Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts

Research articles

research in microbiology journal

Personalized bacteriophage therapy outcomes for 100 consecutive cases: a multicentre, multinational, retrospective observational study

Analysis of 100 consecutive personalized bacteriophage therapy cases supports the use of personalized and sometimes pre-adapted, bacteriophage preparations, often in combination with antibiotics.

  • Jean-Paul Pirnay
  • Sarah Djebara
  • Maya Merabishvili

research in microbiology journal

A Marine Group A isolate relies on other growing bacteria for cell wall formation

Marine Group A isolate IA91 captures peptidoglycan fragments produced by other growing bacteria and uses them to build its own cell wall, an effective and potentially common energy-saving strategy for surviving in energy-limited ecosystems.

  • Taiki Katayama
  • Masaru K. Nobu
  • Hideyuki Tamaki

research in microbiology journal

Microbial polyphenol metabolism is part of the thawing permafrost carbon cycle

Diverse microbial polyphenol transformations in thawing permafrost refute the theory that these compounds stabilize soil carbon across Arctic landscapes.

  • Bridget B. McGivern
  • Dylan R. Cronin
  • Kelly C. Wrighton

research in microbiology journal

Mycobacterium tuberculosis produces d -serine under hypoxia to limit CD8 + T cell-dependent immunity in mice

Hypoxia induces d -serine production by Mycobacterium tuberculosis which inhibits mTORC1 and T-bet expression to restrict downstream CD8 + T cell immune responses and macrophage activation, and inhibit control of tuberculosis in mice.

  • Hongyu Cheng

research in microbiology journal

Multiple sclerosis patient-derived spontaneous B cells have distinct EBV and host gene expression profiles in active disease

Characterizing EBV and host gene expression profiles of spontaneous lymphoblastoid cell lines isolated from multiple sclerosis (MS) patients with acute or stable disease, as well as healthy donors, suggests antivirals as a potential road to treat MS.

  • Samantha S. Soldan
  • Paul M. Lieberman

research in microbiology journal

Self-assembling viral histones are evolutionary intermediates between archaeal and eukaryotic nucleosomes

Phylogenetic and functional analyses suggest that viral histones were acquired from early eukaryotes during eukaryogenesis and that nucleosome evolution proceeded through histone repeat intermediates.

  • Nicholas A. T. Irwin
  • Thomas A. Richards

research in microbiology journal

Host defence peptide plectasin targets bacterial cell wall precursor lipid II by a calcium-sensitive supramolecular mechanism

Plectasin, a natural host defence peptide of fungal origin, uses a calcium-sensitive supramolecular killing mechanism to block bacterial cell wall synthesis.

  • Shehrazade Jekhmane
  • Maik G. N. Derks
  • Markus Weingarth

research in microbiology journal

Peptostreptococcus anaerobius mediates anti-PD1 therapy resistance and exacerbates colorectal cancer via myeloid-derived suppressor cells in mice

Interactions between Peptostreptococcus anaerobius and host cells promote recruitment and activation of myeloid-derived suppressor cells, leading to anti-PD1 immune checkpoint inhibitor resistance and exacerbated colorectal cancer in mice.

  • Chi Chun Wong

research in microbiology journal

Microbial dark matter could add uncertainties to metagenomic trait estimations

  • Ernest D. Osburn
  • Steven G. McBride
  • Michael S. Strickland

research in microbiology journal

Reply to: Microbial dark matter could add uncertainties to metagenomic trait estimations

  • Gabin Piton
  • Steven D. Allison
  • Adam C. Martiny

research in microbiology journal

Dynamics of measles immunity from birth and following vaccination

Serological data modelling informs on immune interference from maternal antibodies on measles vaccine responses in infants and children.

  • Megan O’Driscoll

research in microbiology journal

Propionate prevents loss of the PDIM virulence lipid in Mycobacterium tuberculosis

Propionate supplementation alleviates methylmalonyl-CoA deficiency in Mycobacterium tuberculosis to maintain PDIM virulence lipid production and select against the emergence of less virulent PDIM-negative mutants that affect experimental reliability and BCG vaccine efficacy.

  • Claire V. Mulholland
  • Thomas J. Wiggins
  • Michael Berney

research in microbiology journal

Oral bacteria relative abundance in faeces increases due to gut microbiota depletion and is linked with patient outcomes

Increased relative abundance of oral bacteria in faecal samples can be explained by the marker hypothesis—their relative increase reflects the depletion of other microbiota members—which has implications for patient outcomes and microbiota-directed therapies.

  • Thierry Rolling
  • Joao B. Xavier

research in microbiology journal

Akkermansia muciniphila exoglycosidases target extended blood group antigens to generate ABO-universal blood

Discovery, biochemical and structural characterization of exoglycosidases from Akkermansia muciniphila reveals combinations that efficiently target extended A and B blood group antigens to produce ABO-universal blood for transfusions.

  • Mathias Jensen
  • Linn Stenfelt
  • Maher Abou Hachem

research in microbiology journal

Age and sex influence antibody profiles associated with tuberculosis progression

Analysis of Mycobacterium tuberculosis -specific antibody responses in previously exposed South African cohorts reveals that profile features associated with progression to active tuberculosis are affected by age and sex.

  • Leela R. L. Davies
  • Chuangqi Wang
  • Sarah M. Fortune

research in microbiology journal

Phage defence system CBASS is regulated by a prokaryotic E2 enzyme that imitates the ubiquitin pathway

Here the authors show that anti-phage signalling in the CBASS immune system is activated by a ubiquitination-like poly-cGASylation mechanism.

research in microbiology journal

Interferon-induced transmembrane protein-1 competitively blocks Ephrin receptor A2-mediated Epstein–Barr virus entry into epithelial cells

IFITM1 can compete with EBV glycoproteins for EphA2 binding and prevent virus entry into epithelial cells, in vitro and in vivo. YTHDF3 suppresses IFITM1 via the degradation-related protein DDX5.

  • Yinggui Yang
  • Tengteng Ding

research in microbiology journal

Infection with mpox virus via the genital mucosae increases shedding and transmission in the multimammate rat ( Mastomys natalensis )

Comparing infection routes and subsequent transmission of MPXV in the multimammate rat ( Mastomys natalensis ) indicates increased susceptibility, shedding and transmission via the genital mucosae.

  • Julia R. Port
  • Jade C. Riopelle
  • Vincent J. Munster

research in microbiology journal

Cerastecins inhibit membrane lipooligosaccharide transport in drug-resistant Acinetobacter baumannii

Antibiotics to treat carbapenem-resistant Acinetobacter baumannii infection are an urgent need. The cerastecins are potent, bactericidal and efficacious in animal models of infection, and may enable new treatment modalities targeting LOS transport.

  • Andrii Ishchenko
  • Scott S. Walker

research in microbiology journal

Targeted accurate RNA consensus sequencing (tARC-seq) reveals mechanisms of replication error affecting SARS-CoV-2 divergence

Targeted accurate RNA consensus sequencing enables study of de novo errors caused by RNA-dependent RNA polymerases and provides deeper insights into how SARS-CoV-2 genetic diversity emerges.

  • Catherine C. Bradley
  • Christophe Herman

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

research in microbiology journal

BMC Microbiology

Latest collections open to submissions, the impact of gut microbiota on human health.

Guest Edited by Yongbo Kang

Synthetic and systems biology approaches applied to microbial engineering

Guest Edited by Mingfeng Cao and Sang Jun Lee

Microbial secondary metabolites for plant growth regulation and stress management

Guest Edited by Rainer Borriss, Chetan Keswani and Estibaliz Sansinenea

Phage therapy in the age of antimicrobial resistance

Guest Edited by Vijay Singh Gondil, Neelam Jain and Mianzhi Wang

Editor's choice

New Content Item

Spotlight On

New Content Item

Climate Change

Understanding the impact of climate change and exploring potential solutions to this global threat is more important than ever. Here we share recent highlights from the BMC Series journals.

  • Most accessed

Production and optimization of surfactin produced from locally isolated Bacillus halotolerans grown on agro-industrial wastes and its antimicrobial efficiency

Authors: Mohamed Abdelraof, Mohamed U. Nooman, Amr H. Hashem and Amr S. Al-kashef

Characterization of the intestinal microbiota in MSM with HIV infection

Authors: Yuansheng Fu, Susu Ke, Gan Tang, Qisheng Guo, Qian Guo, Ziwei Wang, Ruixue Leng and Yinguang Fan

Proteome profiling of Campylobacter jejuni 81–176 at 37 °C and 42 °C by label-free mass spectrometry

Authors: Annika Dreyer, Wycliffe O. Masanta, Raimond Lugert, Wolfgang Bohne, Uwe Groß, Andreas Leha, Mohammed Dakna, Christof Lenz and Andreas E. Zautner

Peptidoglycan endopeptidase MepM of uropathogenic Escherichia coli contributes to competitive fitness during urinary tract infections

Authors: Wen-Chun Huang, Ida Bagus Nyoman Putra Dwija, Masayuki Hashimoto, Jiunn-Jong Wu, Ming-Cheng Wang, Cheng-Yen Kao, Wei-Hung Lin, Shuying Wang and Ching-Hao Teng

Effect of preservation fluid contamination and associated possible donor-derived infections on early postoperative prognosis in kidney transplant recipients

Authors: Fei Zhang, Wenbo Wang, Jinbiao Zhong, Handong Ding, Guiyi Liao and Chaozhao Liang

Most recent articles RSS

View all articles

The Firmicutes/Bacteroidetes ratio of the human microbiota changes with age

Authors: D Mariat, O Firmesse, F Levenez, VD Guimarăes, H Sokol, J Doré, G Corthier and J-P Furet

Fungal-specific PCR primers developed for analysis of the ITS region of environmental DNA extracts

Authors: Kendall J Martin and Paul T Rygiewicz

ITS as an environmental DNA barcode for fungi: an in silico approach reveals potential PCR biases

Authors: Eva Bellemain, Tor Carlsen, Christian Brochmann, Eric Coissac, Pierre Taberlet and Håvard Kauserud

Protein secretion systems in bacterial-host associations, and their description in the Gene Ontology

Authors: Tsai-Tien Tseng, Brett M Tyler and João C Setubal

Standardisation of disk diffusion results for antibiotic susceptibility testing using the sirscan automated zone reader

Authors: Michael Hombach, Reinhard Zbinden and Erik C Böttger

Most accessed articles RSS

Aims and scope

BMC Microbiology is an open access, peer-reviewed journal that considers articles on all microorganisms - bacteria, archaea, algae and fungi, viruses, unicellular parasites and helminths. It considers studies on all aspects of the biology and biochemistry of microorganisms including but not limited to cell biology, genomics, signalling, the interaction of the microbes with the environment and host, mechanistic and functional insights into infection and disease, and biotechnological application in science and industry.

Become an Editorial Board Member

New Content Item

BMC Microbiology is recruiting new Editorial Board Members

New Content Item

Trending articles in BMC Microbiology

Click here to view which Articles have been shared the most in the last quarter!

Get credit for your data!

New Content Item © © ra2studio

Valuable data often go unpublished when they could be helping to progress science. Hence, the  BMC  Series introduced  Data notes,  a short article type allowing you to describe your data and publish them to make your data easier to find, cite and share.

You can publish your data in  BMC Genomic Data   (genomic, transcriptomic and high-throughput genotype data) or in  BMC Research Notes   (data from across all natural and clinical sciences). 

More information about our unique article type can be found on the BMC Genomic Data   and  BMC Research Notes  journal websites. 

Open data badge Pilot

ODB logo

BMC Microbiology  participated in a pilot project in which all accepted papers are evaluated for eligibility to receive an Open data badge. 

BMC Series Blog

World Bee Day 2024: A hive of research from the BMC Series

World Bee Day 2024: A hive of research from the BMC Series

20 May 2024

Highlights of the BMC series – April 2024

Highlights of the BMC series – April 2024

15 May 2024

Cleft Lip and Palate Awareness Week: Highlights from the BMC Series

Cleft Lip and Palate Awareness Week: Highlights from the BMC Series

14 May 2024

Latest Tweets

Your browser needs to have JavaScript enabled to view this timeline

Important information

Editorial board

For authors

For editorial board members

For reviewers

  • Manuscript editing services

Annual Journal Metrics

2022 Citation Impact 4.2 - 2-year Impact Factor 4.7 - 5-year Impact Factor 1.131 - SNIP (Source Normalized Impact per Paper) 0.937 - SJR (SCImago Journal Rank)

2023 Speed 19 days submission to first editorial decision for all manuscripts (Median) 135 days submission to accept (Median)

2023 Usage  2,970,572 downloads 1,619 Altmetric mentions 

  • More about our metrics
  • Follow us on Twitter

ISSN: 1471-2180

  • All subject areas
  • Agricultural and Biological Sciences
  • Arts and Humanities
  • Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology
  • Business, Management and Accounting
  • Chemical Engineering
  • Computer Science
  • Decision Sciences
  • Earth and Planetary Sciences
  • Economics, Econometrics and Finance
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Science
  • Health Professions
  • Immunology and Microbiology
  • Materials Science
  • Mathematics
  • Multidisciplinary
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology, Toxicology and Pharmaceutics
  • Physics and Astronomy
  • Social Sciences
  • All subject categories
  • Acoustics and Ultrasonics
  • Advanced and Specialized Nursing
  • Aerospace Engineering
  • Agricultural and Biological Sciences (miscellaneous)
  • Agronomy and Crop Science
  • Algebra and Number Theory
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine
  • Animal Science and Zoology
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Microbiology and Biotechnology
  • Applied Psychology
  • Aquatic Science
  • Archeology (arts and humanities)
  • Architecture
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous)
  • Assessment and Diagnosis
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atmospheric Science
  • Atomic and Molecular Physics, and Optics
  • Automotive Engineering
  • Behavioral Neuroscience
  • Biochemistry
  • Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology (miscellaneous)
  • Biochemistry (medical)
  • Bioengineering
  • Biological Psychiatry
  • Biomaterials
  • Biomedical Engineering
  • Biotechnology
  • Building and Construction
  • Business and International Management
  • Business, Management and Accounting (miscellaneous)
  • Cancer Research
  • Cardiology and Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Care Planning
  • Cell Biology
  • Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience
  • Ceramics and Composites
  • Chemical Engineering (miscellaneous)
  • Chemical Health and Safety
  • Chemistry (miscellaneous)
  • Chiropractics
  • Civil and Structural Engineering
  • Clinical Biochemistry
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Colloid and Surface Chemistry
  • Communication
  • Community and Home Care
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Complementary and Manual Therapy
  • Computational Mathematics
  • Computational Mechanics
  • Computational Theory and Mathematics
  • Computer Graphics and Computer-Aided Design
  • Computer Networks and Communications
  • Computer Science Applications
  • Computer Science (miscellaneous)
  • Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition
  • Computers in Earth Sciences
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Conservation
  • Control and Optimization
  • Control and Systems Engineering
  • Critical Care and Intensive Care Medicine
  • Critical Care Nursing
  • Cultural Studies
  • Decision Sciences (miscellaneous)
  • Dental Assisting
  • Dental Hygiene
  • Dentistry (miscellaneous)
  • Dermatology
  • Development
  • Developmental and Educational Psychology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Developmental Neuroscience
  • Discrete Mathematics and Combinatorics
  • Drug Discovery
  • Drug Guides
  • Earth and Planetary Sciences (miscellaneous)
  • Earth-Surface Processes
  • Ecological Modeling
  • Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics
  • Economic Geology
  • Economics and Econometrics
  • Economics, Econometrics and Finance (miscellaneous)
  • Electrical and Electronic Engineering
  • Electrochemistry
  • Electronic, Optical and Magnetic Materials
  • Emergency Medical Services
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Emergency Nursing
  • Endocrine and Autonomic Systems
  • Endocrinology
  • Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism
  • Energy Engineering and Power Technology
  • Energy (miscellaneous)
  • Engineering (miscellaneous)
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Environmental Engineering
  • Environmental Science (miscellaneous)
  • Epidemiology
  • Experimental and Cognitive Psychology
  • Family Practice
  • Filtration and Separation
  • Fluid Flow and Transfer Processes
  • Food Animals
  • Food Science
  • Fuel Technology
  • Fundamentals and Skills
  • Gastroenterology
  • Gender Studies
  • Genetics (clinical)
  • Geochemistry and Petrology
  • Geography, Planning and Development
  • Geometry and Topology
  • Geotechnical Engineering and Engineering Geology
  • Geriatrics and Gerontology
  • Gerontology
  • Global and Planetary Change
  • Hardware and Architecture
  • Health Informatics
  • Health Information Management
  • Health Policy
  • Health Professions (miscellaneous)
  • Health (social science)
  • Health, Toxicology and Mutagenesis
  • History and Philosophy of Science
  • Horticulture
  • Human Factors and Ergonomics
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Immunology and Allergy
  • Immunology and Microbiology (miscellaneous)
  • Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering
  • Industrial Relations
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Information Systems
  • Information Systems and Management
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Insect Science
  • Instrumentation
  • Internal Medicine
  • Issues, Ethics and Legal Aspects
  • Leadership and Management
  • Library and Information Sciences
  • Life-span and Life-course Studies
  • Linguistics and Language
  • Literature and Literary Theory
  • LPN and LVN
  • Management Information Systems
  • Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law
  • Management of Technology and Innovation
  • Management Science and Operations Research
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Materials Science (miscellaneous)
  • Maternity and Midwifery
  • Mathematical Physics
  • Mathematics (miscellaneous)
  • Mechanical Engineering
  • Mechanics of Materials
  • Media Technology
  • Medical and Surgical Nursing
  • Medical Assisting and Transcription
  • Medical Laboratory Technology
  • Medical Terminology
  • Medicine (miscellaneous)
  • Metals and Alloys
  • Microbiology
  • Microbiology (medical)
  • Modeling and Simulation
  • Molecular Biology
  • Molecular Medicine
  • Nanoscience and Nanotechnology
  • Nature and Landscape Conservation
  • Neurology (clinical)
  • Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology
  • Neuroscience (miscellaneous)
  • Nuclear and High Energy Physics
  • Nuclear Energy and Engineering
  • Numerical Analysis
  • Nurse Assisting
  • Nursing (miscellaneous)
  • Nutrition and Dietetics
  • Obstetrics and Gynecology
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Ocean Engineering
  • Oceanography
  • Oncology (nursing)
  • Ophthalmology
  • Oral Surgery
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management
  • Orthodontics
  • Orthopedics and Sports Medicine
  • Otorhinolaryngology
  • Paleontology
  • Parasitology
  • Pathology and Forensic Medicine
  • Pathophysiology
  • Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health
  • Periodontics
  • Pharmaceutical Science
  • Pharmacology
  • Pharmacology (medical)
  • Pharmacology (nursing)
  • Pharmacology, Toxicology and Pharmaceutics (miscellaneous)
  • Physical and Theoretical Chemistry
  • Physical Therapy, Sports Therapy and Rehabilitation
  • Physics and Astronomy (miscellaneous)
  • Physiology (medical)
  • Plant Science
  • Political Science and International Relations
  • Polymers and Plastics
  • Process Chemistry and Technology
  • Psychiatry and Mental Health
  • Psychology (miscellaneous)
  • Public Administration
  • Public Health, Environmental and Occupational Health
  • Pulmonary and Respiratory Medicine
  • Radiological and Ultrasound Technology
  • Radiology, Nuclear Medicine and Imaging
  • Rehabilitation
  • Religious Studies
  • Renewable Energy, Sustainability and the Environment
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Research and Theory
  • Respiratory Care
  • Review and Exam Preparation
  • Reviews and References (medical)
  • Rheumatology
  • Safety Research
  • Safety, Risk, Reliability and Quality
  • Sensory Systems
  • Signal Processing
  • Small Animals
  • Social Psychology
  • Social Sciences (miscellaneous)
  • Social Work
  • Sociology and Political Science
  • Soil Science
  • Space and Planetary Science
  • Spectroscopy
  • Speech and Hearing
  • Sports Science
  • Statistical and Nonlinear Physics
  • Statistics and Probability
  • Statistics, Probability and Uncertainty
  • Strategy and Management
  • Stratigraphy
  • Structural Biology
  • Surfaces and Interfaces
  • Surfaces, Coatings and Films
  • Theoretical Computer Science
  • Tourism, Leisure and Hospitality Management
  • Transplantation
  • Transportation
  • Urban Studies
  • Veterinary (miscellaneous)
  • Visual Arts and Performing Arts
  • Waste Management and Disposal
  • Water Science and Technology
  • All regions / countries
  • Asiatic Region
  • Eastern Europe
  • Latin America
  • Middle East
  • Northern America
  • Pacific Region
  • Western Europe
  • ARAB COUNTRIES
  • IBEROAMERICA
  • NORDIC COUNTRIES
  • Afghanistan
  • Bosnia and Herzegovina
  • Brunei Darussalam
  • Czech Republic
  • Dominican Republic
  • Netherlands
  • New Caledonia
  • New Zealand
  • Papua New Guinea
  • Philippines
  • Puerto Rico
  • Russian Federation
  • Saudi Arabia
  • South Africa
  • South Korea
  • Switzerland
  • Syrian Arab Republic
  • Trinidad and Tobago
  • United Arab Emirates
  • United Kingdom
  • United States
  • Vatican City State
  • Book Series
  • Conferences and Proceedings
  • Trade Journals

research in microbiology journal

  • Citable Docs. (3years)
  • Total Cites (3years)

research in microbiology journal

Follow us on @ScimagoJR Scimago Lab , Copyright 2007-2024. Data Source: Scopus®

research in microbiology journal

Cookie settings

Cookie Policy

Legal Notice

Privacy Policy

  • Search Menu
  • Sign in through your institution
  • FEMS Microbiology Ecology
  • FEMS Microbiology Letters
  • FEMS Microbiology Reviews
  • FEMS Yeast Research
  • Pathogens and Disease
  • FEMS Microbes
  • Awards & Prizes
  • Editor's Choice Articles
  • Thematic Issues
  • Virtual Special Issues
  • Call for Papers
  • Journal Policies
  • Open Access Options
  • Submit to the FEMS Journals
  • Why Publish with the FEMS Journals
  • About the Federation of European Microbiological Societies
  • About the FEMS Journals
  • Advertising and Corporate Services
  • Conference Reports
  • Editorial Boards
  • Investing in Science
  • Journals Career Network
  • Journals on Oxford Academic
  • Books on Oxford Academic

Six Key Topics in Microbiology 2024_960x160.jpg

Six Key Topics in Microbiology: 2024

This collection from the FEMS journals presents the latest high-quality research in six key topic areas of microbiology that have an impact across the world. All of the FEMS journals aim to serve the microbiology community with timely and authoritative research and reviews, and by investing back into the science community . 

Interested in publishing your research relevant to the six key microbiology topics?

Learn more about why the FEMS journals are the perfect home for your microbiology research.  

Browse the collection categories: 

Antimicrobial resistance, environmental microbiology, pathogenicity and virulence, biotechnology and synthetic biology, microbiomes, food microbiology.

research in microbiology journal

FEMS and Open Access: Embracing an Open Future

As of January 2024, FEMS has flipped four of its journals to fully open access (OA), making six out of its seven journals OA. FEMS Microbiology Letters remains a subscription journal and free to publish in. 

We are excited to be making high quality science freely available to anyone to read anywhere in the world and further supporting the advancement of our discipline. 

View our FAQs page

research in microbiology journal

Never miss the latest research from the FEMS Journals

Stay up to date on the latest microbiology research with content alerts delivered directly to your inbox. This free service  from OUP allows you to create custom email alerts to make sure you never miss our on the latest research from your favorite FEMS journals. 

Learn more & sign up

Affiliations

  • Copyright © 2024
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Institutional account management
  • Rights and permissions
  • Get help with access
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

New Content Item

Systems Microbiology and Biomanufacturing

Systems Microbiology and Biomanufacturing is a platform for high-quality research in the interdisciplinary areas of industrial microbiology and biomanufacturing process development.

  • Topics include Food Microbiology and Biotechnology, Industrial Microbiology and Biotechnology, Metabolic Engineering and Synthetic Biology, Fermentation Engineering, and so on.
  • Served by a dedicated international editorial board for swift editorial response.
  • Offers free online publication for traditional subscription-based submissions, and OA options are available.
  • Aims to bridge the gap between microbiology advances and biomanufactioring processes.
  • Ashok Pandey

Societies and partnerships

New Content Item

Latest issue

Volume 4, Issue 2

Latest articles

Extremophiles and their enzymatic diversity and biotechnological potential.

  • Fatima Atif
  • Nida Maqsood
  • Muhammad Irfan

research in microbiology journal

Recycling spent animal cell culture media using the thermally resistant microalga Chlorella sorokiniana

  • Richard Thyden
  • Tanja Dominko
  • Glenn Gaudette

research in microbiology journal

An in vitro evaluation of partial energy replacement in a total mixed ration with volatile fatty acids derived from agro-industrial residues

  • Milad Parchami
  • Bengt-Ove Rustas
  • Amir Mahboubi

research in microbiology journal

Rearrangement in the regulation of sigD gene expression promotes 4-hydroxyisoleucine production in Corynebacterium glutamicum

  • Youhe Xiang

research in microbiology journal

Metabolic engineering of Bacillus subtilis for de novo synthesis of 6′-sialyllactose

research in microbiology journal

Journal updates

Systems microbiology and biomanufacturing (smab) best reviewers (2023), systems microbiology and biomanufacturing (smab) top cited articles (2023), good news: systems microbiology and biotechnology – now indexed by esci, web of science.

We are pleased to announce that Systems Microbiology and Biotechnology (SMAB) has been accepted for indexing by Emerging Sources Citation Index, Web of Science!

Enjoy reading our published articles and welcome to submit your next article to our journal!

Call for Reading: Special Issue on Synthetic Biocatalysts for Biomanufacturing

We are pleased to announce that the special issue on Synthetic Biocatalysts for Biomanufacturing  was successfully published with 11 articles in Issue 3, Volume 3.

Please enjoy reading our selected articles on this topic!

Journal information

  • Biological Abstracts
  • CAB Abstracts
  • Chemical Abstracts Service (CAS)
  • EI Compendex
  • Emerging Sources Citation Index
  • Google Scholar
  • OCLC WorldCat Discovery Service
  • TD Net Discovery Service

Rights and permissions

Editorial policies

© Jiangnan University

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Short Reports

Short Reports offer exciting novel biological findings that may trigger new research directions.

See Journal Information »

The human auditory system uses amplitude modulation to distinguish music from speech

Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

* E-mail: [email protected]

Affiliation Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America

ORCID logo

Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Affiliation Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Affiliation Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Juriquilla, Querétaro, México

Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Affiliations Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America, Ernst Struengmann Institute for Neuroscience, Frankfurt am Main, Germany, Center for Language, Music, and Emotion (CLaME), New York University, New York, New York, United States of America, Music and Audio Research Lab (MARL), New York University, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Andrew Chang, 
  • Xiangbin Teng, 
  • M. Florencia Assaneo, 
  • David Poeppel

PLOS

  • Published: May 28, 2024
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631
  • Reader Comments

Fig 1

Music and speech are complex and distinct auditory signals that are both foundational to the human experience. The mechanisms underpinning each domain are widely investigated. However, what perceptual mechanism transforms a sound into music or speech and how basic acoustic information is required to distinguish between them remain open questions. Here, we hypothesized that a sound’s amplitude modulation (AM), an essential temporal acoustic feature driving the auditory system across processing levels, is critical for distinguishing music and speech. Specifically, in contrast to paradigms using naturalistic acoustic signals (that can be challenging to interpret), we used a noise-probing approach to untangle the auditory mechanism: If AM rate and regularity are critical for perceptually distinguishing music and speech, judging artificially noise-synthesized ambiguous audio signals should align with their AM parameters. Across 4 experiments ( N = 335), signals with a higher peak AM frequency tend to be judged as speech, lower as music. Interestingly, this principle is consistently used by all listeners for speech judgments, but only by musically sophisticated listeners for music. In addition, signals with more regular AM are judged as music over speech, and this feature is more critical for music judgment, regardless of musical sophistication. The data suggest that the auditory system can rely on a low-level acoustic property as basic as AM to distinguish music from speech, a simple principle that provokes both neurophysiological and evolutionary experiments and speculations.

Citation: Chang A, Teng X, Assaneo MF, Poeppel D (2024) The human auditory system uses amplitude modulation to distinguish music from speech. PLoS Biol 22(5): e3002631. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631

Academic Editor: Manuel S. Malmierca, Universidad de Salamanca, SPAIN

Received: October 15, 2023; Accepted: April 17, 2024; Published: May 28, 2024

Copyright: © 2024 Chang et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: All stimuli, experimental programs, raw data, and analysis codes have been deposited at a publicly available OSF repository ( https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/RDTGC ).

Funding: A.C. was supported by a Ruth L. Kirschstein Postdoctoral Individual National Research Service Award, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders/National Institutes of Health (F32DC018205) and Leon Levy Scholarships in Neuroscience, Leon Levy Foundation/New York Academy of Sciences. X.T. was supported by Improvement on Competitiveness in Hiring New Faculties Funding Scheme, the Chinese University of Hong Kong (4937113). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Music and speech, two complex auditory signals, are frequently compared across many levels of biological sciences, ranging from system and cognitive neuroscience to comparative and evolutionary biology. As acoustic signals, they exhibit a range of interesting similarities (e.g., temporal structure [ 1 , 2 ]) and differences (e.g., music, but not speech, features discrete pitch intervals). In the brain, they are processed by both shared [ 3 – 6 ] and specialized [ 7 – 10 ] neural substrates. However, which acoustic information underpins a sound to be perceived as music or speech remains an open question.

One way to address the broader question of how music and speech are organized in the human mind/brain is to capitalize on ecologically valid, “real” signals, a more holistic approach. That strategy has the advantage of working with stimulus materials that are naturalistic and, therefore, engage the perceptual and neural systems in a typical manner (e.g., [ 11 , 12 ]). The disadvantage of adopting such an experimental attack is that it can be quite challenging to identify and isolate the components and processes that underpin perception. Here, we pursue the alternative reductionist approach: parametrically generating and manipulating ambiguous auditory stimuli with basic, analytically tractable amplitude modulation (AM) features. If the auditory system distinguishes music and speech according to the low-level acoustic parameters, the music/speech judgment on artificially noise-synthesized ambiguous audio signals should align with their AM parameters, even if no real music or speech is contained in the signal.

In the neural domain, AM is a basic acoustic feature that drives auditory neuronal circuits and underlying complex communicative functions across both humans and nonhuman animals. At the micro- and meso-levels, single-cell and population recording of auditory cortex neurons in nonhuman animals demonstrated various mechanisms to encode AM features (e.g., [ 13 , 14 ]). At the macro-level, human neuroimaging studies showed that the acoustic AM synchronizes the neural activities at auditory cortex and correlated with perception and speech comprehensions (e.g., [ 15 – 18 ]). A critical but underexplored gap is the mechanism of how low-level AM features affect a sound to be processed as a complex high-level signal such as music and speech.

Our experiments tested the hypothesis that a remarkably basic acoustic parameter can, in part , determine a sound to be perceptually judged as music or speech. The conjecture is that AM ( Fig 1 ) is one crucial acoustic factor to distinguish music and speech. Previous studies that quantified many hours and a wide variety of music and speech recordings showed distinct peak AM rates in the modulation spectrum: music peaks at 1 to 2 Hz and speech peaks at 3.5 to 5.5 Hz [ 19 – 21 ]. Consistent with those findings, these rate differences are also observed in spontaneous speech and music production [ 22 ]. Next, temporal regularity of AM could also be important, as music is often metrically organized with an underlying beat, whereas speech is not periodic and is better considered quasirhythmic [ 20 , 23 ]. Also, supporting the relevant role played by AM, neuroimaging evidence showed that temporally scrambled but spectrally intact signals weaken neural activity in speech- or music-related cortical clusters [ 9 , 24 ]. Finally, a preliminary study ( n = 12) showed that listeners were able to near-perfectly categorize 1-channel noise-vocoded realistic speech and music excerpts [ 19 ]. However, the noise-vocoding approach was insufficient to mechanistically pinpoint the degree to which AM rate and regularity contribute to music/speech distinction, as this manipulation preserved all the envelope temporal features above and beyond rate and regularity. For example, onset sharpness of speech envelope is encoded by the spoken language cortical network (superior temporal gyrus) and critical to comprehension [ 25 – 27 ]; also, the onset sharpness of the music envelope is crucial for timbre perception, e.g., a piano tone typically has a sharper onset than violin. We therefore build on the notion that the AM distinction between music and speech signals appears to be acoustically robust. However, in order to advance our understanding of potential mechanisms, we ask what aspects of the AM influence listeners to make this perceptual distinction. How acoustically reduced and simple can a signal be and still be judged to be speech or music?

thumbnail

  • PPT PowerPoint slide
  • PNG larger image
  • TIFF original image

research in microbiology journal

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631.g001

Based on the literature, we hypothesized that stimuli with a lower-in-modulation-frequency and narrower-in-variance peak (i.e., higher temporal regularity, more isochrony) in the AM spectrum would be judged as music, while those with higher and broader peaks (i.e., lower temporal regularity) as speech. If these hypotheses are plausible, artificial sounds synthesized with the designated AM properties should be perceptually categorized accordingly. This noise-probing approach is conceptually similar to the reverse-correlation approach in studies seeking to understand what features are driving the “black-box” perceptual system (e.g., [ 28 , 29 ]). In short, we synthesized stimuli with specific AM parameters by “reversing” a pipeline for analyzing realistic, naturalistic music and speech recordings ( Fig 1B ). First, we used a lognormal function that resembles the empirically determined AM spectra reported in previous studies [ 19 , 20 ]; this function permits the independent manipulation of peak frequency and temporal regularity parameters. Next, after transforming each AM spectrum into a time-domain AM signal (inverse Fourier transform), that signal was used to modulate a flat white noise (i.e., low-noise noise) carrier to generate a 4-s duration experimental stimulus. This approach, importantly, eliminates typical spectral features of both music and speech. In our 4 online experiments, participants were told that each stimulus came from a real music or speech recording but was synthesized with noise, and their task was to judge whether it was music or speech. Although none of the stimuli sounded like real music or speech, participants’ judgments revealed how well each stimulus matched their internal representation of one or the other perceptual category.

In Experiment 1, we manipulated peak AM frequency while σ (the regularity parameter, or the width of the peak of the AM spectrum; see Methods ) was fixed at 0.35 (the value was chosen as it sounded the most “natural” or “comfortable” according to the informal feedback from colleagues in the lab). Stimuli were presented one at a time, and participants were requested to judge whether a stimulus is music or speech. Data from 129 participants were included in the analyses. The overall responses are presented in Fig 2A . To investigate the effect of peak frequencies, each participant’s responses (speech = 1, music = 0) were linearly regressed on the peak frequencies (mean ± standard error of R 2 = 0.53 ± 0.03; Fig 2B ). The response slopes were significantly above 0 ( Fig 2C ; t (128) = 7.70, p < 10 −11 , Cohen’s d = 0.68), suggesting that people judge sounds with a higher peak AM frequency as speech and sounds with a lower peak AM frequency as music. We then explored the association of this judgment with participants’ musical sophistication and found that the participants with a higher General Musical Sophistication score (Gold-MSI [ 30 ]; see Methods ) were more likely to have a higher response slope ( r (127) = 0.17, p = 0.056; but after removing 1 outlier: r (126) = 0.20, p = 0.023; Fig 2D ). We further split the participants by slope at 0 and performed an unequal-variance 2-sample t test without removing that 1 outlier. This analysis confirmed that the participants with a positive response slope have higher General Musical Sophistication scores than the participants with a negative slope ( t (57.25) = 2.96, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.57). We further correlated the response slope with each subscale of the musical sophistication index, but none of them were significant (unsigned r (127) < 0.16, p > 0.075). While null effects should be interpreted with caution, this suggests that general musical sophistication, rather than a specific musical aspect, is driving the outcome. In short, the findings show that the sounds with a higher peak AM frequency are more likely to be judged as speech and lower as music, and this tendency is positively associated with participants’ general musical sophistication.

thumbnail

( A ) The music vs. speech judgment response of each participant at different levels of AM peak frequencies. ( B ) Fitted regression lines of each participant’s response. ( C ) Each dot represents the response slope on peak frequencies of a participant, and the bar and the error bars represent the mean ± standard error. The participants’ response slopes were significantly above 0, suggesting that the participants tend to judge the stimuli with a higher peak AM frequency as speech and lower as music. ( D ) The response slopes and the General Musical Sophistication score of the participants were positively correlated, suggesting that the musically more sophisticated participants are more likely to judge the stimuli with a higher peak AM frequency as speech and a lower peak frequency as music. Note that the gray circle marks the outlier, and the regression line and the p -value reported on the figure were based on the analysis without the outlier. Underlying data and scripts are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/RDTGC and in S1 Data .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631.g002

Note that we attempted to fit the data with a logistic psychometric function. Although the findings were consistent as the fitted slopes of the logistic model were also significantly above 0 ( t (128) = 6.85, p < 10 −9 ), suggesting the sounds with a higher peak AM frequency are more likely to be judged as speech over music, the R 2 of the logistic model were much lower than the linear model (mean R 2 difference: 0.19), so did the following experiments (see Methods for more details), suggesting that the linear model was a more appropriate model. Therefore, only the linear models were interpreted.

To investigate the effect of temporal regularity, in Experiment 2, we manipulated AM temporal regularity (σ) at 3 peak AM frequencies (1, 2.5, and 4 Hz, which roughly correspond to the AM range of music, a midpoint, and speech). The procedure was identical to Experiment 1, and data from 48 participants were included. The overall responses are presented in Fig 3A . Each participant’s responses were linearly regressed on the σ under each peak frequency ( R 2 = 0.37 ± 0.02; Fig 3B ). The response slopes were significantly above 0 for the peak frequency at 1 Hz ( t (47) = 6.19, p < 10 −6 , Cohen’s d = 0.89) and 2.5 Hz ( t (47) = 6.37, p < 10 −7 , Cohen’s d = 0.92), suggesting that listeners tend to judge sounds with lower temporal regularity (higher σ) as speech and higher regularity as music ( Fig 3C ). Note that this pattern was the opposite for the peak frequency at 4 Hz, with a lower effect size ( t (47) = −3.34, p = 0.016, Cohen’s d = 0.48). It suggests that the association between temporal regularity and the music judgment is conditional on the low-to-mid peak AM frequency range, and the influence of temporal regularity is weaker when peak AM frequency is in the AM range of speech. We also examined the associations between participants’ musical sophistication levels and response slope, but no correlation was significant ( Fig 3D ; unsigned r (46) < 0.13, p = 0.404).

thumbnail

( A ) The music vs. speech judgment response of each participant at different levels of temporal regularity (σ). ( B ) Fitted regression lines of each participant’s response. ( C ) The participants’ response slopes on σ were significantly above 0 for the peak AM frequencies at 1 and 2.5 Hz but not 4 Hz. This suggests that participants tend to judge the temporally more regular stimuli as music and irregular as speech, but this tendency was not observed when the peak frequency was as high as 4 Hz. ( D ) The response slopes and the General Musical Sophistication scores were not correlated at any peak AM frequencies. Underlying data and scripts are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/RDTGC and in S1 Data . n . s ., nonsignificant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631.g003

The dichotomy of the behavioral judgment that our task imposes could be a concern because it only allows a stimulus to be judged as music or speech, while ignoring other possible categories. It is, to be sure, reasonable to directly contrast music and speech, as these are arguably among the most dominant high-level auditory forms in human cognition, sharing many commonalities (cf., [ 1 , 9 ]), and a discrimination task between two categories is usually considered psychophysically more powerful than two separate detection tasks on each category [ 31 ]. However, other auditory categories, such as animal calls and environmental sounds, are critical in human perception as well. Therefore, we tested the robustness of the findings of Experiments 1 and 2 by replicating them with detection tasks, and we investigated whether there were effects specific to music or speech.

In Experiment 3, peak AM frequency was manipulated with σ fixed at 0.35; 80 participants were included in the analyses. In the “music detection” task, participants were instructed that 50% of the stimuli were music and 50% were not music (“others”), and they were asked to judge whether it was music or something else. For the “speech detection” task, the task was analogous. The 50% instruction was added to prevent participants with a strong response bias. Each participant performed both tasks with the same stimuli. The overall responses are presented in Fig 4A . Each participant’s responses (music or speech = 1, others = 0) were linearly regressed on peak frequency for each task ( R 2 = 0.68 ± 0.02; Fig 4B ). For the speech task, the response slopes were significantly above 0 ( t (79) = 12.79, p < 10 −20 , Cohen’s d = 1.43; Fig 4C ), suggesting that the sounds with a higher peak AM frequencies are more likely to be judged as speech over others. Musical sophistication did not correlate with the speech response slope ( r (78) = 0.04, p = 0.717; Fig 4D ). For the music task, the response slope was not significantly different from 0 ( t (79) = 0.49, p = 0.628, Cohen’s d = 0.05; Fig 4C ). Interestingly, there was a significant correlation suggesting that the more musically sophisticated participants are more likely to judge the sound with a lower peak AM frequency as music ( r (78) = −0.28, p = 0.011; Fig 4D ), and this is again confirmed by the unequal-variance 2-sample t test between split-data at slope equals to 0 ( t (72.57) = 2.66, p = 0.010, Cohen’s d = 0.58). We also correlated the response slope with each subscale; however, once again, none of them passed the Bonferroni-corrected statistical threshold at 0.01 (unsigned r (78) < 0.28, p > 0.013). Together, the effect of peak AM frequency reported in Experiment 1 is robustly replicated for the speech judgment, but the music judgment was conditional on participants’ general musical sophistication.

thumbnail

Results of Experiments 3 (A-D) and 4 (E-H). ( A ) The “music vs. others” and “speech vs. others” judgment response of each participant at different levels of peak AM frequencies. ( B ) The fitted regression line of each participant’s response. ( C ) The participants’ response slopes on peak frequencies were significantly above 0 for the speech task but not for the music task, suggesting that the participants tend to judge the stimuli with a higher peak AM frequency as speech. ( D ) The response slopes and the General Musical Sophistication scores of the participants were positively correlated for the music task but not for the speech task, suggesting that the musically more sophisticated participants are more likely to judge the stimuli with a lower peak AM frequency as music. ( E - H ) The same format as above, but at different levels of temporal regularity (σ). The participants tend to judge the stimuli with a higher temporal regularity as music. Underlying data and scripts are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/RDTGC and in S1 Data . n . s ., nonsignificant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631.g004

In Experiment 4, AM temporal regularity was manipulated while the peak AM frequency was fixed at 2 Hz (equally likely to be judged as music or speech, according to the previous experiments). The tasks were as in Experiment 3, and data from 78 participants were included. The overall responses are shown in Fig 4E . Each participant’s responses were linearly regressed on σ for each task ( R 2 = 0.32 ± 0.02; Fig 4F ). For the music task, the response slope was significantly below 0 ( t (77) = -4.95, p < 10 −5 , Cohen’s d = 0.56; Fig 4G ), suggesting that people tend to judge the sounds with higher temporal regularity (lower σ) as music. For the speech task, the response slopes were slightly above 0 but not reaching the statistical threshold ( t (77) = 1.89, p = 0.063, Cohen’s d = 0.21; Fig 4G ). We did not observe any associations between participants’ musical sophistication and response slope (unsigned r (76) < 0.13, p > 0.293; Fig 4H ). Together, the effect of AM temporal regularity reported in Experiment 2 was robustly replicated for music but only a trend was observed for speech.

These surprising findings and their replications show that listeners use acoustic amplitude modulations in sounds, one of the most basic features, fundamental to human auditory perception, to judge whether a sound is “like” music or speech, even when spectral features are eliminated. We show that peak AM frequency can affect high-level categorization: Sounds with a higher peak AM frequency tend to be judged as speech, those with a lower peak as music, especially among musically sophisticated participants. This pattern is consistent with previous quantifications of natural music recordings showing that the peak AM frequency of music is lower than speech [ 19 ]. This result might arise because of participants’ (implicit) knowledge of this acoustic feature. We note that, while the effect of peak AM frequency in Experiment 1 was robustly replicated in the speech task in Experiment 3, in the music task, the effect was more salient among musically sophisticated participants but not visible when pooling all participants. In other words, peak AM frequency is a universal cue for speech but not for music. A possible explanation is that this effect depends on listeners’ experience or sophistication with music or speech sounds. While our participants exhibited a ceiling effect for speech (as university students, every listener can be classified as an “expert” in speech), their musical sophistication scores appeared lower than the norm (Experiment 3 versus Müllensiefen and colleagues [ 30 ]: 71.39 versus 81.58, Cohen’s d = 0.49; but it is similar to other studies (e.g., [ 32 , 33 ])). The potential effect of speech expertise would need to be examined, for example, in future developmental studies in which expertise can be more carefully controlled.

Temporal regularity (and, in the extreme, isochrony, if σ = 0) of AM also has an effect: Sounds with more regular modulation are more likely to be judged as music than speech. This is consistent with the fact that Western music is usually metrically organized while speech is quasirhythmic [ 20 , 23 ]. There are a few aspects worth discussing. First, this effect is more relevant to music than to speech. The detection tasks in Experiment 4 show that the effect of temporal regularity is only robustly observed for music but not for speech. It appears that temporal regularity is a more prevalent principle than peak AM frequency to judge a sound as music as this effect does not depend on the listener’s musical sophistication. Second, in Experiment 2, the effect of temporal regularity was slightly opposite when the peak AM frequency was at 4 Hz. A possible explanation is that temporal regularity might be less critical for distinguishing music and speech when peak AM frequency is already in the canonical speech range 3.5 to 5.5 Hz [ 19 – 21 ]. Last but not least, while temporal regularity in the current parameter range did not drastically influence the auditory judgments, the current data demonstrate a clear pattern across participants: A sound with a more temporally regular AM is more like music.

AM is one of the most fundamental building blocks for auditory perception, and especially so for human speech. While frequency/spectral information is critical for auditory object identification, pitch perception, and timbre, AM is considered a key information-bearing component and critical for speech intelligibility [ 34 , 35 ]. AM, especially around the 2-4 Hz, is faithfully encoded by neurons in the primary auditory cortex [ 14 , 36 ]. While previous studies have demonstrated that temporal envelope information alone is arguably sufficient for speech perception (e.g., [ 37 ]), the current findings further show that AM rate can be used to identify a sound as speech or not (i.e., Fig 4C ). Relatedly, AM rate helps identify music, at least among musically sophisticated listeners. This could be for different reasons. First, music has salient features in both time and frequency domains. A recent survey showed that adults explicitly consider both AM regularity (rhythm/beat) and melody (frequency/spectral domain), but not AM rate, as being the primary acoustic features for distinguishing speech and song [ 38 ]. This is consistent with the current finding that people rely on AM regularity more than rate to identify music. Second, the association between AM rate and music perception might require musical experience. This is consistent with the neural entrainment studies showing that the fidelity of auditory cortex entraining to music rhythm is positively associated with the musical expertise of the listeners [ 25 , 39 ]. Together, our data provide the empirical advance that AM rate or regularity alone, regardless of the fine temporal features (e.g., onset sharpness) preserved by the noise-vocoded approach [ 19 ], have an effect on the music/speech judgment. Given that the AM rate and regularity are processed early in the auditory cortex [ 14 ], notably prior to superior temporal gyrus encoding of speech onset (e.g., [ 26 , 27 ]), AM rate or regularity should have more decisive roles than temporal envelope features for distinguishing music and speech at an early stage of the auditory cortical pathway.

The current study has four noteworthy limitations. First, the lognormal function can resemble the average AM spectrum of many hours of music or speech recordings [ 19 ], but it does not necessarily approximate individual recordings well. Second, the current forced-choice task design can only demonstrate how acoustic features affect the auditory judgments , but whether participants subjectively experienced the percepts of our stimuli as “reduced” forms of music or speech is unclear, as the rich spectral and timbral features of typical music or speech were by design eliminated from the stimuli. Third, while the current experimental design only showed the influences of AM rate and regularity on distinguishing music and speech, we did not compare their influences to those of other acoustic features. Although spectral or frequency modulation, orthogonal to AM, is another promising acoustic feature fundamental to auditory perception, the current study focuses on only the AM aspect as it has been demonstrated distinct between music and speech acoustics while the spectral aspect has not. Lastly, the factors that contributed to the substantial individual differences in music-related tasks remain unclear, and musical sophistication only partially accounts for it. Other perceptual and cognitive factors (e.g., preference for fast or slow music, unawareness of hearing loss among young adults) and experimental factors (e.g., whether the participants were exposed to any specific music or speech in the environment while performing our experiment online, remotely, and on their own) likely contributed to the individual differences as well. Nevertheless, our reductionist approach demonstrates the striking fact that music or speech judgment starts from basic acoustic features such as AM.

A related phenomenon that builds on the role of temporal structure can be illuminated by these data. The speech-to-song illusion demonstrates that, by looping a (real) speech excerpt, the perceptual judgment can gradually shift from speech toward song [ 40 – 42 ]. The effects reported here are consistent with the speech-to-song illusion: The low frequency power of the AM spectrum would emerge from the repeating-segment periodicity and, therefore, bias the judgment toward music. Supporting this view, this illusion disappears if speech is temporally jumbled in every repetition [ 40 ], which eliminates low-frequency periodicity across repetitions. Furthermore, consistent with our findings, the strength of the illusion is also positively associated with beat regularity and participants’ musical expertise [ 41 , 43 – 45 ].

The properties of AM that support the distinction of music and speech merit consideration in the context of human evolution and neurophysiology. Group cohesion and interpersonal interaction have been hypothesized as one primary function of music [ 46 – 53 ]. If music serves as an auditory cue for coordinating group behaviors, predictable temporal regularity at the optimal rate for human movements and audiomotor synchronization (1 to 2 Hz; [ 54 – 56 ]) would be important. And, in fact, motor brain networks are involved while processing auditory rhythms (e.g., [ 57 – 64 ]). The AM rate of speech, analogously, has been attributed to the neurophysiological properties of the specialized auditory-motor oscillatory network for speech perception and production, as well as the associated biomechanics of the articulatory movements [ 17 , 20 , 65 , 66 ]. Consistent with these data patterns, perceptual studies have also shown a general pattern that music versus speech task performance is optimal with rates ranging around 0.5 to 6.7 and 2 to 9 Hz, respectively [ 67 , 68 ].

The experimental results we present demonstrate that human listeners can use a basic acoustic feature fundamental to auditory perception to judge whether a sound is like music or speech. These data reveal a potential processing principle that invites both neurophysiological and evolutionary experiments and speculations that could further address the long-lasting questions on the comparison between music and speech in both the humanities and the sciences.

Resource availability

All stimuli, experimental programs, raw data, and analysis codes have been deposited at a publicly available OSF repository ( https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/RDTGC ).

Participants

The participants were students at New York University who signed up for the studies via the SONA online platform and received course credit for completing the experiments. The local Institutional Review Board (New York University’s Committee on Activities Involving Human Subjects) approved all protocols (IRB-FY2016-1357), in complete adherence to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent via an online form. Participants had self-reported normal hearing, were at least 18 years old, and reported no cognitive, developmental, neurological, psychiatric, or speech-language disorders. The total number of online participants was 488, and the data of 335 participants (208 females, 122 males, 5 other/prefer not to say, age range: 18 to 25) were included for analysis (see Quantification and statistical analysis for exclusion criteria, and Results for the sample size of each experiment).

The pipeline to generate audio stimuli with a designated peak AM frequency and temporal regularity parameters is composed of the following steps (resembling an inverse pipeline for analyzing audio recordings), which are conceptually illustrated in Fig 1 .

research in microbiology journal

  • An inverse fast Fourier transformation with random phases was applied to an AM spectrum to generate a 20-s time-domain signal with a 44.1-kHz sampling rate, and then it was transformed to an amplitude envelope [ 69 , 70 ].
  • The resulting amplitude envelope was used to modulate a 20- to 20,000-Hz low-noise noise (LNN) carrier sound. The LNN is a white noise with a flat amplitude envelope [ 71 , 72 ], which ensures that the amplitude fluctuations of the final stimuli were not caused by the carrier signal.
  • The middle 4-s segment of each 20-s amplitude-modulated LNN was extracted as a stimulus.
  • There were 100, 50, 50, and 50 stimuli generated for each condition of Experiments 1 to 4, respectively, and the root-mean-square values of all the stimuli were equalized within each experiment. All steps were performed using MATLAB R2020a.

The experiments were programmed on PsychoPy Builder (v2020.1.2) and executed on the Pavlovia.org platform.

The participants were required to perform the experiment using a browser on their personal computer, in a quiet environment with headphones on, and each listener could set the audio volume at a comfortable level. First, only those participants who passed a headphone screening task (see below) could proceed. Next, the practice phase included 4 trials; the AM parameters of these stimuli were within the range of, but not identical to, the parameter values used in the subsequent testing phase. On each practice trial, a stimulus was presented, and then participants were asked to make a binary judgment by clicking a button on the screen, without time limit. After the response, the next trial started. A probe trial was inserted in the practice phase, which presented 1 to 4 brief tones without warning in a 2-s window with random stimulus-onset asynchronies, and the participants were requested to indicate the number of tones by pressing the corresponding key. Participants could repeat the practice phase until they felt comfortable to proceed to the testing phase. Only in Experiments 3 and 4, a practice phase was inserted prior to each of the first music and speech blocks.

In the testing phase, for Experiments 1 and 2, for each participant, a set of 150 unique stimuli (15 or 10 per condition in Experiments 1 or 2, respectively) were randomly drawn from the stimulus pool, and they were randomly ordered within each of the first and second half of the experiment, resulting in a total of 300 testing trials. There was no cue between two halves of the experiment. The participants were not instructed regarding the occurrence rates of “music” or “speech.”

For Experiments 3 and 4, within each of the first and second halves of the experiment, there were 1 music block and 1 speech block, randomly ordered. Within each block, there were 75 unique stimuli (15 per condition) randomly drawn from the stimulus pool, and the same set of stimuli was used for all 4 blocks for each participant, resulting in a total of 150 trials for each task and, therefore, totaling 300 testing trials for the entire experiment. Before and during each block, there were text and visual cues on the screen to remind the participants of the current block type. The participants were instructed that 50% of the trials were music or speech and 50% were not music or speech (“others”), respectively, for each block type.

For all the experiments, the procedure of each testing trial was identical to the practice trial. A self-paced break was inserted every 10 trials, and the percentage of progress in the experiment was shown on the screen during the break. Twelve probe trials were mixed with roughly even spaces with the testing trials.

After the experiment, participants were directed to another webpage to anonymously fill out demographic information, the Goldsmiths musical sophistication index, and other background and task-related questions (not analyzed).

Headphone screening task.

The participants were requested to perform a headphone screening task prior to the main task, to ensure that they used headphones to complete our online experiments [ 73 ]. On each trial, participants were asked to identify the quietest tone (3-alternative forced choice) among three 1-s duration 200 Hz pure tones (with 100 ms ramps), including a binaurally in-phase loud tone, an antiphase loud tone, and an in-phase quiet tone of (−6 dB). Stimuli were presented sequentially with counterbalanced orders across 6 trials. Because the antiphase loud tone would be attenuated by phase cancelation in the air if it was played through loudspeakers, the quietest tone can only be correctly identified with headphones. Participants had to perform at least 5 out of 6 trials correctly to proceed.

Goldsmith musical sophistication index (Gold-MSI).

The Gold-MSI is one of the most common and reliable indices and for assessing musicality [ 30 ]. It is composed of 39 questions to assess multiple aspects of music expertise, including active engagement, perceptual abilities, musical training, singing abilities, and emotional responses. The General Musical Sophistication subscale is a general index that covers all the aspects of Gold-MSI, which ranges from 18 to 126; the mean and the standard deviation of the norm (147,633 participants) are 81.58 and 20.62, and the reliability α is 0.926.

Quantification and statistical analysis

Since all participants completed the study online without supervision, we used several exclusion criteria to ensure data quality. (1) The participants who did not complete both the experiment and the questionnaire, who did not pass the headphone screening task, admitted not using headphones throughout the experiment, made the same response for all the trials, or whose probe trial accuracy below 90%, were excluded. These criteria excluded 41, 19, 31, and 23 participants from Experiments 1, 2, 3, and 4. (2) Since the participants were instructed that the occurrence rate of music/speech was 50% in Experiments 3 and 4, the participants whose response biases exceeded 50 ± 15% in any task were excluded. This criterion excluded 16 and 23 participants from Experiments 3 and 4. Statistical test significance was assessed with α = .05, two-tailed. The specific tests used are reported in the Results section. The computations were performed on MATLAB R2020a and R2021b.

research in microbiology journal

Power analysis and sample sizes

As the effect size of this task was unknown, in the Experiment 1, we recruited more than 100 participants to reduce the risk of being underpowered and to estimate the statistical power for the following experiments. Based on the data of Experiment 1, a power analysis showed that the required number of participants was 20 when alpha level was set at 0.05 and statistical power at 0.8, and 36 when alpha level was set at 0.01 and statistical power at 0.9. Therefore, we targeted the sample size of Experiment 2 to be slightly above those levels ( n > 40). Although the tasks of Experiments 3 and 4 were similar to Experiments 1 and 2, the judgment of “speech versus others” and “music versus others” might have a lower statistical power than “music versus speech,” as “others” is not a well-defined category. Therefore, we set the target sample sizes to be double ( n ≈ 80) as the required sample size of alpha at 0.01 and power at 0.9.

Supporting information

S1 data. data underlying the plots in fig 2 – 4 ..

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002631.s001

Acknowledgments

We thank the Poeppel Lab members at New York University, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Ernst Struengmann Institute for Neuroscience, and Benjamin Morillon for their comments and support.

  • 1. Patel AD. Music, language, and the brain. Oxford University Press; 2008.
  • View Article
  • PubMed/NCBI
  • Google Scholar
  • 5. Schön D, Morillon B. Music and language. In: Thaut MH, Hodges DA, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Music and the Brain. Oxford University Press; 2019:391–416.
  • 31. Kingdom FAA, Prins N. Psychophysics: a practical introduction. Academic Press; 2016
  • 35. Elhilali M. Modulation representations for speech and music. In: Siedenburg K, Saitis C, McAdams S, Popper A, Fay R, editors. Timbre: Acoustics, Perception, and Cognition. Cham: Springer; 2019. pp. 335–359.
  • 71. Gaudrain E. Vocoder, v1.0. 2016. Available from: https://github.com/egaudrain/vocoder . https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.48120

College of Biological Sciences

College of Biological Sciences

Biochemistry and molecular biology major gemma le wins second place in this year’s lang prize.

Gemma Le headshot against blue and gold background

Prize recognizes students who make exceptional use of library resources and service

  • by Kristin Burns
  • June 04, 2024

The 2024 winners of the library’s Lang Prize were announced on Thursday, May 30. Now in its eighth year, the Norma J. Lang Prize for Undergraduate Information Research recognizes students who make exceptional use of library resources and services — such as primary source materials and special collections, online databases and journal articles, interlibrary loan services, or consulting with a librarian.

The Lang Prize is supported by a bequest from the late Professor Emerita of Botany Norma J. Lang, who taught at UC Davis for nearly 30 years. The prize honors Professor Lang’s devotion to her students and love of the research process.

This year, the College of Biological Sciences was pleased to see Gemma Le, a biochemistry and molecular biology major, received second place in the category of science, engineering and mathematics. 

"My priority was to identify and compare findings across different studies, while also pointing out strengths, limitations and potential risk of biases,” said Le. Her project was entitled, Efficacy of Non-Invasive Neuromodulation in the Treatment of Drug-Resistant Epilepsy. It explored the efficacy and safety of non-invasive alternatives to surgery in treating patients with drug-resistant epilepsy. She also investigates potential improvements in quality of life resulting from this treatment. With guidance from librarian Erik Fausak, Le consulted library databases and reviewed clinical trial results.

A group of students standing indoors and holding certificates

Media Resources

  • Read Gemma Le’s Paper (pdf)
  • Read  Gemma Le’s reflective essay  from their Lang Prize application
  • Learn more about Professor Lang’s legacy
  • Celebrating our 2024 Lang Prize Winners – UC Davis Library

Primary Category

Secondary categories.

IMAGES

  1. Research in Microbiology : AkiNik Publications

    research in microbiology journal

  2. Microbiology: Current Research

    research in microbiology journal

  3. Journal of Basic Microbiology

    research in microbiology journal

  4. Novel Research in Microbiology Journal

    research in microbiology journal

  5. Jundishapur Journal of Microbiology

    research in microbiology journal

  6. Canadian Journal of Infectious Diseases and Medical Microbiology

    research in microbiology journal

VIDEO

  1. Microbiology k career aspects|| kyu le MICROBIOLOGY ||Research ||medical microbiology

  2. Top 10 hot research areas in microbiology #microbiology #research

  3. Microbiology lab#medical#microbiology #clinicaodontologica #research#love #mukbang #pathology#doctor

  4. Top 15 Best Research Topics for microbiology for researchers and M.sc. students #study #yt #video

  5. Microbiology International

  6. Microbiology Journal Club

COMMENTS

  1. Research in Microbiology

    Research in Microbiology is a free online journal that publishes original articles, reviews and letters on all aspects of microbiology. The journal covers topics such as microbial taxonomy, ecology, genetics, genomics, gene regulation, viruses, biofilms and interactions with environment and host.

  2. Research in Microbiology

    2005 — Volume 156. Previous. Page 1 of 2. Annales de l'Institut Pasteur / Microbiologie. Read the latest articles of Research in Microbiology at ScienceDirect.com, Elsevier's leading platform of peer-reviewed scholarly literature.

  3. Research in Microbiology

    Research in Microbiology. Supports open access ... Journal pre-proofs: versions of an article that have undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but are not yet definitive versions of record. These versions will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and ...

  4. Microbiology

    Microbiology is the study of microscopic organisms, such as bacteria, viruses, archaea, fungi and protozoa. This discipline includes fundamental research on the biochemistry, physiology, cell ...

  5. Home

    Microbiology is an international peer-reviewed journal that addresses a broad spectrum of topics in both fundamental and applied microbiology. Publishes experimental and theoretical articles, critical reviews, and short communications. The target audience is specialists at research institutions and medical workers.

  6. Articles

    To establish a method to induce Campylobacter jejuni colonization in the intestines of C57BL/6 mice through antibiotic-induced microbiome depletion. Haohao Chen, Yanfang Zhang, Yi Pan, Lin Wu, Wenqian Wang, Hui Zhang and Hongqiang Lou. BMC Microbiology 2024 24 :156. Research Published on: 9 May 2024.

  7. Research articles

    Research Cross-Journal Editorial Team ... Nature Microbiology (Nat Microbiol) ISSN 2058-5276 (online) nature.com sitemap. About Nature Portfolio. About us; Press releases; Press office ...

  8. Home page

    BMC Microbiology is an open access, peer-reviewed journal that considers articles on all microorganisms - bacteria, archaea, algae and fungi, viruses, unicellular parasites and helminths. It considers studies on all aspects of the biology and biochemistry of microorganisms including but not limited to cell biology, genomics, signalling, the interaction of the microbes with the environment and ...

  9. Research in Microbiology

    Research in Microbiology. Filtration of liquids through 0.2-microm filters is a common and often-used method for the removal of microorganisms from heat-sensitive solutions. Such 0.2 microm ...

  10. Subscribe to Research in Microbiology

    ISSN: 0923-2508. Editor-In-Chief: Msadek. Next planned ship date: July 23, 2024. 5 Year impact factor: 3.9. Impact factor: 2.6. Journal metrics. This journal has no page charges, publication is free of charge. Papers published in the journal cover all aspects of microbial taxonomy, phylogeny, ecology, physiology and metabolism, molecular ...

  11. Research in Microbiology

    Research in Microbiology is the direct descendant of the original Pasteur periodical entitled Annales de l'Institut Pasteur, created in 1887 by Emile Duclaux under the patronage of Louis Pasteur. ... Not every article in a journal is considered primary research and therefore "citable", this chart shows the ratio of a journal's articles ...

  12. Microbiology

    Originally Journal of General Microbiology, we have been publishing the latest advances in microbiology since 1947. Today the journal reflects the diversity and importance of microbiology in addressing current global challenges, such as food security, environmental sustainability and health, by publishing fundamental and applied research across ...

  13. Journal Rankings on Microbiology

    International Scientific Journal & Country Ranking. SCImago Journal Country & Rank SCImago Institutions Rankings SCImago Media Rankings SCImago Iber SCImago Research Centers Ranking SCImago Graphica Ediciones Profesionales de la ... Nature Reviews Microbiology: journal: 9.639 Q1: 372: 173: 503: 9764: 12098: 361: 26.41: 56.44: 34.39: 2: Nature ...

  14. Frontiers in Microbiology

    See all (1,737) Learn more about Research Topics. The most cited microbiology journal which advances our understanding of the role microbes play in addressing global challenges such as healthcare, food security, and climate change.

  15. Home

    Journal information. Co-publication with The Microbiological Society of Korea. Publishes papers that deal with research on microorganisms, including archaea, bacteria, yeasts, fungi, microalgae, protozoa, and simple eukaryotic ...

  16. Frontiers in Microbiology

    The most cited microbiology journal which advances our understanding of the role microbes play in addressing global challenges such as healthcare, food security, and climate change. ... Part of a research topic; Not part of a research topic; Clear all Filters Articles. Original Research. Published on 03 Jun 2024 Longitudinal molecular analysis ...

  17. Microbiological Research

    Microbiological Research publishes research on prokaryotic and eukaryotic microorganisms such as yeasts, fungi, bacteria, archaea, and protozoa. The journal considers research on interactions between pathogenic microorganisms and their environment or hosts. The research should be original and include molecular aspects to generate broad interest.

  18. Microbiology Research

    Microbiology Research is an international, scientific, peer-reviewed open access journal published quarterly online by MDPI (from Volume 11 Issue 2-2020). Open Access — free for readers, with article processing charges (APC) paid by authors or their institutions. High Visibility: indexed within Scopus , ESCI (Web of Science) , Embase, and ...

  19. Frontiers in Microbiology

    Global Plant-Microbe Detection Using Metagenomics, Metatrascriptomics and Pathogen Specific Detection Using AI, CRISPR and HTS Technologies. The most cited microbiology journal which advances our understanding of the role microbes play in addressing global challenges such as healthcare, food security, and climate change.

  20. Six Key Topics in Microbiology: 2024

    Six Key Topics in Microbiology: 2024. This collection from the FEMS journals presents the latest high-quality research in six key topic areas of microbiology that have an impact across the world. All of the FEMS journals aim to serve the microbiology community with timely and authoritative research and reviews, and by investing back into the ...

  21. Reviews and Research in Medical Microbiology

    Reviews and Research in Medical Microbiology is a quarterly review journal which provides a balanced coverage of the whole field of medical microbiology. The Journal publishes state-of-the art reviews, mini-reviews, case presentations and original research from on-going research of the latest developments and techniques in medical microbiology, virology, mycology, parasitology, clinical ...

  22. Novel Research in Microbiology Journal

    Editor Guide Video. "Novel Research in Microbiology Journal, NRMJ" is a new blind peer-reviewed international and open-access journal that bimonthly publishes papers that deal with novel research on microorganisms, including bacteria, yeasts, mold fungi, and viruses. Novel Research in Microbiology Journal, NRMJ" is published by The ...

  23. Home

    Systems Microbiology and Biomanufacturing is a platform for high-quality research in the interdisciplinary areas of industrial microbiology and biomanufacturing process development.. Topics include Food Microbiology and Biotechnology, Industrial Microbiology and Biotechnology, Metabolic Engineering and Synthetic Biology, Fermentation Engineering, and so on.

  24. A concept for international societally relevant microbiology education

    Microbiology research projects and coursework: undergraduate and postgraduate students in some universities are already being offered the creation of IMiLI teaching resources as projects and coursework. For example, a trial group of year-2 undergraduates designed games, posters or quizzes based on a TF of their choice that could be used by ...

  25. Guide for authors

    Please check the relevant section in this Guide for Authors for more details. Ensure that the following items are present: One author has been designated as the corresponding author with contact details: • E-mail address. • Full postal address. All necessary files have been uploaded: Manuscript: • Include keywords.

  26. Trends and Prospects of Genetic and Molecular Research in Plants

    Editor's Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area.

  27. Research in Microbiology

    Yuanyuan Ma, Xiuqin Wu, Shuying Li, Lie Tang, ... Qianli An. Article 103924. View PDF. Previous vol/issue. Next vol/issue. ISSN: 0923-2508. Read the latest articles of Research in Microbiology at ScienceDirect.com, Elsevier's leading platform of peer-reviewed scholarly literature.

  28. Journal of Zoological Systematics and Evolutionary Research

    Journal of Zoological Systematics and Evolutionary Research is a peer-reviewed, international forum for publication of high-quality research on systematic zoology and evolutionary biology. ... Ciona intestinalis is a model chordate in various fields of biology. Molecular studies have identified two forms, named "type A" and "type B", as ...

  29. The human auditory system uses amplitude modulation to distinguish

    What enables the human auditory system to distinguish between music and speech? Through a reductionist approach involving judgments on artificially generated ambiguous noise clips, this study shows that the auditory system relies on basic acoustic parameters, such as amplitude modulation rates and regularities, to differentiate between music and speech.

  30. Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Major Gemma Le Wins Second Place in

    The 2024 winners of the library's Lang Prize were announced on Thursday, May 30. Now in its eighth year, the Norma J. Lang Prize for Undergraduate Information Research recognizes students who make exceptional use of library resources and services — such as primary source materials and special collections, online databases and journal articles, interlibrary loan services, or consulting with ...