risky decisions are made in emotionally charged contexts while other decisions are made in a cold deliberative state and these states can affect individuals' risk-taking levels (Figner and Weber, 2011 ).

  13. Determinants of the decision-making process in organizations

    IDI was used in research on different topics related to decision-making processes. Kozioł-Nadolna & Wiśniewska presented examples of the research using IDI as an inspiration in an attempt to test the suitability of this technique for decision support systems and used IDI in supporting managerial decisions in innovative activities ...

  14. (PDF) decision-making

    Definition. Decision-making is the process whereby an individual, group or organization reaches conclusions about what. future actions to pursue given a set of objectives and limits. on available ...

  15. (PDF) Strategies for Selecting a Research Topic

    Researchers inevitably draw on all three when making key research decisions concerning content (Adhikari, 2020; Davis, 2001;Gibbons, 1999;Jensen, 2013). Science covers universally applicable ...

  16. The futility of decision making research

    Abstract. We have each spent more than 50 years doing research that has had little impact. Even more lamentable is that our field, judgment and decision making (JDM), has on the whole had little impact during that span. We attribute that failure to the use of methodologies that emphasize testing models rather than looking for differences in ...

  17. Decision making process and factors contributing to research

    Although the decision-making process of research participation in Fig 1 appears to lead from one stage to another, all three factors operated simultaneously. The flow merely serves to facilitate understanding of the decision-making process. ... Personal interest, relevant research topics and attitude to research also have a strong influence on ...

  18. RESEARCH ON RISK AND DECISION MAKING

    Research is needed not only on the identification, assessment, and quantification of various risks, but also on improved methods of analysis for decision making and on improved market, legislative, regulatory, and judicial decision-making processes. Such research should draw on experiences within the United States as well as those in other ...

  19. Sentiments and economic decision-making

    Keywords: decision-making, opinion, polarity, subjectivity, bias, sentiments . Important Note: All contributions to this Research Topic must be within the scope of the section and journal to which they are submitted, as defined in their mission statements.Frontiers reserves the right to guide an out-of-scope manuscript to a more suitable section or journal at any stage of peer review.

  20. Decision Making under Uncertainty

    Little. is known about how different forms of uncertainty, such as risk or ambiguity, are processed and learned about and how they are integrated with expected rewards and individual preferences throughout the decision making process. With this Research Topic we aim to provide a deeper and more detailed understanding of the processes behind ...

  21. Using Research for Investigative Decision-Making

    In general, research is systematic inquiry. It starts with a question and uses a process to find answers. This search for answers is expanded through the scientific method, which ensures the testability of initial questions and the generalizable and predictive quality of findings.. Behavioral and investigative research can be actively sought out by research-savvy decision makers to address a ...

  22. Decision-Making

    Trending Topics. Coronavirus Disease 2019; ... Decision-making usually involves a mixture of intuition and ... These can include doing too little or too much research, mistaking opinions for facts ...

  23. The Art of Asking Smarter Questions

    The Art of Asking Smarter Questions. These five techniques can drive great strategic decision-making. Summary. With organizations of all sorts facing increased urgency and unpredictability, being ...

  24. What is the Decision-Making Process? Definition, Steps, Examples, and

    Ethical Decision-Making Process. Ethical decision-making involves considering moral principles, values, and standards when making choices. Here's a structured approach to ethical decision-making: 1. Identify the Ethical Issue: Recognize that there is an ethical dilemma or decision to be made.

  25. A theoretical framework to guide AI ethical decision making

    This model considers the cultural, industry, organizational, and legal standards that shape AI ethical decision making. The model is based on individual decision making and parallels the decision process in autonomous AI system decision making. Topics for additional research are advanced to create expanded knowledge on this topic.

  26. Decision making for patients with severe dementia versus normal

    RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS: We retrospectively linked survey responses about end-of-life treatment decisions to Medicare claims for Health and Retirement Study respondents dying between 2002 and 2015 whose next-of-kin reported a need for surrogate decision making. We estimated multivariable logistic regression models to study measures of ...

  27. Research will aid law enforcement officer decision-making in high

    In discussing her research, Dailey said that examining an officer's use of force decision-making allows for a better understanding of the circumstances in which officers use different levels of force. Specifically, circumstances in which officers are more likely to react in a way that is not commensurate with the situation or threat level at ...

  28. AI will provide heart transplant surgeons with new decision-making data

    "The advantage of algorithms is they provide an unbiased decision-support system that can help physicians determine the 3 Rs: the Right donor at the Right time with the Right patient," he said.

  29. Software-driven Synergy between Internet of Things and High ...

    By combining the capabilities of IoT and HPC, researchers and practitioners can unlock more opportunities for data-driven knowledge creation, real-time decision-making, and advanced automation. The synergy between IoT and HPC also presents several challenges that this Research Topic aims to address.