• Privacy Policy

Research Method

Home » Basic Research – Types, Methods and Examples

Basic Research – Types, Methods and Examples

Table of Contents

Basic Research

Basic Research

Definition:

Basic Research, also known as Fundamental or Pure Research , is scientific research that aims to increase knowledge and understanding about the natural world without necessarily having any practical or immediate applications. It is driven by curiosity and the desire to explore new frontiers of knowledge rather than by the need to solve a specific problem or to develop a new product.

Types of Basic Research

Types of Basic Research are as follows:

Experimental Research

This type of research involves manipulating one or more variables to observe their effect on a particular phenomenon. It aims to test hypotheses and establish cause-and-effect relationships.

Observational Research

This type of research involves observing and documenting natural phenomena without manipulating any variables. It aims to describe and understand the behavior of the observed system.

Theoretical Research

This type of research involves developing and testing theories and models to explain natural phenomena. It aims to provide a framework for understanding and predicting observations and experiments.

Descriptive Research

This type of research involves describing and cataloging natural phenomena without attempting to explain or understand them. It aims to provide a comprehensive and accurate picture of the observed system.

Comparative Research

This type of research involves comparing different systems or phenomena to identify similarities and differences. It aims to understand the underlying principles that govern different natural phenomena.

Historical Research

This type of research involves studying past events, developments, and discoveries to understand how science has evolved over time. It aims to provide insights into the factors that have influenced scientific progress and the role of basic research in shaping our understanding of the world.

Data Collection Methods

Some common data collection methods used in basic research include:

  • Observation : This involves watching and recording natural phenomena in a systematic and structured way. Observations can be made in a laboratory setting or in the field and can be qualitative or quantitative.
  • Surveys and questionnaires: These are tools for collecting data from a large number of individuals about their attitudes, beliefs, behaviors, and experiences. Surveys and questionnaires can be administered in person, by mail, or online.
  • Interviews : Interviews involve asking questions to a person or a group of people to gather information about their experiences, opinions, and perspectives. Interviews can be structured, semi-structured, or unstructured.
  • Experiments : Experiments involve manipulating one or more variables and observing their effect on a particular phenomenon. Experiments can be conducted in a laboratory or in the field and can be controlled or naturalistic.
  • Case studies : Case studies involve in-depth analysis of a particular individual, group, or phenomenon. Case studies can provide rich and detailed information about complex phenomena.
  • Archival research : Archival research involves analyzing existing data, documents, and records to answer research questions. Archival research can be used to study historical events, trends, and developments.
  • Simulation : Simulation involves creating a computer model of a particular phenomenon to study its behavior and predict its future outcomes. Simulation can be used to study complex systems that are difficult to study in the real world.

Data Analysis Methods

Some common data analysis methods used in basic research include:

  • Descriptive statistics: This involves summarizing and describing data using measures such as mean, median, mode, and standard deviation. Descriptive statistics provide a simple and easy way to understand the basic properties of the data.
  • Inferential statistics : This involves making inferences about a population based on data collected from a sample. Inferential statistics can be used to test hypotheses, estimate parameters, and quantify uncertainty.
  • Qualitative analysis : This involves analyzing data that are not numerical in nature, such as text, images, or audio recordings. Qualitative analysis can involve coding, categorizing, and interpreting data to identify themes, patterns, and relationships.
  • Content analysis: This involves analyzing the content of text, images, or audio recordings to identify specific words, phrases, or themes. Content analysis can be used to study communication, media, and discourse.
  • Multivariate analysis: This involves analyzing data that have multiple variables or factors. Multivariate analysis can be used to identify patterns and relationships among variables, cluster similar observations, and reduce the dimensionality of the data.
  • Network analysis: This involves analyzing the structure and dynamics of networks, such as social networks, communication networks, or ecological networks. Network analysis can be used to study the relationships and interactions among individuals, groups, or entities.
  • Machine learning : This involves using algorithms and models to analyze and make predictions based on data. Machine learning can be used to identify patterns, classify observations, and make predictions based on complex data sets.

Basic Research Methodology

Basic research methodology refers to the approach, techniques, and procedures used to conduct basic research. The following are some common steps involved in basic research methodology:

  • Formulating research questions or hypotheses : This involves identifying the research problem and formulating specific questions or hypotheses that can guide the research.
  • Reviewing the literature: This involves reviewing and synthesizing existing research on the topic of interest to identify gaps, controversies, and areas for further investigation.
  • Designing the study: This involves designing a study that is appropriate for the research question or hypothesis. The study design can involve experiments, observations, surveys, case studies, or other methods.
  • Collecting data: This involves collecting data using appropriate methods and instruments, such as observation, surveys, experiments, or interviews.
  • Analyzing data: This involves analyzing the collected data using appropriate methods, such as descriptive or inferential statistics, qualitative analysis, or content analysis.
  • Interpreting results : This involves interpreting the results of the data analysis in light of the research question or hypothesis and the existing literature.
  • Drawing conclusions: This involves drawing conclusions based on the interpretation of the results and assessing their implications for the research question or hypothesis.
  • Communicating findings : This involves communicating the research findings in the form of research reports, journal articles, conference presentations, or other forms of dissemination.

Applications of Basic Research

Some applications of basic research include:

  • Medical breakthroughs : Basic research in fields such as biology, chemistry, and physics has led to important medical breakthroughs, including the discovery of antibiotics, vaccines, and new drugs.
  • Technology advancements: Basic research in fields such as computer science, physics, and engineering has led to advancements in technology, such as the development of the internet, smartphones, and other electronic devices.
  • Environmental solutions: Basic research in fields such as ecology, geology, and meteorology has led to the development of solutions to environmental problems, such as climate change, air pollution, and water contamination.
  • Economic growth: Basic research can stimulate economic growth by creating new industries and markets based on scientific discoveries and technological advancements.
  • National security: Basic research in fields such as physics, chemistry, and biology has led to the development of new technologies for national security, including encryption, radar, and stealth technology.

Examples of Basic Research

Here are some examples of basic research:

  • Astronomy : Astronomers conduct basic research to understand the fundamental principles that govern the universe, such as the laws of gravity, the behavior of stars and galaxies, and the origins of the universe.
  • Genetics : Geneticists conduct basic research to understand the genetic basis of various traits, diseases, and disorders. This research can lead to the development of new treatments and therapies for genetic diseases.
  • Physics : Physicists conduct basic research to understand the fundamental principles of matter and energy, such as quantum mechanics, particle physics, and cosmology. This research can lead to new technologies and advancements in fields such as medicine and engineering.
  • Neuroscience: Neuroscientists conduct basic research to understand the structure and function of the brain, including how it processes information and controls behavior. This research can lead to new treatments and therapies for neurological disorders and brain injuries.
  • Mathematics : Mathematicians conduct basic research to develop and explore new mathematical theories, such as number theory, topology, and geometry. This research can lead to new applications in fields such as computer science, physics, and engineering.
  • Chemistry : Chemists conduct basic research to understand the fundamental properties of matter and how it interacts with other substances. This research can lead to the development of new materials, drugs, and technologies.

Purpose of Basic Research

The purpose of basic research, also known as fundamental or pure research, is to expand knowledge in a particular field or discipline without any specific practical application in mind. The primary goal of basic research is to advance our understanding of the natural world and to uncover fundamental principles and relationships that underlie complex phenomena.

Basic research is often exploratory in nature, with researchers seeking to answer fundamental questions about how the world works. The research may involve conducting experiments, collecting and analyzing data, or developing new theories and hypotheses. Basic research often requires a high degree of creativity, innovation, and intellectual curiosity, as well as a willingness to take risks and pursue unconventional lines of inquiry.

Although basic research is not conducted with a specific practical outcome in mind, it can lead to significant practical applications in various fields. Many of the major scientific discoveries and technological advancements of the past century have been rooted in basic research, from the discovery of antibiotics to the development of the internet.

In summary, the purpose of basic research is to expand knowledge and understanding in a particular field or discipline, with the goal of uncovering fundamental principles and relationships that can help us better understand the natural world. While the practical applications of basic research may not always be immediately apparent, it has led to significant scientific and technological advancements that have benefited society in numerous ways.

When to use Basic Research

Basic research is generally conducted when scientists and researchers are seeking to expand knowledge and understanding in a particular field or discipline. It is particularly useful when there are gaps in our understanding of fundamental principles and relationships that underlie complex phenomena. Here are some situations where basic research might be particularly useful:

  • Exploring new fields: Basic research can be particularly valuable when researchers are exploring new fields or areas of inquiry where little is known. By conducting basic research, scientists can establish a foundation of knowledge that can be built upon in future studies.
  • Testing new theories: Basic research can be useful when researchers are testing new theories or hypotheses that have not been tested before. This can help scientists to gain a better understanding of how the world works and to identify areas where further research is needed.
  • Developing new technologies : Basic research can be important for developing new technologies and innovations. By conducting basic research, scientists can uncover new materials, properties, and relationships that can be used to develop new products or technologies.
  • Investigating complex phenomena : Basic research can be particularly valuable when investigating complex phenomena that are not yet well understood. By conducting basic research, scientists can gain a better understanding of the underlying principles and relationships that govern complex systems.
  • Advancing scientific knowledge: Basic research is important for advancing scientific knowledge in general. By conducting basic research, scientists can uncover new principles and relationships that can be applied across multiple fields of study.

Characteristics of Basic Research

Here are some of the main characteristics of basic research:

  • Focus on fundamental knowledge : Basic research is focused on expanding our understanding of the natural world and uncovering fundamental principles and relationships that underlie complex phenomena. The primary goal of basic research is to advance knowledge without any specific practical application in mind.
  • Exploratory in nature: Basic research is often exploratory in nature, with researchers seeking to answer fundamental questions about how the world works. The research may involve conducting experiments, collecting and analyzing data, or developing new theories and hypotheses.
  • Long-term focus: Basic research is often focused on long-term outcomes rather than immediate practical applications. The insights and discoveries generated by basic research may take years or even decades to translate into practical applications.
  • High degree of creativity and innovation : Basic research often requires a high degree of creativity, innovation, and intellectual curiosity. Researchers must be willing to take risks and pursue unconventional lines of inquiry.
  • Emphasis on scientific rigor: Basic research is conducted using the scientific method, which emphasizes the importance of rigorous experimental design, data collection and analysis, and peer review.
  • Interdisciplinary: Basic research is often interdisciplinary, drawing on multiple fields of study to address complex research questions. Basic research can be conducted in fields ranging from physics and chemistry to biology and psychology.
  • Open-ended : Basic research is open-ended, meaning that it does not have a specific end goal in mind. Researchers may follow unexpected paths or uncover new lines of inquiry that they had not anticipated.

Advantages of Basic Research

Here are some of the main advantages of basic research:

  • Advancing scientific knowledge: Basic research is essential for expanding our understanding of the natural world and uncovering fundamental principles and relationships that underlie complex phenomena. This knowledge can be applied across multiple fields of study and can lead to significant scientific and technological advancements.
  • Fostering innovation: Basic research often requires a high degree of creativity, innovation, and intellectual curiosity. By encouraging scientists to pursue unconventional lines of inquiry and take risks, basic research can lead to breakthrough discoveries and innovations.
  • Stimulating economic growth : Basic research can lead to the development of new technologies and products that can stimulate economic growth and create new industries. Many of the major scientific and technological advancements of the past century have been rooted in basic research.
  • Improving health and well-being: Basic research can lead to the development of new drugs, therapies, and medical treatments that can improve health and well-being. For example, many of the major advances in medical science, such as the development of antibiotics and vaccines, were rooted in basic research.
  • Training the next generation of scientists : Basic research is essential for training the next generation of scientists and researchers. By providing opportunities for young scientists to engage in research and gain hands-on experience, basic research helps to develop the skills and expertise needed to advance scientific knowledge in the future.
  • Encouraging interdisciplinary collaboration : Basic research often requires collaboration between scientists from different fields of study. By fostering interdisciplinary collaboration, basic research can lead to new insights and discoveries that would not be possible through single-discipline research alone.

Limitations of Basic Research

Here are some of the main limitations of basic research:

  • Lack of immediate practical applications : Basic research is often focused on long-term outcomes rather than immediate practical applications. The insights and discoveries generated by basic research may take years or even decades to translate into practical applications.
  • High cost and time requirements: Basic research can be expensive and time-consuming, as it often requires sophisticated equipment, specialized facilities, and large research teams. Funding for basic research can be limited, making it difficult to sustain long-term projects.
  • Ethical concerns : Basic research may involve working with animal models or human subjects, raising ethical concerns around the use of animals or the safety and well-being of human participants.
  • Uncertainty around outcomes: Basic research is often open-ended, meaning that it does not have a specific end goal in mind. This uncertainty can make it difficult to justify funding for basic research, as it is difficult to predict what outcomes the research will produce.
  • Difficulty in communicating results : Basic research can produce complex and technical findings that may be difficult to communicate to the general public or policymakers. This can make it challenging to generate public support for basic research or to translate basic research findings into policy or practical applications.

About the author

' src=

Muhammad Hassan

Researcher, Academic Writer, Web developer

You may also like

Questionnaire

Questionnaire – Definition, Types, and Examples

Case Study Research

Case Study – Methods, Examples and Guide

Observational Research

Observational Research – Methods and Guide

Quantitative Research

Quantitative Research – Methods, Types and...

Explanatory Research

Explanatory Research – Types, Methods, Guide

Survey Research

Survey Research – Types, Methods, Examples

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Basic Research in Psychology

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

what is basic research study

Emily is a board-certified science editor who has worked with top digital publishing brands like Voices for Biodiversity, Study.com, GoodTherapy, Vox, and Verywell.

what is basic research study

Basic research—also known as fundamental or pure research—refers to study and research meant to increase our scientific knowledge base. This type of research is often purely theoretical, with the intent of increasing our understanding of certain phenomena or behavior. In contrast with applied research, basic research doesn't seek to solve or treat these problems.

Basic Research Examples

Basic research in psychology might explore:

  • Whether stress levels influence how often students engage in academic cheating
  • How caffeine consumption affects the brain
  • Whether men or women are more likely to be diagnosed with depression
  • How attachment styles among children of divorced parents compare to those raised by married parents

In all of these examples, the goal is merely to increase knowledge on a topic, not to come up with a practical solution to a problem.

The Link Between Basic and Applied Research

As Stanovich (2007) noted, many practical solutions to real-world problems have emerged directly from basic research. For this reason, the distinction between basic research and applied research is often simply a matter of time. As social psychologist Kurt Lewin once observed, "There is nothing so practical as a good theory."

For example, researchers might conduct basic research on how stress levels impact students academically, emotionally, and socially. The results of these theoretical explorations might lead to further studies designed to solve specific problems. Researchers might initially observe that students with high stress levels are more prone to dropping out of college before graduating. These first studies are examples of basic research designed to learn more about the topic.

As a result, scientists might then design research to determine what interventions might best lower these stress levels. Such studies would be examples of applied research. The purpose of applied research is specifically focused on solving a real problem that exists in the world. Thanks to the foundations established by basic research, psychologists can then design interventions that will help students effectively manage their stress levels , with the hopes of improving college retention rates.

Why Basic Research Is Important

The possible applications of basic research might not be obvious right away. During the earliest phases of basic research, scientists might not even be able to see how the information gleaned from theoretical research might ever apply to real-world problems. However, this foundational knowledge is essential. By learning as much as possible about a topic, researchers are able to gather what they need to know about an issue to fully understand the impact it may have.

"For example, early neuroscientists conducted basic research studies to understand how neurons function. The applications of this knowledge were not clear until much later when neuroscientists better understood how this neural functioning affected behavior," explained author Dawn M. McBride in her text The Process of Research in Psychology . "The understanding of the basic knowledge of neural functioning became useful in helping individuals with disorders long after this research had been completed."

Basic Research Methods

Basic research relies on many types of investigatory tools. These include observation, case studies, experiments, focus groups, surveys, interviews—anything that increases the scope of knowledge on the topic at hand.

Frequently Asked Questions

Psychologists interested in social behavior often undertake basic research. Social/community psychologists engaging in basic research are not trying to solve particular problems; rather, they want to learn more about why humans act the way they do.

Basic research is an effort to expand the scope of knowledge on a topic. Applied research uses such knowledge to solve specific problems.

An effective basic research problem statement outlines the importance of the topic; the study's significance and methods; what the research is investigating; how the results will be reported; and what the research will probably require.

Basic research might investigate, for example, the relationship between academic stress levels and cheating; how caffeine affects the brain; depression incidence in men vs. women; or attachment styles among children of divorced and married parents.

By learning as much as possible about a topic, researchers can come to fully understand the impact it may have. This knowledge can then become the basis of applied research to solve a particular problem within the topic area.

Stanovich KE.  How to Think Straight About Psychology . 8th edition. Boston, MA: Pearson Allyn and Bacon; 2007.

McCain KW. “Nothing as practical as a good theory” Does Lewin's Maxim still have salience in the applied social sciences? Proceedings of the Association for Information Science and Technology . 2015;52(1):1-4. doi:10.1002/pra2.2015.145052010077

McBride DM. The Process of Research in Psychology . 3rd edition . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2015.

Committee on Department of Defense Basic Research. APPENDIX D: Definitions of basic, applied, and fundamental research . In: Assessment of Department of Defense Basic Research. Washington, D.C.: The National Academic Press; 2005.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

what is basic research study

What is Basic Research?

what is basic research study

Introduction

What is the meaning of basic research, examples of basic research, how do i perform basic research.

Basic science research is an essential pillar of scientific knowledge, because it extends understanding, provides new insights, and contributes to the advancement of science and fundamental knowledge across disciplines. In contrast, applied research aims for the discovery of practical solutions, which can involve using a technology or innovation that stems from existing knowledge. Basic science research potentially allows for generating ideas on which applied science can build novel inquiry and useful applications.

The process for conducting basic research is essentially the same as in an applied research orientation, but a better understanding of the distinction may prove increasingly important when crafting your research inquiry. In this article, we'll detail the characteristics and importance of basic research.

what is basic research study

One of the key distinctions in science is the divide between basic and applied research . Applied research is directly associated with practical applications such as:

  • career development
  • program evaluation
  • policy reform
  • community action

In inquiries regarding each of these applications, researchers identify a specific problem to be solved and design a study intentionally aimed at developing solutions to that problem. Basic research is less concerned about specific problems and more focused on the nature of understanding.

what is basic research study

Characteristics of basic research

Research that advances understanding of knowledge has distinguishing characteristics and important considerations.

  • Focus on theoretical development . Rather than focus on practical applications, scholars in basic science research are more interested in ordering data and understanding in a scientific manner. This means expanding the consensus understanding of theory and the proposal of new theoretical frameworks that ultimately further research.
  • Exploratory research questions . Basic research tends to look at areas where there is insufficient theoretical coherence to empirically understand phenomena. In other words, basic research often employs research questions that seek greater definition of knowledge.
  • Funding for basic science . The nature of the support available for research depends on whether the science is basic or applied . Government agencies, national institutes, and private organizations all have different objectives, making some more appropriate for basic research than others.
  • Writing for research dissemination . Academic journals exist on a continuum between theoretical and practical orientations. Journals that are more interested in theoretical and methodological discussions are more appropriate for basic research than are journals that look for more practical implications arising from research.

The brief survey of these characteristics should guide researchers about how they should approach research design in terms of feasibility, methods, and execution. This discussion shouldn't preclude you from pursuing basic research if it is more appropriate to your research inquiry. Instead, it should inform you of the opportunities, advantages, and challenges of basic research.

what is basic research study

Importance of basic research

Fundamental research may seem aimless and unfocused if it doesn't yield any direct practical implications. However, its contribution to scholarly discussion cannot be overstated as it guides the development of theories and facilitates critical discussion about what applied studies to pursue next.

Basic science has guided fields such as microbiology, engineering, and chemistry. Scientists ultimately use its findings to develop new methods in treating disease and innovating on new technology.

Its contribution to the social sciences through observation and longitudinal study is also immeasurable. While basic research is often a precursor to more applied science, the theories it generates spur further study that ultimately leads to professional development programs and policy reform in social institutions.

what is basic research study

Shape your data, shape your research with ATLAS.ti

Dive into a transformative analysis experience with a free trial of our powerful data analysis software.

Different fields rely on both applied and basic science for generating new knowledge. While applied research looks to yield direct benefits through real-world applications, fundamental research provides the necessary theoretical foundation for practical research in various fields.

Basic research example in education

Basic research in schooling contexts focuses on understanding the nature of teaching and learning or the processes within educational environments before any focused investigation can be designed, let alone conducted. Basic research is necessary in this case because of the various situated differences across learners who come from different cultures and backgrounds.

Basic research in education looks at various inquiries such as how teachers and students interact with each other and how alternative assessments can create positive learning outcomes. Ultimately, this may lead to applied research that can facilitate the creation of teacher education and professional development programs.

what is basic research study

Basic research example in psychology

Psychology is a field that is under constant development. Basic research is essential to developing theories related to human behavior and mental processes. The subfield of cognition is a significant benefactor of basic research as it relies on novel theoretical frameworks relating to memory and learning.

what is basic research study

Basic research example in health

A great deal of health research that reaches public consciousness is undoubtedly applied research. The development of vaccines and other medicine to combat the COVID-19 pandemic was one such line of inquiry that addressed a practical need.

That said, scientists will undoubtedly credit basic research as a precursor to medical breakthroughs in applied science research. The knowledge gained through basic research laid the foundation for genomic sequencing of the COVID-19 virus, while experiments on living systems created knowledge about how to safely vaccinate the human body.

The National Institute of Health sponsors such basic research and research in other areas such as human DNA, while the National Science Foundation funds basic research on topics such as gender stereotypes and stress levels.

what is basic research study

At its core, all scientific inquiry seeks to identify causal factors, relationships, and distinguishing characteristics among concepts and phenomena. As a result, the process is essentially the same for basic or applied science. Nonetheless, it is worth reviewing the process.

  • Research design . Identify gaps in existing research that novel inquiry can address. A rigorous literature review can help identify theoretical or methodological gaps that a new study with an exploratory research question can address.
  • Data collection . Exploratory research questions tend to prioritize data collection methods such as interviews , focus groups , and observations . Basic research, as a result, casts a wide net for any and all potential data that can facilitate generation of theoretical developments.
  • Data analysis . At this stage, the goal is to organize and view your data in such a way that facilitates the identification of key insights. Analysis in basic research serves the dual purpose of filtering data through existing theoretical frameworks and generating new theory.
  • Research dissemination . Once you determine your findings, you will want to present your insights in an empirical and rigorous manner. Visualizing data in your papers and presentations is useful for pointing out the most relevant data and analysis in your study.

Whatever your research, make it happen with ATLAS.ti

Turn data into knowledge and innovation with our powerful analysis tools. Download a free trial today.

what is basic research study

  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer
  • QuestionPro

survey software icon

  • Solutions Industries Gaming Automotive Sports and events Education Government Travel & Hospitality Financial Services Healthcare Cannabis Technology Use Case NPS+ Communities Audience Contactless surveys Mobile LivePolls Member Experience GDPR Positive People Science 360 Feedback Surveys
  • Resources Blog eBooks Survey Templates Case Studies Training Help center

what is basic research study

Home Market Research Research Tools and Apps

Basic Research: What it is with examples

basic research

In building knowledge, there are many stages and methodologies to generate insights that contribute to its understanding and advancement; basic research and applied research are usually the most effective on this path.

Understanding research allows us to understand all the properties of a specific science or phenomenon at a fundamental level. Some examples are branches such as sociology, humanities, and other scientific fields; below, we will tell you everything you need to know about this type of research and its possible applications.

What is Basic Research?

Basic Research is a type of research used in the scientific field to understand and extend our knowledge about a specific phenomenon or field. It is also accepted as pure investigation or fundamental research .

This type of research contributes to the intellectual body of knowledge. Basic research is concerned with the generalization of a theory in a branch of knowledge; its purpose is usually to generate data that confirm or refute the initial thesis of the study.

It can also be called foundational research; many things get built on this foundation, and more practical applications are made.

Basic Research vs. Applied Research

Basic Research finds its counterpart and complement in applied research. They are two handy research methods when generating and giving a utility to the generated data. There are very marked differences, and understanding them will allow you to understand the path followed to create new knowledge.

The most important difference between basic research and applied research lies in the objective of each. It seeks to expand the information and understanding of the object of study, while applied research aims to provide a solution to the problem studied.

The relationship between these two types of research is usually very close since the methodologies used are often quite similar; the significant change is found in the initial and final point of the investigation.

Basic Research Examples

There can be many examples of basic research; here are some of them:

  • A study of how stress affects labor productivity.
  • Studying the best factors of pricing strategies.
  • Understand the client’s level of satisfaction before certain interactions with the company providing solutions.
  • The understanding of the leadership style of a particular company.

Advantages & Disadvantages

Basic research is critical for expanding the pool of knowledge in any discipline. The introductory course usually does not have a strict period, and the researcher’s concern commonly guides them. The conclusion of the fundamental course is generally applicable in a wide range of cases and plots.

At the same time, the basic study has disadvantages as well. The findings of this type of study have limited or no constructive conclusions. In another sense, fundamental studies do not resolve complex and definite business problems, but it does help you understand them better.

Taking actions and decisions based on the results of this type of research will increase the impact these insights may have on the problem studied if that is the purpose.

LEARN ABOUT: Theoretical Research

How to do basic research?

This process follows the same steps as a standard research methodology. The most crucial point is to define a thesis or theory that involves a perfectly defined case study; this can be a phenomenon or a research problem observed in a particular place.

There are many types of research, such as longitudinal studies , observational research , and exploratory studies. So the first thing you should do is determine if you can obtain the desired result with research or if it is better to opt for another type of research.

Once you have determined your research methodology, the data collection process begins, also depending on your type of study; sometimes, you can collect the data passively through observation or experimentation. On other occasions, intervene directly and collect quantitative information with tools such as surveys.

Platforms like QuestionPro will help you have a wide range of functions and tools to carry out your research; its survey software has helped students and professionals obtain all the information necessary to generate high-value insights.

In addition, it has a data analysis suite with which you can analyze all this information using all kinds of reports for a more straightforward interpretation of the final results.

QuestionPro is much more than survey software ; we have a solution for each specific problem and industry. We also offer data management platforms such as our research data repository called Insights Hub.

MORE LIKE THIS

what is basic research study

What Are My Employees Really Thinking? The Power of Open-ended Survey Analysis

May 24, 2024

When I think of “disconnected”, it is important that this is not just in relation to people analytics, Employee Experience or Customer Experience - it is also relevant to looking across them.

I Am Disconnected – Tuesday CX Thoughts

May 21, 2024

Customer success tools

20 Best Customer Success Tools of 2024

May 20, 2024

AI-Based Services in Market Research

AI-Based Services Buying Guide for Market Research (based on ESOMAR’s 20 Questions) 

Other categories.

  • Academic Research
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Assessments
  • Brand Awareness
  • Case Studies
  • Communities
  • Consumer Insights
  • Customer effort score
  • Customer Engagement
  • Customer Experience
  • Customer Loyalty
  • Customer Research
  • Customer Satisfaction
  • Employee Benefits
  • Employee Engagement
  • Employee Retention
  • Friday Five
  • General Data Protection Regulation
  • Insights Hub
  • Life@QuestionPro
  • Market Research
  • Mobile diaries
  • Mobile Surveys
  • New Features
  • Online Communities
  • Question Types
  • Questionnaire
  • QuestionPro Products
  • Release Notes
  • Research Tools and Apps
  • Revenue at Risk
  • Survey Templates
  • Training Tips
  • Uncategorized
  • Video Learning Series
  • What’s Coming Up
  • Workforce Intelligence

Dana-Farber Cancer Institute

Research Types Explained: Basic, Clinical, Translational

“Research” is a broad stroke of a word, the verbal equivalent of painting a wall instead of a masterpiece. There are important distinctions among the three principal types of medical research — basic, clinical and translational.

Whereas basic research is looking at questions related to how nature works, translational research aims to take what’s learned in basic research and apply that in the development of solutions to medical problems. Clinical research, then, takes those solutions and studies them in clinical trials. Together, they form a continuous research loop that transforms ideas into action in the form of new treatments and tests, and advances cutting-edge developments from the lab bench to the patient’s bedside and back again.

Basic Research

When it comes to science, the “basic” in basic research describes something that’s an essential starting point. “If you think of it in terms of construction, you can’t put up a beautiful, elegant house without first putting in a foundation,” says David Frank, MD , Associate Professor of Medicine, Medical Oncology, at Dana-Farber Cancer Institute. “In science, if you don’t first understand the basic research, then you can’t move on to advanced applications.”

David Frank, MD.

Basic medical research is usually conducted by scientists with a PhD in such fields as biology and chemistry, among many others. They study the core building blocks of life — DNA, cells, proteins, molecules, etc. — to answer fundamental questions about their structures and how they work.

For example, oncologists now know that mutations in DNA enable the unchecked growth of cells in cancer. A scientist conducting basic research might ask: How does DNA work in a healthy cell? How do mutations occur? Where along the DNA sequence do mutations happen? And why?

“Basic research is fundamentally curiosity-driven research,” says Milka Kostic, Program Director, Chemical Biology at Dana-Farber Cancer Institute. “Think of that moment when an apple fell on Isaac Newton’s head. He thought to himself, ‘Why did that happen?’ and then went on to try to find the answer. That’s basic research.”

Dan Stover, MD, and Heather Parsons, MD, conduct basic research in metastatic breast cancer.

Clinical Research

Clinical research explores whether new treatments, medications and diagnostic techniques are safe and effective in patients. Physicians administer these to patients in rigorously controlled clinical trials, so that they can accurately and precisely monitor patients’ progress and evaluate the treatment’s efficacy, or measurable benefit.

“In clinical research, we’re trying to define the best treatment for a patient with a given condition,” Frank says. “We’re asking such questions as: Will this new treatment extend the life of a patient with a given type of cancer? Could this supportive medication diminish nausea, diarrhea or other side effects? Could this diagnostic test help physicians detect cancer earlier or distinguish between fast- and slow-growing cancers?”

Successful clinical researchers must draw on not only their medical training but also their knowledge of such areas as statistics, controls and regulatory compliance.

Translational Research

It’s neither practical nor safe to transition directly from studying individual cells to testing on patients. Translational research provides that crucial pivot point. It bridges the gap between basic and clinical research by bringing together a number of specialists to refine and advance the application of a discovery. “Biomedical science is so complex, and there’s so much knowledge available.” Frank says. “It’s through collaboration that advances are made.”

For example, let’s say a basic researcher has identified a gene that looks like a promising candidate for targeted therapy. Translational researchers would then evaluate thousands, if not millions, of potential compounds for the ideal combination that could be developed into a medicine to achieve the desired effect. They’d refine and test the compound on intermediate models, in laboratory and animal models. Then they would analyze those test results to determine proper dosage, side effects and other safety considerations before moving to first-in-human clinical trials. It’s the complex interplay of chemistry, biology, oncology, biostatistics, genomics, pharmacology and other specialties that makes such a translational study a success.

Collaboration and technology have been the twin drivers of recent quantum leaps in the quality and quantity of translational research. “Now, using modern molecular techniques,” Frank says, “we can learn so much from a tissue sample from a patient that we couldn’t before.”

Translational research provides a crucial pivot point after clinical trials as well. Investigators explore how the trial’s resulting treatment or guidelines can be implemented by physicians in their practice. And the clinical outcomes might also motivate basic researchers to reevaluate their original assumptions.

“Translational research is a two-way street,” Kostic says. “There is always conversation flowing in both directions. It’s a loop, a continuous cycle, with one research result inspiring another.”

Learn more about research at Dana-Farber .

Research-Methodology

Fundamental Research

Fundamental research , also known as  basic research  or  pure research  does not usually generate findings that have immediate applications in a practical level. Fundamental research is driven by curiosity and the desire to expand knowledge in specific research area. This type of research makes a specific contribution to the academic body of knowledge in the research area.

Fundamental studies tend to make generalizations about the phenomenon, and the philosophy of this type of studies can be explained as ‘gathering knowledge for the sake of knowledge’. Fundamental researches mainly aim to answer the questions of why, what or how and they tend to contribute the pool of fundamental knowledge in the research area .

Opposite to fundamental research is  applied research  that aims to solve specific problems, thus findings of applied research do have immediate practical implications.

Differences between Fundamental and Applied Research

Differences between applied and fundamental research have been specified in a way that fundamental research studies individual cases without generalizing, and recognizes that other variables are in constant change.

Applied research, on the contrary, seeks generalizations and assumes that other variables do not change. The table below summarizes the differences between the two types of research in terms of purpose and context:

Differences between fundamental and applied research [1]

It is important to note that although fundamental studies do not pursue immediate commercial objectives, nevertheless, findings of fundamental studies may result in innovations, as well as, generating solutions to practical problems. For example, a study entitled “A critical assessment of the role of organizational culture in facilitating management-employee communications” is a fundamental study, but findings of this study may be used to increase the levels of effectiveness of management-employee communications, thus resulting in practical implications.

Examples of Fundamental Research

The following are examples for fundamental researches in business:

  • A critical analysis of product placement as an effective marketing strategy
  • An investigation into the main elements of brands and branding
  • A study of factors impacting each stage of product life cycle

Advantages and Disadvantages of Fundamental Research

Advantages of fundamental research are considered as disadvantages of applied research and vice versa. Fundamental researches are important to expand the pool of knowledge in any discipline. Findings of fundamental studies are usually applicable in a wide range of cases and scenarios. Fundamental studies usually do not have strict deadlines and they are usually driven by the curiosity of the researcher.

At the same time, fundamental studies have disadvantages as well. Findings of this type of studies have little or no practical implications. In other words, fundamental studies do not resolve concrete and specific business problems.

My e-book,  The Ultimate Guide to Writing a Dissertation in Business Studies: a step by step assistance   contains discussions of research types and application of research methods in practice. The e-book also explains all stages of the  research process  starting from the  selection of the research area  to writing personal reflection. Important elements of dissertations such as  research philosophy ,  research approach ,  research design ,  methods of data collection  and  data analysis , sampling and others are explained in this e-book in simple words.

John Dudovskiy

Fundamental research

[1] Table adapted from Saunders, M., Lewis, P. & Thornhill, A. (2012) “Research Methods for Business Students” 6 th  edition, Pearson Education Limited

National Academies Press: OpenBook

Assessment of Department of Defense Basic Research (2005)

Chapter: appendix d definitions of basic, applied, and fundamental research, appendix d definitions of basic, applied, and fundamental research.

This appendix contains definitions of basic, applied, and fundamental research quoted from various sources.

BASIC RESEARCH

DOD Financial Management Regulation, DOD 7000.14-R, Vol. 2B, Ch. 5: Basic research is systematic study directed toward greater knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind. It includes all scientific study and experimentation directed toward increasing fundamental knowledge and understanding in those fields of the physical, engineering, environmental, and life sciences related to long-term national security needs. It is farsighted high payoff research that provides the basis for technological progress. Basic research may lead to: (a) subsequent applied research and advanced technology developments in Defense-related technologies, and (b) new and improved military functional capabilities in areas such as communications, detection, tracking, surveillance, propulsion, mobility, guidance and control, navigation, energy conversion, materials and structures, and personnel support. Program elements in this category involve pre-Milestone A efforts. Available online at http://www.dod.mil/comptroller/fmr/02b/Chapter05.pdf . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

The objective of basic research is to gain more comprehensive knowledge or understanding of the subject under study, without specific applications in mind. In industry, basic research is defined as research that advances scientific knowledge but does not have specific immediate commercial objectives, although it

may be in fields of present or potential commercial interest. [National Science Foundation, Directorate for Social, Behavioral & Economic Sciences, US definitions for resource surveys, 1996.] Available online at http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/srs/seind96/ch4_defn.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Scientific efforts that seek to gain more comprehensive knowledge or understanding of the subject under study, without specific applications or commercial objectives in mind. Available online at http://energytrends.pnl.gov/glosn_z.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Basic research analyzes properties, structures, and relationships toward formulating and testing hypotheses, theories, or laws. As used in this survey, industrial basic research is the pursuit of new scientific knowledge or understanding that does not have specific immediate commercial objectives, although it may be in fields of present or potential commercial interest. Available online at http://caspar.nsf.gov/nsf/srs/IndRD/glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

The investigation of the natural phenomena as contrasted with applied research. Available online at http://www.onlineethics.org/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Systematic study directed toward greater knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind. [OMB Circular A-11, June 1996.] See Conduct of Research and Development. Available online at https://radius.rand.org/radius/demo/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Fundamental scientific inquiry to understand the unknown and contribute to improved general knowledge (cf. with applied research). Available online at http://www.ipmrc.com/lib/glossary.shtml . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research done to further knowledge for knowledge’s sake. Available online at http://www.modernhumanorigins.com/b.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Fundamental research; it often produces a wide range of applications, but the output of basic research itself usually is not of direct commercial value. The output is knowledge, rather than a product; it typically cannot be patented. Available online at http://www.wwnorton.com/stiglitzwalsh/economics/glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research aimed at expanding knowledge rather than solving a specific, pragmatic problem. Available online at https://www.quirks.com/resources/glossary.asp . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Focused, systematic study and investigation undertaken to discover new knowledge or interpretations and establish facts or principles in a particular field. See Research. Available online at http://www.siu.edu/orda/general/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Fundamental research; it often produces a wide range of applications, but the output of basic research itself usually is not of direct commercial value; the output is knowledge, rather than a product; the output of basic research typically cannot be patented. Available online at http://wellspring.isinj.com/sample/econ/micro/glossb.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research emphasizing the solution of theoretical problems. Binomial probability distribution: The probabilities associated with every possible outcome of an experiment involving n independent trials and a success or failure on each trial. Bivariate analysis: The analysis of relationships among pairs of variables. Available online at http://www.prm.nau.edu/prm447/definitions.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Basic research is research undertaken to advance the knowledge of methodologies and techniques of research. (Compare applied research.) Available online at http://www.rigneyassoc.com/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research that is directed at the growth of scientific knowledge, without any near-term expectations of commercial applications. Available online at http://highered.mcgraw-hill.com/sites/0072443901/student_view0/chapter4/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research which adds something new to the body of knowledge of a particular field. Available online at http://researchoffice.astate.edu/glossary_of_proposal_terms.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Designed to test and refine theory. The purpose is to increase our knowledge about communication phenomena by testing, refining, and elaborating theory. Available online at http://www.uky.edu/~drlane/cohort/define.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

The purpose is to increase knowledge without concern for practical application. Available online at http://www.ied.edu.hk/csnsie/ar/chap1/1_glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

In basic research the objective of the sponsoring agency is to gain more complete knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts, without specific applications toward processes or products in mind. Available online at http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/srs/fedfunds/glossary/def.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

NSF Definition of Basic Research: Basic research is defined as systematic study directed toward fuller knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind. (In Bill Berry’s presentation, Meeting 1, Committee on Department of Defense Basic Research.)

In basic research the objective of the sponsoring agency is to gain fuller knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications toward processes or products in mind. Available online at http://www.aaas.org/spp/cstc/pne/pubs/regrep/alaska/appendices.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

OMB (Circular A-11, 2003): Basic research is defined as systematic study directed toward fuller knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind.

Research directed toward the increase of knowledge, the primary aim being a greater knowledge or understanding of the subject under study. Available online at http://usmilitary.about.com/library/glossary/b/bldef00823.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Commission of the European Communities: While there is no strict, unanimously accepted definition of what constitutes basic research, in practice one can identify and distinguish from other types of research, those which are carried out with no direct link to a given application and, if not exclusively, in any case and above all with the objective of progressing knowledge. Available online at http://europa.eu.int/comm/research/press/2004/pdf/acte_en_version_final_15janv_04.pdf . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

APPLIED RESEARCH

OMB (Circular A-11, 2003): Applied research is defined as systematic study to gain knowledge or understanding necessary to determine the means by which a recognized and specific need may be met.

The investigation of some phenomena to discover whether its properties are appropriate to a particular need or want. In contrast, basic research investigates phenomena without reference to particular human needs and wants. Available online at http://www.onlineethics.org/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Aimed at gaining knowledge or understanding to determine the means by which a specific, recognized need may be met. In industry, applied research includes investigations oriented to discovering new scientific knowledge that has specific commercial objectives with respect to products, processes, or services. [National Science Foundation, Directorate for Social, Behavioral & Economic Sciences, US definitions for resource surveys, 1996.] Available online at http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/srs/seind96/ch4_defn.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Applied research is undertaken either to determine possible uses for the findings of basic research or to determine new ways of achieving some specific, predetermined objectives. As used in this survey, industrial applied research is investigation that may use findings of basic research toward discovering new scientific knowledge that has specific commercial objectives with respect to new products, services, processes, or methods. Available online at http://www.caspar.nsf.gov/nsf/srs/IndRD/glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Inquiry aimed at gaining the knowledge or understanding to meet a specific, recognized need of a practical nature, especially needs to achieve specific commercial objectives with respect to products, processes, or services. Available online at http://energytrends.pnl.gov/glosn_z.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Any research which is used to answer a specific question, determine why something failed or succeeded, solve a specific, pragmatic problem, or to gain better understanding. Available online at https://www.quirks.com/resources/glossary.asp . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Focused, systematic study and investigation undertaken to discover the applications and uses of theories, knowledge, and principles in actual work or in solving problems. See Research. Available online at http://www.siu.edu/orda/general/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Systematic study to gain knowledge or understanding necessary to determine the means by which a recognized and specific need may be met. [OMB Circular A-11, June 1996.] See also Conduct of Research and Development. Available online at https://radius.rand.org/radius/demo/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

The investigation of some phenomena to discover whether its properties are appropriate to a particular need or want. In contrast, basic research investigates phenomena without reference to particular human needs and wants. Available online at http://www.unmc.edu/ethics/words.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Is designed to solve practical problems of the modern world, rather than to acquire knowledge for knowledge’s sake. Available online at http://ventureline.com/glossary_A.asp . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research designed for the purpose of producing results that may be applied to real world situations. Topic areas: Accountability and Evaluation. Available online at http://www.nonprofitbasics.org/SearchEntireSite.aspx?Source=2&SiteSearchText=research&PW=No&PreviousWord=research&C0=178&C4=1&C3=4&C5=36&C6=18&C1=92&C2=1 . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research that studies the relationship or applicability for theories or principles of a particular field to a particular problem. Available online at http://researchoffice.astate.edu/glossary_of_proposal_terms.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research done with the intent of applying results to a specific problem. Evaluation is a form of applied research. This can be conducted as part of an action research approach. Available online at http://www.sachru.sa.gov.au/pew/glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Research aimed at improving the quality of life and solving practical problems. Available online at http://highered.mcgraw-hill.com/sites/0072358327/student_view0/chapter1/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

The aim is to address an immediate problem. The purpose is to try ideas in the context of educational (classroom) settings. Available online at http://www.ied.edu.hk/csnsie/ar/chap1/1_glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Applied research is that effort that (1) normally follows basic research, but may not be severable from the related basic research, (2) attempts to determine and exploit the potential of scientific discoveries or improvements in technology, materials, processes, methods, devices, or techniques, and (3) attempts to advance the state of the art. Applied research does not include efforts whose principal aim is design, development, or test of specific items or services to be considered for sale; these efforts are within the definition of the term development. Available online at http://www-agecon.ag.ohio-state.edu/class/AEDE601/glossary/glossa.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Conducted to solve particular problems or answer specific questions. Available online at http://www.nelson.com/nelson/hmcanada/ob/glossary.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

In applied research the objective of the sponsoring agency is to gain knowledge or understanding necessary to determine how a recognized need may be met. Available online at http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/srs/sfsucni/method99/help/glossary.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Utilizing pure research to develop real-world products. Available online at http://www.investorwords.com/236/applied_research.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

As opposed to basic research, applied research is the type of research which is conducted to solve practical problems, find cures to illnesses, develop therapies with the purpose of helping people, and other similar types of practical problem-solving research. Available online at http://www.alleydog.com/glossary/definition.cfm?term=Applied%20Research . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

FUNDAMENTAL RESEARCH

National Security Decision Directives: “Fundamental research” means basic and applied research in science and engineering, the results of which ordinarily are published and shared broadly within the scientific community, as distinguished from proprietary research and from industrial development, design, production, and product utilization, the results of which ordinarily are restricted for proprietary or national security reasons. Available online at http://www.fas.org/irp/offdocs/nsdd/nsdd-189.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Fundamental research is basic and applied research in science and engineering where the resulting information is ordinarily published and shared broadly within the scientific community. It is distinguished from proprietary research and from industrial development, design, production, and product utilizations, the results of which ordinarily are restricted for proprietary and/or specific national secu-

rity reasons. Normally, the results of “fundamental research.” are published in scientific literature, thus making it publicly available. Research which is intended for publication, whether it is ever accepted by scientific journals or not, is considered to be “fundamental research.” A large segment of academic research is considered “fundamental research”. Because any information, technological or otherwise, that is publicly available is not subject to the Export Administration Regulations (EAR) (except for encryption object code and source code in electronic form or media) and thus does not require a license, ‘fundamental research’ is not subject to the EAR and does not require a license. Available online at http://www.umbi.umd.edu/rcc/fundamentalresearch.pdf . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

National Security Decision Directive 189: Fundamental Research defined: basic and applied research in science and engineering, the results of which are available to interested scientific community. National Policy: No restriction may be placed upon the conduct or reporting of federally funded Fundamental Research that has not received national security classification. This is reflected in ITAR at 22 CFRR 120.11(8). Executive Order 12356 (1985). Available online at http://www.epic.org/open_gov/eo_12356.html . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Federal Acquisition Regulation 27.404 (g) (2): In contracts for basic or applied research universities or colleges, no restrictions may be placed upon the conduct of or reporting on the results of unclassified basic or applied research, except as provided in applicable U.S. Statutes. Available online at http://supply.lanl.gov/Property/ecco/History/2004/presentations2004/default.shtml . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

DoD Supplement to the FAR: It is DOD policy … to allow the publication and public presentation of unclassified contracted fundamental research results. The mechanism for control of information generated by DOT funded contracted fundamental research … is security classification. Available online at http://supply.lanl.gov/Property/ecco/History/2004/presentations2004/default.shtml . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

Defense of Basic Research by Joseph Henry: In 1852, Henry defended basic research. It was “profitable,” he said, when that word was defined properly. “The true, the beautiful, as well as the immediately practical, are all entitled to a share of attention. All knowledge is profitable; profitable in its ennobling effect on the character, in the pleasure it imparts in its acquisition, as well as in the power it gives over the operations of mind and matter. All knowledge is useful; every part of this complex system of nature is connected with every other. Nothing is isolated. The discovery of to-day, which appears unconnected with any useful process, may, in the course of a few years, become the fruitful source of a thousand inventions.” Available online at http://www.si.edu/archives/ihd/jhp/joseph04.htm . Last accessed on November 16, 2004.

The Department of Defense (DOD) supports basic research to advance fundamental knowledge in fields important to national defense. Over the past six years, however, several groups have raised concern about whether the nature of DOD-funded basic research is changing. The concerns include these: Funds are being spent for research that does not fall under DOD's definition of basic research; reporting requirements have become cumbersome and onerous; and basic research is handled differently by the three services. To explore these concerns, the Congress directed DOD to request a study from the National Research Council (NRC) about the nature of basic research now being funded by the Department. Specifically the NRC was to determine if the programs in the DOD basic research portfolio are consistent with the DOD definition of basic research and with the characteristics associated with fundamental research.

READ FREE ONLINE

Welcome to OpenBook!

You're looking at OpenBook, NAP.edu's online reading room since 1999. Based on feedback from you, our users, we've made some improvements that make it easier than ever to read thousands of publications on our website.

Do you want to take a quick tour of the OpenBook's features?

Show this book's table of contents , where you can jump to any chapter by name.

...or use these buttons to go back to the previous chapter or skip to the next one.

Jump up to the previous page or down to the next one. Also, you can type in a page number and press Enter to go directly to that page in the book.

Switch between the Original Pages , where you can read the report as it appeared in print, and Text Pages for the web version, where you can highlight and search the text.

To search the entire text of this book, type in your search term here and press Enter .

Share a link to this book page on your preferred social network or via email.

View our suggested citation for this chapter.

Ready to take your reading offline? Click here to buy this book in print or download it as a free PDF, if available.

Get Email Updates

Do you enjoy reading reports from the Academies online for free ? Sign up for email notifications and we'll let you know about new publications in your areas of interest when they're released.

Basic Research: Definition, Examples

what is basic research?

Basic research focuses on the search for truth or the development of theory. Because of this property, basic research is fundamental. Researchers with their fundamental background knowledge “design studies that can test, refine, modify, or develop theories.”

Meaning and Definition of Basic Research

Generally, these researchers are affiliated with an academic institution and perform this research as part of their graduate or doctoral studies. Gathering knowledge for knowledge’s sake is the sole purpose of basic research .

Basic research is also called pure research. Basic research is driven by a scientist’s curiosity or interest in a scientific question.

The main motivation in basic research is to expand man’s knowledge, not to create or invent something. There is no obvious commercial value to the discoveries that result from basic research.

The term ‘basic’ indicates that, through theory generation, basic research provides the foundation for applied research. This research approach is essential for nourishing the expansion of knowledge.

It deals with questions that are intellectually interesting and challenging to the investigator. It focuses on refuting or supporting theories that operate in a changing society.

Basic research generates new ideas, principles, and theories, which may not be of immediate practical utility, though such research lays the foundations of modern progress and development in many fields.

Basic research rarely helps practitioners directly with everyday concerns but can stimulate new ways of thinking about our daily lives.

Basic researchers are more detached and academic in their approach and tend to have motives.

For example, an anthropologist may research to try and understand the physical properties, symbolic meanings, and practical qualities of things.

Such research contributes to understanding broad issues of interest to many social sciences-issues of self, family, and material culture.

Having said so, we come up with the following definition of basic research:

When the solution to the research problem has no apparent applications to any existing practical problem but serves only the scholarly interests of a community of a researcher, the research is basic.

Most scientists believe that a fundamental understanding of all branches of science is needed for progress to take place.

In other words, basic research lays the foundation for the following applied research. If basic work is done first, then this research often results from applied spin-offs.

A person wishing to do basic research in any specialized area must have studied the concepts and assumptions of that specialization enough to know what has been done in the past and what remains to be done.

For example, basic research is necessary for the health sector to generate new knowledge and technology to deal with major unsolved health problems.

Here are a few examples of questions asked in pure research:

  • How did the universe begin?
  • What are protons, neutrons, and electrons composed of?
  • How do slime molds reproduce?
  • How do the Neo-Malthusians view the Malthusian theory?
  • What is the specific genetic code of the fruit fly?
  • What is the relevance of the dividend theories in the capital market?

As there is no guarantee of short-term practical gain, researchers find it difficult to obtain funding for basic research.

Examples of Basic Research

The author investigated the smoothness of the solution of the degenerate Hamilton-Bellman (HJB) equation associated with a linear-quadratic regulator control.

The author established the existence of a classical solution of the degenerate HJB equation associated with this problem by the technique of viscosity solutions and hence derived an optimal control from the optimality conditions in the HJB equation.

Hasan (2009) gave a solution to linear programming problems through computer algebra. He developed a computer technique for solving such linear fractional programming problems in his paper.

At the outset, he determined all basic feasible solutions to the constraints, which are a system of linear equations.

The author then computed and compared the objective function values and obtained the optimal objective function value and optimal solutions. The method was then illustrated with a few numerical examples.

What is the primary focus of basic research?

Basic research primarily focuses on the search for truth or the development of theory. It is fundamental in nature and aims to design studies that test, refine, modify, or develop theories.

How does basic research differ from applied research in terms of its purpose?

The sole purpose of basic research is to gather knowledge for knowledge’s sake. It is driven by a scientist’s curiosity or interest in a scientific question without any immediate commercial value to the discoveries, whereas applied research has practical applications.

What is the significance of the term “basic” in basic research?

The term “basic” indicates that the research provides the foundation for applied research through theory generation. It lays the groundwork for modern progress and development in various fields.

Why might researchers face challenges in obtaining funding for basic research?

Since there is no guarantee of short-term practical gain from basic research, researchers often find it difficult to secure funding for such endeavors.

30 Accounting Research Paper Topics and Ideas for Writing

what is basic research study

What is basic research?

What is basic research, and is it important a new theme on sciencenordic..

This article is part of our Basic Research theme

Basic research is often described as research without any clear goals, or science to satisfy the curiosity of a lone scientist without anyone else even realising that it exists.

But this totally misses the mark, says Søren-Peter Olesen, the director of the Danish National Research Foundation Centre for Cardiac Arrhythmia at the University of Copenhagen.

The goal of basic research is “to collect information about how nature and people are put together. It’s not important that this knowledge can be used for anything concrete, the most important thing is that we improve our understanding,” he says.

   Basic Research

ScienceNordic takes you into the engine room of basic research to find out:

  • What is it?
  • Who does it?
  • And how does it benefit us?

Read More: What is basic research?

In the coming weeks, ScienceNordic is running a special theme on basic research. We will try to cover questions like: what is the point of basic research, who pays for it, who does it, and how does it benefit society?

You can bookmark the basic research theme homepage to stay up to date with the new articles on basic research here at ScienceNordic.

Basic research expands our knowledge and leads to innovation

Basic research or “blue skies research” is conducted just as any other scientific research: scientists have a hypothesis and test it by designing experiments and making observations to develop theories that explain how the world around us works.

“It’s important for humanity to understand the world in which we live,” says Olesen.

Basic research seeks answers to fundamental questions and provides broad insights to many different scientific fields. Applied research, on the other hand, tends to have a much narrower focus within a specific field.

“Basic research is incredibly important because it lays the ground for major discoveries,” says Poul Nissen, a centre director at the Centre for Membrane Pumps in Cells and Disease (PUMPkin) at Aarhus University, Denmark.

“When we use social media and electronic gadgets, and when we develop medicine to treat diseases--this is all possible because someone was doing basic research on it twenty or forty years earlier,” he says.

“Today’s discoveries are built on investments made in previous decades to fund basic research into the unknown challenges of the future,” says Nissen.

Blurred boundaries between applied and basic researching

Public debate usually tries to draw clear distinctions between basic research and applied research, so that they appear as almost polar opposites.

Applied research appears to have specific goals: a vaccine, a new windmill, or a new battery—concrete inventions that can improve our daily lives and lead to new products in the marketplace.

It may seem obvious that scientists should start with a strategic goal for a results-orientated project that can benefit society. But the reality is very different. The two types of research are closely intertwined and in reality it’s difficult to draw a strict boundary between them.

Take Nissen’s research, for example.

His research centre studies how so-called ion-pumps work in animal and plant cells. This may at first sound like a rather narrow research field, but over the past nine years, their research has led to developments in the treatment of fungal infections following cases of pneumonia, cancer treatments, as well as advances in the understanding of migraine, muscle diseases, and the important sodium-potassium pump mechanism in cells.

Basic research is also applied research

Nissen’s results have led to several spin-off companies and other results-orientated projects.

“All good basic research ends with new perspectives that can also have practical applications,” says Nissen. “All basic research projects lead to further projects. So it has a big effect on people’s daily lives.”

In that sense, basic research is no different to applied research, he says. “It just has a longer term perspective.”

Basic research is not defined by what scientists study, but how they do it

So if a scientist discovers a new mechanism that can potentially fight malaria, or if a scientists find a correlation between certain genes and our birth weight or certain diseases, is this basic or applied research?

But that is the wrong way to think about it, according to Olesen. It is not what you study, but how you study it, that defines whether or not your research is considered basic or applied, he says.

“Basic research is not defined by the expedition in itself, but by the goals you have,” says Olesen.

In other words, if a scientist studies malaria to improve our understanding of the disease, then that is basic research. But when that understanding seeks out new treatments, it becomes applied.

Can you conduct basic research in any subject?

So are any topics off limits when it comes to basic research? What about football, for example?

“That would be great!” says Olesen.

“Football is something that you can approach from many different angles: psychology, sports medicine, and by studying game theory and other mathematical techniques. So it can be extremely beneficial,” he says.

Olesen studies heart medicine and lectures on extreme sport and the changes that occur in the body when we take part in sport.

“There’s a lot to do, and often the breakthroughs come from research fields that are a bit off the beaten track, like football.”

Amateur scientists can also do basic research

So if there are no limits for what can be studied, are there any limits for who can do it or can anyone get involved in basic research?

“A lot of research is done outside universities,” says Olesen. “In industry, of course, but also by amateurs--amateur archaeologists for example.”

Nissen agrees.

“Research is driven by many things. Partly technical developments that make new discoveries possible and partly a need to recognise something. Sometimes it’s just luck when you come across something unexpected. Even amateurs can discover things that no one else had thought of before,” he says.

-------------

Read the Danish version of this article on Videnskab.dk  

Translated by: Catherine Jex

External links

  • Søren-Peter Olesen
  • Poul Nissen

Related content

A boost for open access to research.

The Research Council of Norway is introducing a new funding scheme to promote publication in open access journals.

what is basic research study

Arctic research on the rise

With an increased interest in the natural resources of the Arctic, geologists are increasingly drawn to research on the archipelago of Svalbard.

what is basic research study

Excessive funding for popular research creates science bubble

Research grants are increasingly being awarded to the same few popular research fields. This results in homogenised projects that rarely deliver what they promise. The phenomenon is similar to real estate bubbles, argue two Danish philosophers.

Voicing concern that industry funding distorts academic research

Director of bioethics program urges researchers to look at how money from the industry influences their work.

what is basic research study

Funding to defend industry, or defend Norway?

New EU regulations can force the Norwegian government to make a choice. Will it subsidise the defence industry to promote national security, or is economic stimulus the major aim? A researcher says we should go for the first option.

what is basic research study

Researchers warn against pills that reduce stomach acid: Many become addicted and are unable to stop

what is basic research study

Do you often get low blood sugar? No, you probably don't

what is basic research study

3,000 Viking swords have been found in Norway

what is basic research study

These animals are drained of blood in order to save human lives

what is basic research study

The silent revolution: Is the HIV epidemic over in Norway?

what is basic research study

Antidepressants don´t make wildlife happier: how the pills you take impact nature

Drug waste in the ocean affects behavior and reproduction of both fish and other organisms. But what is happening under the sea surface?

what is basic research study

Did you know energy possibly speaks?

Understanding the language of energy flows can help us prevent energy blackouts due to climate change and cyber attacks

Understanding basic research

Last updated

8 February 2023

Reviewed by

Cathy Heath

Research is an essential activity for all of us. It's how we foster curiosity, gather information, learn about the world, and find solutions. There are many different types of research, but they all fall into one of two categories: basic and applied.

This article will look at basic research, what it is, how we use it, and how it compares to applied research.

Analyze your basic research

Dovetail streamlines analysis to help you uncover and share actionable insights

  • What is basic research?

Basic research is all about information-gathering and answering What, Why, and How research questions. It involves learning the facts about a subject, finding out why things happen, perhaps by investigation or observation, and then using this expanded knowledge to better understand the topic.

Basic research is also called fundamental, foundational, or pure research.

  • Where is basic research used?
  • What are examples of basic research?

Studying how a client moves through the sales funnel process

Examining the components of a cell

Analyzing performance before and after coffee consumption

Looking at how stress affects productivity

Determining which areas of a country are driest and wettest

Understanding how a doctor makes a mental health diagnosis

  • What are basic research methods?

You can conduct basic research using several different methods. The best method will depend on what you are studying and what questions you are trying to answer.

Some methods available for basic research include:

Experimentation

Conducting a survey or interview

Observation

  • What is the value of basic research?

Basic research teaches us about the world around us. It helps us gather more primary data about a subject, which we can use later in applied research. Without that basic information on a subject, we wouldn't have the data we need to make informed decisions.

That's why basic research is often called  foundational research . The information we acquire through basic research gives us a foundation of knowledge we can build on in the future.

  • Is basic research qualitative or quantitative?

Basic research can be qualitative, quantitative, or both.

Quantitative data deals with  quantities  or numbers. You could chart this data on a graph, using measurements such as:

Other numerical data

Qualitative data deals with  qualities .  It focuses more on the language and sentiment found in:

Interview responses

Observations about human behavior

Verbal answers to surveys

You can get qualitative and quantitative data from basic research, depending on what you are researching and the methods you use.

  • Basic research vs. applied research

Where basic research gathers information and data on a subject, applied research uses that data to look for answers to questions. Applied research takes the data obtained in basic research and applies it to answer a question and provide a possible solution.

There are three types of applied research:

Evaluation research: determining how well something is working or what it’s worth

Action research: understanding how to improve a process, for example in education or business

Research and development: looking at new products and services that businesses can offer consumers to solve a problem

Applied research often hinges on data collected during basic research. For example, you might gather data on how customers move through the sales funnel. The information you learn about the customer journey comes from your basic research, which you might gather through website analytics and customer interviews.

Now you want to know the best method for bringing more people into the sales funnel. You could use applied research to determine if it's better to increase your digital ad spending, send more emails, or use telemarketing to capture more sales leads.

Where is applied research used?

As applied research is solution-based, agencies concerned with medical research, psychology, and education all use it to improve lives. For businesses, applied research is the foundation of research and development (R&D) departments that are looking to create new consumer products and services.

You can also use applied research in your everyday life. You might use basic research to gather data on how caffeine affects your sleeping patterns, then use applied research techniques to determine how to get a better night's sleep.

Examples of applied research

Examples of applied research include:

Identifying new products to help aging seniors stay safe at home

Looking at ways to treat a medical condition safely

Determining how to make products last longer 

Examining the best ways to prevent Type 2 diabetes

Understanding how to engage teens in math classes

How to market a product to Gen Z

Understanding how businesses could help reduce their impact on climate change

What is the value of applied research?

Applied research's value lies in its ability to help us solve problems with data-backed solutions. However, without comprehensive basic research at the outset, we wouldn't have the information we need to find those solutions. This illustrates why basic research and applied research often work hand in hand, supporting research efforts and validating better outcomes.

Should you be using a customer insights hub?

Do you want to discover previous research faster?

Do you share your research findings with others?

Do you analyze research data?

Start for free today, add your research, and get to key insights faster

Editor’s picks

Last updated: 11 January 2024

Last updated: 15 January 2024

Last updated: 17 January 2024

Last updated: 25 November 2023

Last updated: 12 May 2023

Last updated: 30 April 2024

Last updated: 13 May 2024

Latest articles

Related topics, .css-je19u9{-webkit-align-items:flex-end;-webkit-box-align:flex-end;-ms-flex-align:flex-end;align-items:flex-end;display:-webkit-box;display:-webkit-flex;display:-ms-flexbox;display:flex;-webkit-flex-direction:row;-ms-flex-direction:row;flex-direction:row;-webkit-box-flex-wrap:wrap;-webkit-flex-wrap:wrap;-ms-flex-wrap:wrap;flex-wrap:wrap;-webkit-box-pack:center;-ms-flex-pack:center;-webkit-justify-content:center;justify-content:center;row-gap:0;text-align:center;max-width:671px;}@media (max-width: 1079px){.css-je19u9{max-width:400px;}.css-je19u9>span{white-space:pre;}}@media (max-width: 799px){.css-je19u9{max-width:400px;}.css-je19u9>span{white-space:pre;}} decide what to .css-1kiodld{max-height:56px;display:-webkit-box;display:-webkit-flex;display:-ms-flexbox;display:flex;-webkit-align-items:center;-webkit-box-align:center;-ms-flex-align:center;align-items:center;}@media (max-width: 1079px){.css-1kiodld{display:none;}} build next, decide what to build next.

what is basic research study

Users report unexpectedly high data usage, especially during streaming sessions.

what is basic research study

Users find it hard to navigate from the home page to relevant playlists in the app.

what is basic research study

It would be great to have a sleep timer feature, especially for bedtime listening.

what is basic research study

I need better filters to find the songs or artists I’m looking for.

Log in or sign up

Get started for free

  • What is Pure or Basic Research? + [Examples & Method]

busayo.longe

Sometimes, research may be aimed at expanding a field of knowledge or improving the understanding of a natural phenomenon. This type of research is known as a basic, pure or fundamental research, and it is a major means of generating new ideas, principles and theories. 

In many cases, basic research fuels scientific innovations and development because it is driven by the need to unravel the unknown. In this article, we will define what basic research is, its data collection methods and how it differs from other approaches to research. 

What is Basic Research?

Basic research is a type of research approach that is aimed at gaining a better understanding of a subject, phenomenon or basic law of nature. This type of research is primarily focused on the advancement of knowledge rather than solving a specific problem. 

Basic research is also referred to as pure research or fundamental research. The concept of basic research emerged between the late 19th century and early 20th century in an attempt to bridge the gaps existing in the societal utility of science. 

Typically, basic research can be exploratory , descriptive or explanatory; although in many cases, it is explanatory in nature. The primary aim of this research approach is to gather information in order to improve one’s understanding, and this information can then be useful in proffering solutions to a problem. 

Examples of Basic Research 

Basic research can be carried out in different fields with the primary aim of expanding the frontier of knowledge and developing the scope of these fields of study. Examples of basic research can be seen in medicine, education, psychology, technology, to mention but a few.

Basic Research Example in Education  

In education, basic research is used to develop pedagogical theories that explain teaching and learning behaviours in the classroom. Examples of basic research in education include the following:

  • How does the Language Acquisition Device work  on children?
  • How does the human retentive memory work?
  • How do teaching methods affect student’s concentration in class?

Basic Research Example in Science

Basic research advances scientific knowledge by helping researchers understand the function of newly discovered molecules and cells, strange phenomena, or little-understood processes. As with other fields, basic research is responsible for many scientific breakthroughs; even though the knowledge gained may not seem to yield immediate benefits. 

Examples of basic research in science include: 

  • A research to determine the chemical composition of organic molecules.
  • A research to discover the components of the human DNA.

Basic Research Example in Psychology  

In psychology, basic research helps individuals and organisations to gain insights and better understanding into different conditions. It is entirely theoretical and allows psychologists to understand certain behaviors better without providing  solutions to these behaviours or phenomena.  

Examples of basic research in psychology include: 

  • Do stress levels make individuals more aggressive?
  • To what extent does caffeine consumption affect classroom concentration?
  • A research on behavioral differences between children raised by separated families and children raised by married parents.
  • To what extent do gender stereotypes  trigger depression?

Basic Research Example in Health   

Basic research methods improve healthcare by providing different dimensions to the understanding and interpretation of healthcare issues. For example, it allows healthcare practitioners to gain more insight into the origin of diseases which can help to provide cures to chronic medical conditions. 

Many health researchers opine that many vaccines are developed based on an understanding of the causes of the disease such as in the case of the polio vaccine. Several medical breakthroughs have been attributed to the wealth of knowledge provided through basic research. 

Examples of basic research in health include: 

  • An investigation into the symptoms of Coronavirus.
  • An investigation into the causative factors of malaria
  • An investigation into the secondary symptoms of high blood pressure.

Basic Research Method

 An interview is a common method of data collection in basic research that involves having a one-on-one interaction with an individual in order to gather relevant information about a phenomenon. Interview can be structured, unstructured or semi-structured depending on the research process and objectives.  

In a structured interview , the researcher asks a set of premeditated questions while in an unstructured interview, the researcher does not make use of a set of premeditated questions. Rather he or she depends on spontaneity and follow-up questioning in order to gather relevant information. 

On the other hand, a semi-structured interview is a type of interview that allows the researcher to deviate from  premeditated questions in order to gather more  information about the research subject. You can conduct structured interviews online by creating and administering a survey online on Formplus .

  • Observation

Observation is a type of data-gathering method that involves paying close attention to a phenomenon for a specific period of time in order to gather relevant information about its behaviors. When carrying out basic research, the researcher may need to study the research subject for a stipulated period as it interacts with its natural environment. 

Observation can be structured or unstructured depending on its procedures and approach. In structured observation, the data collection is carried out using a predefined procedure and in line with a specific schedule while unstructured observation is not restricted to a predetermined procedure. 

An experiment is a type of quantitative data-gathering method that seeks to validate or refute a hypothesis and it can also be used to test existing theories. In this method of data collection , the researcher manipulates dependent and independent variables to achieve objective research outcomes. 

Typically, in an experiment, the independent variable is modified or changed in order to determine its effects on the dependent variables in the research context. This can be done using 3 major methods; controlled experiments , field experiments, and natural experiments 

  • Questionnaire

A questionnaire is a data collection tool that is made up of a series of questions to which the research subjects provide answers. It is a cost-effective method of data gathering because it allows you to collect large samples of data from the members of the group simultaneously. 

You can create and administer your pure research questionnaire online using Formplus and you can also make use of paper questionnaires; although these are  easily susceptible to damage. [

Here is a step-by-step guide of how to create and administer questionnaires for basic research using Formplus: 

  • Sign in to Formplus

what is basic research study

In the Formplus builder, you can easily create different questionnaires for applied research by dragging and dropping preferred fields into your form. To access the Formplus builder, you will need to create an account on Formplus. 

Once you do this, sign in to your account and click on “Create Form ” to begin.

Edit Form Title

Click on the field provided to input your form title, for example, “Basic Research Questionnaire”.

what is basic research study

Click on the edit button to edit the form.

i. Add Fields: Drag and drop preferred form fields into your form from  the Formplus builder   Inputs column. There are several field input options for questionnaires in the Formplus builder. 

ii. Edit fields

iii. Click on “Save”

iv. Preview form. 

Form Customization

basic-research-questionnaire

With the form customization options in the form builder, you can easily change the look and feel of your form and make it more unique and personalized. Formplus allows you to change your form theme, add background images and even change the font according to your brand specifications. 

Multiple Sharing Options

what is basic research study

Formplus offers multiple form sharing options which enables you to easily share your questionnaire with respondents. You can use the direct social media sharing buttons to share your form link to your  social media pages. 

In addition, Formplus has an option to convert form links to QR codes; you can personalize and display your form QR code on your website/banners for easy access. You also can send out survey forms as email invitations to your research subjects.  

  • Data Reporting

 Data reporting is a type of data collection method where the researcher gathers relevant data and turns them in for further analysis in order to arrive at specific conclusions. The crux of this method depends, almost entirely, on the validity of the data collected. 

  • Case Studies

A case study is a type of data collection method that involves the detailed examination of a specific subject matter in order to gather objective information about the features and behaviors of the research subject. This method of data gathering is primarily qualitative , although it can also be quantitative or numerical in nature.  

Case studies involve a detailed contextual analysis of a limited number of events or conditions and their relationships. In carrying out a case study, the researcher must take extra care to identify the research questions, collect relevant data then evaluate and analyze the data in order to arrive at objective conclusions. 

Read More: Research Questions: Definition, Types +[Examples]

How is Basic Research Different from Applied Research? 

 Applied research is a type of research that is concerned with solving practical problems using scientific methods while basic research is a type of research that is concerned with the expansion of knowledge. 

Basic research generates new theories or improves on existing theories hence, it is theoretical in nature. On the other hand, applied research creates practical solutions to specific problems hence, it is practical in nature. 

 Basic research is knowledge-specific while applied research is solution-specific. 

  • Research Purpose

The purpose of basic research is to improve on existing knowledge or to discover new knowledge while the purpose of applied research is to solve specific problems. 

The scope of basic research is universal while applied research is limited in nature. This means that while applied research addresses a specific problem and is limited to the problem which it addresses, basic research explores multiple dimensions of various fields. 

  • Basic research is primarily explanatory while applied research is descriptive in nature .
  • Basic research adopts an indirect approach to problem-solving while applied research adopts a direct approach to problem-solving.
  • In basic research, generalizations are common while in applied research, specific problems are investigated without the aim of generalizations.
Read Also: What is Applied Research? +[Types, Examples & Methods]

Characteristics of Basic Research 

  • Basic research is analytical in nature.
  • It aims at theorizing concepts and not solving specific problems.
  • It is primarily concerned with the expansion of knowledge and not with the applicability of the research outcomes.
  • Basic research is explanatory in nature.
  • Basic research is carried out without any primary focus on possible practical ends.
  • It improves the general knowledge and understanding of different fields of study.

Importance of Basic Research

  • Acquisition of New Knowledge: Basic research results in new knowledge. It is responsible for many research breakthroughs in different fields of study and it is often considered as the pacesetter in technological and innovative solutions.
  • Basic research also enhances the understanding of different subject matters and provides multiple possible dimensions for interpretation of these subject matters.
  • Findings of fundamental research are extremely useful in expanding the pool of knowledge in different disciplines.
  • Basic research offers the foundation for applied research.

Disadvantages of Basic Research

  • Findings from pure research have little or no immediate practical implications. However, these findings may be useful in providing solutions to different problems, in the long run.
  • Fundamental research does not have strict deadlines.
  • Basic research does not solve any specific problems.

Basic research is an important research method because it exposes researchers to varying dimensions within a field of study. This proves useful, not only for improving scholarship and the general knowledge-base, but for solving problems as is the concern of applied research. 

When carrying out basic research, the investigator adopts one or more qualitative and quantitative observation methods which includes case studies, experiments and observation. These data collection methods help the researcher to gather the most valid and relevant information for the research. 

In the case of using a survey or questionnaire for data collection , this can easily be done with the use of Formplus forms. Formplus allows you to create and administer different kinds of questionnaires, online and you can easily monitor and categ orise your form responses too. 

Logo

Connect to Formplus, Get Started Now - It's Free!

  • applied basic research differences
  • basic research example
  • basic-research-characteristics
  • basic-research-method
  • pure research
  • pure research example
  • busayo.longe

Formplus

You may also like:

Basic vs Applied Research: 15 Key Differences

Differences between basic and applied research in definition, advantages, methods, types and examples

what is basic research study

What is Applied Research? + [Types, Examples & Method]

Simple guide on applied research; its types, examples, characteristics, methods, and advantages

21 Chrome Extensions for Academic Researchers in 2022

In this article, we will discuss a number of chrome extensions you can use to make your research process even seamless

Selection Bias in Research: Types, Examples & Impact

In this article, we’ll discuss the effects of selection bias, how it works, its common effects and the best ways to minimize it.

Formplus - For Seamless Data Collection

Collect data the right way with a versatile data collection tool. try formplus and transform your work productivity today..

what is basic research study

Cleveland awards $600,000 to study ‘universal basic employment’

C LEVELAND, Ohio — City Hall is giving $600,000 to United Way of Greater Cleveland to conduct research on how to set up a “universal basic employment” pilot program in Cleveland.

A city news release said Cleveland is the first city in the United States to explore the idea of “universal basic employment” as a way to reduce poverty, provide well-paid jobs, and reduce the need for public assistance.

If the city-funded research-and-development phase proves successful, local leaders hope to pursue a three-year pilot program that would pay roughly 100 working participants a $50,000-a-year salary. In some cases, the money would augment the lower wages those employees are already earning. In other cases, unemployed participants would receive a job. Wage subsidies would be provided to their employers, and employers would pay the cost of health insurance and other benefits, according to the news release.

While the concept of “universal basic income” has gained growing recognition in recent years, the concept of “universal basic employment” is more obscure. Here in Cleveland, the concept has been championed by Devin Cotten , founder and CEO of the organization Universal Basic Employment & Opportunity. His idea was recognized as a finalist in the 2023 Accelerate awards.

Cotten is working with the city and United Way on the city-funded research, which is expected to take two years. The United Way will provide office space, administrative support, marketing, communications, and fundraising assistance.

The city is using some its last remaining money from the American Rescue Plan Act to pursue the idea, which was proposed by Councilwoman Stephanie Howse-Jones.

“Far too often, the public sector spends an enormous amount of money on programs and resources for residents in need – rather than the immediate self-sustaining support that can assist them in making real change and improvement right now,” Howse-Jones said in the release. “I believe this innovative program will move the needle and empower Clevelanders to improve their financial futures.”

One of the goals of UBE is to provide sufficient wages to workers in industries that often pay little. Cotten’s organization defines a sufficient wage, in part, as one that makes a worker ineligible for most social safety net benefits, and provides them with access to private banking services.

©2024 Advance Local Media LLC. Visit cleveland.com. Distributed by Tribune Content Agency, LLC.

Cleveland City Hall. Interior of grand hall. January 23, 2023.

A Basic Bibliometric Review of Research on Arab Female Entrepreneurship

  • Published: 24 May 2024

Cite this article

what is basic research study

  • Aref Al-Attari 1 ,
  • Eman Bani Essa   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1087-8402 1 &
  • Fahad Atari 2  

27 Accesses

Explore all metrics

This study aims to provide an evidence-mapping of the knowledge base of Arab female entrepreneurship. It reviews 107 articles on Arab FE published in both Arabic and English language journals between 1990 and 2020. Being a systematic review, this study utilizes the basic bibliometric analysis to uncover the field’s structure and dynamics, gain deeper insights, and enrich conversation on the subject. The results identified some patterns as well as gaps, inter alia the modest contribution of mainstream Arab universities; the tiny number of articles written in the Arabic language; the shyly addressed topics besides some that are totally absent. The results show that the Arab FE scholarship is promising and has the potential to mature should gaps be addressed. This study contributes in many ways. First, it provides a focused analysis of Arab FE research published since the 1990s. In addition, it outlines areas for future research in scholarship on Arab FE. In this sense, it responds to the calls to explore FE in diverse geographical settings, to conduct research that reflects the actual rather than the assumed status quo, and to celebrate the diversity, richness, and complexities of women’s experiences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

what is basic research study

Similar content being viewed by others

what is basic research study

Deconstructing the Myth: African Women Entrepreneurs’ Access to Resources

Women entrepreneurship: research review and future directions.

what is basic research study

A systematic literature review of women in social entrepreneurship

Abou-Moghli, A. A., & Al-Abdallah, G. M. (2019). A systematic review of women entrepreneurs: Opportunities and challenges in Saudi Arabia. Journal of Entrepreneurship Education, 22 (6), 1–12.

Google Scholar  

Ahl, H. (2006). Why research on women entrepreneurs needs new directions. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 30 (5), 595–621.

Article   Google Scholar  

Ahl, H., & Marlow, S. (2012). Exploring the dynamics of gender, feminism and entrepreneurship: Advancing debate to escape a dead end? Organization, 19 (5), 543–562.

Al-Dajani, H., Carter, S., Shaw, E., & Marlow, S. (2015). Entrepreneurship among the displaced and dispossessed: Exploring the limits of emancipatory entrepreneuring. British Journal of Management, 26 (4), 713–730.

Aparicio, G., Iturralde, T., & Rodríguez, A. (2023). Developments in the knowledge-based economy research field: a bibliometric literature review. Management Review Quarterly, 73 (1), 317–352.

Bastian, B., Sidani, Y., & El Amine, Y. (2018). Women entrepreneurship in the Middle East and North Africa: A review of knowledge areas and research gaps. Gender in Management: An International Journal, 33 (1), 14–29.

Ben Slimane, S., Coeurderoy, R., & Mhenni, H. (2022). Digital transformation of small and medium enterprises: A systematic literature review and an integrative framework. International Studies of Management and Organization, 52 (2), 96–120.

Bendickson, J. (2021). Building entrepreneurship research for impact: Scope, phenomenon, and translation. Journal of Small Business Management, 59 (4), 535–543.

Borgatti, S. P., & Halgin, D. S. (2011). On network theory. Organization Science, 22 (5), 1168–1181.

Browne, E., Haider, H., Oddsdottir, F., Rohwerder, B., & Strachan, A. L. (2014). Gender in fragile and conflict-affected situations (Rapid Literature Review) . GSDRC, University of Birmingham.

Brush, C., de Bruin, A., & Welter, F. (2010). A gender-aware framework for women’s entrepreneurship. International Journal of Gender and Entrepreneurship, 1 (1), 8–24.

Brush, C. G., Manolova, T. S., & Edelman, L. F. (2008). Separated by a common language? Entrepreneurship research across the Atlantic. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 32 (2), 249–266.

Busenitz, L., Plummer, L., Klotz, A., Shahzad, A., & Rhoads, K. (2014). Entrepreneurship research (1985–2009) and the emergence of opportunities. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 38 (5), 981–1000.

Calás, M., Smircich, L., & Bourne, K. (2009). Extending the boundaries: Reframing “Entrepreneurship as Social Change” through feminist perspectives. The Academy of Management Review, 34 (3), 552–569.

Cardella, G., Hernández-Sánchez, B., & Sánchez-García, J. (2020). Women entrepreneurship: A systematic review to outline the boundaries of scientific literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 11 , 1557. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01557

Chen, S., Sharma, G., & Munoz, P. (2023). In pursuit of impact: From research questions to problem formulation in entrepreneurship research. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 47 (2), 232–264.

Correa, V., Brito, F., de Lima, R., & Queiroz, M. (2021). Female entrepreneurship in emerging and developing countries: A systematic literature review. International Journal of Gender and Entrepreneurship . https://doi.org/10.1108/IJGE-08-2021-0142

Curran, J., & Blackburn, R. (2001). Researching the Small Enterprise . SAGE.

Book   Google Scholar  

De Bruin, A., Brush, C. G., & Welter, F. (2007). Advancing a framework for coherent research on women’s entrepreneurship. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 31 (3), 323–339.

Deng, W., Liang, Q., Li, J., & Wang, W. (2020). Science mapping: Abibliometric analysis of female entrepreneurship studies. Gender in Management: An International Journal . https://doi.org/10.1108/GM-12-2019-0240

Dimov, D., Schaefer, R., & Pistrui, J. (2020). Look Who Is Talking … and Who Is Listening: Finding an integrative “We” voice in entrepreneurial scholarship. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice , 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/1042258720914507

Drakopoulou Dodd, S., McDonald, S., McElwee, G., & Smith, R. (2014). A Bourdieusian analysis of qualitative authorship in entrepreneurship scholarship. Journal of Small Business Management, 52 (4), 633–654.

Dy, A. M. (2019). Levelling the playing field? Towards a critical-social perspective on digital entrepreneurship. Futures . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.futures.2019.102438

El-Fiky, S. (2021). “ Gendered entrepreneurship contextualization in Egypt: An empirical study on policies for women entrepreneurship empowerment.” [Master's Thesis, the American University in Cairo]. AUC Knowledge Fountain. https://fount.aucegypt.edu/etds/1523 . Accessed 22 Feb 2023.

Ellson, T. (2009). Assessing contribution of research in business to practice. Journal of Business Research, 62 (11), 1160–1164.

ESCWA. (2019). Innovation and entrepreneurship: Opportunities and challenges for Arab youth and women . Lebanon.

Ferreira, M. P., Reis, N. R., & Miranda, R. (2015). Thirty years of entrepreneurship research published in top journals: Analysis of citations, co-citations and themes. Journal of Global Entrepreneurship Research, 5 (1), 2–22.

Frank, H., & Landström, H. (2015). What makes entrepreneurship research interesting? Reflections on strategies to overcome the rigor–relevance gap. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development , 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/08985626.2015.1100687

Gantman, E., & Dabos, M. (2018). Research output and impact of the fields of management, economics, and sociology in Spain and France: An analysis using Google scholar and Scopus. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 69 (8), 1054–1066.

Gartner, W. (2012). Entrepreneurship as organization creation, Handbook on organizational entrepreneurship . Edward Elgar Publishing.

Gartner, W. B., Davidsson, P., & Zahra, S. A. (2006). Are you talking to me? The nature of community in entrepreneurship scholarship. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice, 30 , 321–331.

Gartner, W. B., Roberts, J., & Rabideau, M. (2015). “Perspectives on arts entrepreneurship.” part 2. Artivate, 4 (2), 3–9.

Gerxhani, K., Kulic, N., & Liechti, F. (2020). Double standards? Co-authorship and gender bias in early-stage academic hiring . In LIVES Working Paper 86/2020 . Swiss Centre of Expertise in Life Course Research.

Hanafi, S., & Aravanitis, R. (2014). The marginalization of the Arab language in social science: Structural constraints and dependency by choice. Current Sociology, 62 (5), 723–742.

Henriksen, D. (2018). Research collaboration and co-authorship in the social sciences . Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Denmark.

Henry, C., Coleman, S., Foss, L., Orser, B., & Brush, C. (2021). Richness in diversity: Towards more contemporary research conceptualisations of women’s entrepreneurship. International Small Business Journal: Researching Entrepreneurship, 39 (7), 609–618.

Holquist, C., & Sundin, E. (1990). What’s special about highly educated women entrepreneurs? Entrepreneurship & Regional Development, 2 (2), 181–194.

Hughes, K. D., Jennings, J. E., Brush, C., Carter, S., & Welter, F. (2012). Extending women’s entrepreneurship research in new directions. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 36 (3), 429–442.

Jennings, J., & Brush, C. (2013). Research on women entrepreneur. The Academy of Management Annals, 7 (1), 661–713.

Husseini, R. (1997). Promoting women entrepreneurs in Lebanon: The experience of UNIFEM. Gender and Development, 5 (1), 49–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/741922301

Hyland, K. (2016). Academic publishing and the myth of linguistic injustice. Journal of Second Language Writing, 31 , 58–69.

JEDCO. (2019). “Jordan Enterprise Development Corporation), GEM (Global Entrepreneurship Monitor and the University of Jordan Strategic Studies Center (2018) Entrepreneurship in Jordan.” report retrieved 18/11/2019 (In Arabic)

Kargwell, S. (2015). Landscaping of emerging women entrepreneurs in the MENA countries: Activities, obstacles and empowerment. International Journal of Business and Social Science, 6 (2), 222–234.

Katz, J. (2008). Fully mature but not fully legitimate: A different perspective on the state of entrepreneurship education. Journal of Small Business Management, 64 (4), 550–566.

Kuhn, T. (1970). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions (2nd ed.). University of Chicago Press.

Landström, H., Harirchi, G., & Åström, F. (2012). Entrepreneurship: Exploring the knowledge base. Research Policy, 41 (7), 1154–1181.

Linan, F., & Chen, Y. (2006). Testing the Entrepreneurial Intentional Model on a Two-Country Sample , Document de Treball núm. In 06/7, Departament d'Economia de l'Empresa, Facultat de Ciències Econòmiques i Empresarials . Universitat Autònoma de.

Markin, E., Swab, R. G., & Marshall, D. R. (2017). Who is driving the bus? An analysis of author and institution contributions to entrepreneurship research. Journal of Innovation and Knowledge, 2 (1), 1–9.

Marlow, S., & Dy, A. (2018). Annual review article: Is it time to rethink the gender agenda in entrepreneurship research? International Small Business Journal, 36 (1), 3–22.

Massaro, M., Dumay, J., & Guthrie, J. (2016). On the shoulders of giants: Undertaking a structured literature review in accounting. Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal, 29 (5), 767–801.

Maseda, A., Iturralde, T., Cooper, S., & Aparicio, G. (2022). Mapping women’s involvement in family firms: A review based on bibliographic coupling analysis. International Journal of Management Reviews, 24 , 279–305.

Milic´ V. 1982. “Sociology of knowledge and sociology of science. In: 10th World Congress of sociology.” Mexico City.

Miller, K., Alexander, A., Cunningham, J., & Albtas, E. (2018). Entrepreneurial academics and academic entrepreneurs: A systematic literature review. International Journal of Technology Management, 77 (1/2/3), 9–37.

Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., Shekelle, P., Stewart, LA., & PRISMA-P Group. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement.  Systematic Reviews, 4 (1), 1.

Moore, D. P. (1990). An examination of present research on the female entrepreneur—Suggested research strategies for the 1990's. Journal of Business Ethics, 9 (4-5), 275–281.

Moreira, J., Marques, C., Braga, A., & Ratten, V. (2019). A systematic review of women’s entrepreneurship and internationalization literature. International Business Review, 61 , 635–648.

Newman, M., & Gough, D. (2020). Systematic reviews in educational research: Methodology, perspectives and application, In: O. Zawacki-Richter et al. (eds.), Systematic Reviews in Educational Research . https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-27602-7_1

Nimmon, L., Artino, A., & Varpio, L. (2019). Social network theory in interprofessional education: Revealing hidden power. Journal of Graduate Medical Education , 246–250.

Parhankangas, A., & Darics, E. (2022). Language and communication in entrepreneurship research. In E. Darics (Ed.), language awareness in business and the professions (pp. 39-61) . Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108696159.006

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Plume, A., and van Weijen, D. 2014 “Publish or perish? The rise of the fractional author…,” Research Trends , 38, Article 5.

Pontille, D. (2003). Authorship practices and institutional contexts in sociology: Elements for a comparison of the United States and France. Science, Technology & Human Values, 28 (2), 217–243.

Said, E. (2002). Travelling theory reconsidered. In E. Said (Ed.), Reflections on Exile and Other Essays Convergences (pp. 436–452). Harvard University Press.

Shane, S. (1997). Who is publishing the entrepreneurship research? Journal of Management, 22 , 83–95.

Shane, S., & Venkataraman, S. (2000). The promise of entrepreneurship as a field of research. Academy of Management Review, 25 (1), 217–226.

Stengers I (ed.).1987 “D’une science a l’autre. Des concepts nomads [From One Science to Another: The Traveling Concepts].” Paris: Le Seuil.

Steyaert, C., & Katz, J. (2004). Reclaiming the space of entrepreneurship in society: Geographical, discursive and social dimensions. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development, 16 (3), 179–196.

Surangi, H. (2014). Discourse analysis in women entrepreneurial networks: A review of the literature. Economics World, 2 (1), 25–35.

Thompson, N., Verduijn, K., & Gartner, W. (2020). Entrepreneurship-as-practice: Grounding contemporary theories of practice into entrepreneurship studies, entrepreneurship & regional development , 32 (3-4), 247–256. https://doi.org/10.1080/08985626.2019.1641978

Tlaiss, H. (2019). Contextualizing the career success of Arab women entrepreneurs. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development, 31 (3-4), 226–241.

Trad, M. (2015). Women and entrepreneurship: Evidence from Lebanon and United Arab Emirates. International Journal of Contemporary Management, 14 (4), 7–22. https://doi.org/10.4467/24498939IJCM.15.008.4487

Tranfield, D., Denyer, D., & Smart, P. (2003). Towards a methodology for developing evidence-informed management knowledge by means of systematic review. British Journal of Management, 14 , 207–222.

Welter, F. (2011). Contextualizing entrepreneurship—Conceptual challenges and ways forward. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 35 (1), 165–184.

Williamson, S., Mueller, C., Van Deusen, C., & Perryman, A. (2007). The influence of national religious consciousness on entrepreneurial behavior. International Business: Research Teaching and Practice , 53–76.

Yadav, V., & Unni, J. (2016). Women entrepreneurship: Research review and future directions. Journal of Global Entrepreneurship Research, 6 (1), 12.

Zahra, S., Wright, M., & Abdelgawad, S. (2014). Contextualization and the advancement of entrepreneurship research. International Small Business Journal, 32 (5), 479–500.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Yarmouk University, Irbid, Jordan

Aref Al-Attari & Eman Bani Essa

Entrepreneurship, Princess Sumaya University, Amman, Jordan

Fahad Atari

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eman Bani Essa .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Al-Attari, A., Essa, E.B. & Atari, F. A Basic Bibliometric Review of Research on Arab Female Entrepreneurship. J Knowl Econ (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13132-024-01905-0

Download citation

Received : 15 April 2023

Accepted : 04 March 2024

Published : 24 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s13132-024-01905-0

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Arab scholarship
  • Female entrepreneurship
  • Basic Bibliometrics
  • Systematic review
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

Logo of springeropen

What is Basic Research? Insights from Historical Semantics

Désirée schauz.

Technische Universität München, Fachgebiet Technikgeschichte, c/o Deutsches Museum, Museumsinsel 1, 80538 Munich, Germany

For some years now, the concept of basic research has been under attack. Yet although the significance of the concept is in doubt, basic research continues to be used as an analytical category in science studies. But what exactly is basic research? What is the difference between basic and applied research? This article seeks to answer these questions by applying historical semantics. I argue that the concept of basic research did not arise out of the tradition of pure science. On the contrary, this new concept emerged in the late 19th and early 20th centuries, a time when scientists were being confronted with rising expectations regarding the societal utility of science. Scientists used the concept in order to try to bridge the gap between the promise of utility and the uncertainty of scientific endeavour. Only after 1945, when United States science policy shaped the notion of basic research, did the concept revert to the older ideals of pure science. This revival of the purity discourse was caused by the specific historical situation in the US at that time: the need to reform federal research policy after the Second World War, the new dimension of ethical dilemmas in science and technology during the atomic era, and the tense political climate during the Cold War.

For some years now, the concept of basic research has been under attack. Its relevance has been questioned empirically as a result of changes in academic research, normatively with respect to science policy, and even theoretically in science and technology studies. Yet while the significance of the concept is in doubt, basic research is still a very common analytical category, deployed not least as a means of distinguishing the new future science policy from the old ideal of basic research. But what exactly is basic research? What is the difference between basic and applied research? Aside from a few exceptional studies (Calvert 2006 ; Godin 2005a ; Pielke 2012 ), science studies have only just begun to seriously reflect upon these questions. When and why did the concept of basic research emerge in the first place? Is the ideal of basic research nothing more than a relaunch of the older pure-science discourse? Historical semantics appears to be a useful approach for answering these questions because its historical perspective provides the conceptual clarity required both in current debates in science and technology studies and public debates on science policy.

In the 1990s, sociological studies claimed that science was undergoing profound changes. Since then, prominent labels such as “Mode 2” or “triple helix” have come to signify a new way of organizing science and technology that transgresses institutional boundaries between universities, industry, and governmental research. According to the alleged paradigm shift from Mode 1 to Mode 2, application-oriented research programmes with cooperative and transdisciplinary project teams have replaced the former university-centred basic research mode. Proponents of this new way of comprehending knowledge production even call for science policy to be modified in order to reflect the altered research mode (Gibbons et al. 1994 ; Etzkowitz and Leydesdorff 1997 ). Our “Leonardo world”, as portrayed by Jürgen Mittelstraß, is ruled by the imperative of technology. The interplay of science and technology raises society’s expectations of research applications, even when the outcomes sometimes turn out to be risky (Mittelstraß 1994 ). These arguments have certainly shaped the debates in science and technology studies and science policy in recent years, although discussions about the degree of change and how to evaluate it remain controversial (Weingart 2008 ; Greenberg 2007 ).

According to studies addressing these historical shifts in science, basic research determined the status quo ante. These studies describe basic research as an application-disinterested mode of research embedded in a disciplinary and academic setting that contrasts, in respect of every analytical feature, to Mode 2. The concept of Mode 1, however, is not based upon profound historical analysis; it rather appears to represent the previously prevailing sociological perspective on science in the tradition of Robert Merton, who emphasized disinterestedness and universalism as central characteristics of modern science. Yet historical studies suggest that the way in which science was organized had already undergone significant change in the early 20th century, as politicians, scientists, and industry formed a new alliance from which all three groups hoped to benefit (Ash 2002 ; Mowery and Rosenberg 1993 ).

Moreover, although recent debates in science studies have demonstrated high levels of discontent with the notion of basic research, producing instead new analytic labels like triple helix or Mode 2, the term “basic research” and its antonym “applied research” continue to frame the discourse about science, without any awareness of both terms’ historical conditionality as discursive strategies in research policy. The semantic dichotomy merely gives way to a continuum between basic and applied research in which the favourite mode, the “use-inspired basic research” (in German “ anwendungsorientierte Grundlagenforschung ”), is located somewhere in the middle of the continuum (Stokes 1997 ; Mittelstraß 1994 ). However, aside from the motif of application, we lack an explicit set of distinctive criteria because studies persist in assuming basic research to be a given category.

In other studies, categories such as basic and applied research no longer play a major role. Research grounded in approaches such as actor-network theory, that is studies emphasizing the societal context of science, anthropological studies focusing on day-to-day laboratory work, and the new – although still vague – concept of technoscience are united in their critique of discursive boundaries, which they claim to obstruct the view on the reality of research. While research dealing with Mode 2 indicates the change within the historical development of science, those supporting these new approaches call for a change in theoretical perspective. Bruno Latour, one of the most famous proponents of this idea, identifies demarcations such as nature/society or science/technology as a typically modern delusion covering, albeit quite successfully, the hybrid character of research (Latour 1993 ). Claiming an overall paradigm shift for the social sciences, Latour suspects that traditional sociology has frozen thought within boundaries and institutional separations in its studies for quite a long time and levels his criticisms at a static display of society blind to the dynamics of interactions (Latour 2005 ). For Latour, the distinction between basic and applied research is supposed to be part of these delusive demarcations: such a simple dichotomous order cannot represent the “complicated and unpredictable relations between scientists and other agencies” (Latour 1987 : 117). Latour argues that the high esteem in which basic science is held does not correspond with the reality of technoscience. In his early call for the concept of technoscience, he even argued statistically, interpreting the high proportion of spending on development and applied research in contrast to that spent on basic research evident in research and development statistics as indicative of the real importance of technology and the level of overall support it receives within society (Latour 1987 : esp. 168–173).

Latour’s argument about modern delusions and his opposition to a basic-research-centred perspective on science have found resonance among some historians of science. For instance, Peter Dear identifies the ideology of modern science as misrepresenting the reality of research in the natural sciences. Although, according to Dear, some effort has been made to integrate the instrumental and useful character of the natural sciences in the tradition of science since Francis Bacon, natural philosophy, with its ideal of contemplative understanding, has retained the upper hand (Dear 2005 : 404). From an historical point of view, this discrepancy between the philosophical notion of science and research practice appears as an anachronism requiring explanation. In general, criticism levelled at the long-prevailing ideal of pure science has led to a reorientation in the history of science that includes the applied side of science and opens up the field to the history of technology (Forman 2010 ). Recent studies look beyond the academic core – the universities – and into industrial laboratories, where the majority of researchers have worked throughout the 20th century (Shapin 2008 ).

Despite this growing awareness of the ideological or normative character of basic research, the majority of historians still use the concept as a given, analytical category without questioning its relationship to varying historical contexts. Studies on German war-time science, for instance, try to determine to which extreme of the basic-applied continuum the examined research projects tended. 1 As to the history of US science and innovation policy, the concept of basic research seems to be inevitably associated with the name of Vannevar Bush and the reorganization of US science after the Second World War. The basic-applied taxonomy is therefore primarily regarded as representation of the institutional logic of modern research organization: the so-called linear model which coined the idea of innovation process for so many years. Investigating the negotiation of science policy in the 1940s, historical studies have revealed dissenting political preferences and conflictive institutional interests, demonstrating that the post-war order in science policy had initially been highly contested. However, although the historical contingency of the concept has thus become more and more apparent, many historians still do not reflect on the meanings and functions of the concept of basic research. Even the meteoric career of this relatively young term does not seem to be puzzling historians. They rather interpret the concept as additional part of an existing taxonomy, “supplementing” the former “language of pure and applied science” (Dennis 2004 : 225). As a result, the concept of basic research has been locked up in a black box next to “pure science” whose meaning is also still enigmatic (Galison 2008 ). It is only recently that the investigation of shifting functions, varying meanings and symbolic dimensions of the concept of basic research – beyond the institutional level of research organization and funding – have become an object of interest in the history of science (Krige 2006 ). 2

What remains of the current debates in science and technology studies is the question as to why demarcations such as basic and applied research have occurred at all. If Latour is right in pointing out that the concepts of basic and applied research do not represent actual research practices, why have these terms become so important? Which (other) functions have they fulfilled? More precisely, what has “basic research” meant for the identity of science and for its relationship to technology? Which role has the concept of basic research played in science policy, that is in the negotiations between science and society about aims and values of research? And how has the concept affected the public image of science?

This article therefore seeks to analyze the genesis of the concept of basic research up until the early 1960s, by which time it had become a common concept in science policy in the West. It will also take a brief look at discourses on pure science prevalent in the 19th century as a means of establishing the effects of historical legacy and variation over time. This study has two central aims. Firstly, it intends to detect the different semantic dimensions of basic research – its institutional, epistemic, ethical, social, and political attributions. Secondly, it discusses the significance of the concept of basic research in the natural sciences, in research policy, and in science studies: to which historical challenges faced by research in the 20th century did the concept of basic research respond?

I argue that the concepts of basic research and fundamental research did not arise out of the 19th-century tradition of pure science, which had idealized research as an intrinsically philosophical search for eternal truth. On the contrary, these new concepts emerged in the late 19th and early 20th centuries at a time when society’s expectations regarding the utility of science were rising sharply. In the knowledge that research output is hard to predict, scientists used these concepts to bridge the gap between the promise of utility and the uncertainty of scientific endeavour. Only after 1945, when US policy strongly shaped the notion of basic research, did these concepts revert to the older ideals of pure science. In order to understand this revival of the purity discourse, we need to take the specific historical situation of the post-war US into account, in particular the new plans for federal funding of research, the new dimension of ethical dilemmas faced by science and technology following Hiroshima, and the overall political climate of the Cold-War era. The insights gained from historical semantics show that basic research was not – and cannot be – considered a clearly distinguishable analytical mode of research. After 1945, the concept of basic research formed part of a discursive strategy that adjusted scientific research to complex and even contradictory societal requirements; it was for these socio-political reasons that the concept became so important. Consequently, moral and ideological attributions were and still are inseparably tied to the concept of basic research.

American and German discourses provide the empirical basis of this study. Yet this article is not intended as a fully-fledged comparative study of two countries. Rather, I analyze Germany and the US because these countries were considered best-practice models in science at varying points in time and they both share a long history of mutual exchange and learning. At different points in time, each of the two countries allows us to trace the emergence and evolution of specific understandings of the role of science in society. The first section on the older pure-science ideals of the 19th century revolves mainly around Germany, which had become a leading science nation at that time. In the following section, which discusses how the concept of basic research emerged and evolved until 1945, the German experience also takes centre stage. The third section covers US science policy from the Second World War until the early 1960s, when the term basic research had become established as a key concept in science policy. The article ends, on a more comparative note, with a short history of the concept of basic research in post-war Germany. The second and the third sections overlap in time because the Second World War and the post-war period require a more comparative perspective. For a long time, scientific research during the Nazi period was thought to represent a turning away from all fundamental principles of science. The war, however, confronted both US and German scientists with similar political demands and requirements. After 1945, US policy became a role model for the Federal Republic of Germany (West Germany). Before the empirical analysis commences, however, the next section will introduce readers to historical semantics and discuss how I will use this approach to structure the empirical discussion.

Some Remarks on Historical Semantics

This study resorts to approaches in conceptual history and discourse analysis. Discourse analysis fits with the research questions for several reasons. Firstly, it is designed to make visible what is taken for granted when people think or talk about social phenomena and the implicit rules that apply in the practice of framing topics. Secondly, discourse analysis identifies classifications and demarcations, such as the distinction between basic and applied research, as essential strategies in discursive practice. Thirdly, it is based on the assumption that discursive production is historically contingent. Whereas discourse analysis strives, in the main, to analyze patterns of assertions, conceptual history focuses on semantics and key concepts. Especially the latter takes the polysemy of language and communication into account. Moreover, conceptual history’s foundation in the philosophy of history means that it offers us assumptions about semantic shifts over time.

In contrast to the tradition of semantic analyses in the philosophy of science, which is mainly interested in the epistemic impact of metaphors (Blumenberg 2010 ), my study is based on a strand of historical semantics rooted in historical studies on the dawn of modernity. It focuses on key concepts in social and political language. If we assume that basic research is largely a concept of science policy or of negotiations between the scientific community and the public, then this approach seems more suitable for this study. Moreover, conceptual history is embedded in reflections about the philosophy of history. According to Reinhart Koselleck, the major proponent of the German school of conceptual history, a shifting societal dictionary – the emergence of neologisms or changes in semantic attributions – indicates historical upheaval. Key concepts and parts of their meanings, however, may persist, so that old and new semantic dimensions coexist. Koselleck’s approach thus corresponds with approaches in the philosophy of history that take different layers of time into account. Koselleck clearly demonstrates that language is not an epiphenomenon of reality, but rather that it frames both human experience and the way in which society perceives the world. He conceives key concepts as cognitive strategies designed to deal with reality, especially in situations where expectation and experience diverge. Ideologies, in particular, are supposed to compensate semantically for a lack of convergence between expectations and experiences (Koselleck 2006 : 85).

Whereas Koselleck’s conceptual history defines key concepts primarily as cognitive strategies of the human that deal with reality, discourse analysis goes further in assuming that discursive strategies might serve various societal functions. In his commentary on the concept of the dispositive, Michel Foucault emphasized that discourses, non-discursive practices, institutions, and objects are linked by common strategic functions. This does not mean, however, that the outcome of such a strategic dispositive necessarily corresponds to the initial function. On the one hand, novel discourses have the power to set new practices or different forms of institutional organization. On the other hand, it is also possible that emerging discourses provide existing institutions or operations with new legitimacy. The history of dispositives also turns out to be quite complex. Taking Foucault’s remarks on the philosophy of history into account, the concept of the dispositive is quite similar to Koselleck’s idea of a complex history of different layers of time lying upon one another (Schauz 2010 ).

Since discourse analysis has progressed by adapting aspects of polysemy, the combination with conceptual historical approaches has become more obvious. One approach appears to be particularly fruitful for investigating the history of basic research: Jürgen Link’s idea of “collective symbols”, which came about when Link dealt with the problem of interdiscursive processes. Link believes that multiple meanings of metaphors and symbols are capable of linking different discourses demonstrating diverse patterns of assertions (Link 1986 ). In other words, metaphors can bridge discursive gaps. With regard to this study, science policy may be described as one such interdiscursive process in which scientific expectations encounter society’s expectations. And, without anticipating the detailed analysis of the concept of basic research below, it is obvious that “basic” as the first part of the compound offers a variety of possible interpretations.

Of course, discourse analysis also has a tradition in science studies, in particular regarding demarcation discourses. Most relevant in this context is Thomas F. Gieryn’s study ( 1999 ) on the cultural boundaries of science, which he identifies as resulting from professional boundary work. According to Gieryn, boundary work does not represent fixed or institutional demarcations, but is rather a dynamic process of negotiations with contested boarders and regenerated situations of uncertainty. Gieryn stresses that boundaries linked to key concepts such as pure science vary according to special situations and social circumstances. Unlike Gieryn, however, I do not expect that discursive practices revolving around basic research are strategies exclusively used by scientists to protect their professional interests. Moreover, I doubt that the discursive function of basic research can be restricted to boundary work.

In summary, this study is based upon the followings assumptions derived from conceptual history, discourse analysis, and studies on scientific boundary work. The attributions and linked demarcations of basic research are expected to vary according to space and time. Prior semantic dimensions, however, might persist or experience revival. The emergence of basic research as a new term may at least indicate an historical shift in either science or its role in society. The abstractness of the term basic research offers a wide range of meanings and discursive strategies. The concept has the potential to function as a collective symbol for science policy that links different discourses within society. Given its variability, this key concept of science policy, together with its antonyms, cannot be interpreted as representing fixed institutional boundaries. Rather, the concepts seem to emerge in situations of uncertainty or cognitive dissonance. Yet they may legitimize the institutional organization of research or define operative goals. Moreover, the discourses revolving around basic research communicate a wide range of ideals, expectations, promises, as well on professional and public claims.

Finally, there are some preliminary methodological remarks that need to be addressed. Although the study focuses on the concept of basic research, it also has to detect conceptual variations and alternative or concurrent terms, not to mention antonyms. Relevant terms for the US case are basic research, fundamental research, pure science and basic science. Antonyms and concurrent terms like applied research, applied science, contract research and mission-oriented research are included as far as they are needed to analyze the meanings of basic research, but their own conceptual histories will not be analyzed at full length. For the German case, these terms are Grundlagenforschung , reine Wissenschaft , reine Forschung , angewandte Forschung , angewandte Wissenschaft and Zweckforschung .

With regard to conducting the discourse analysis, it was most relevant to compile a broad sample of documents enabling me to identify prevalent, repeated patterns of assertions. 3 Besides key texts from scientists well-established in research organization, the sample also covers texts produced for normal-science communication. 4 The study is thus based on published documents relating to science policy as well as on scientific articles and books. Especially the volumes of the American journal Science and its German counterpart Die Naturwissenschaften have been subjected to systematic analysis. Furthermore, electronic search functions, in particular those enabling full-text searches with the keywords listed above, have been most useful for periods in which concepts were not yet commonplace. The digital library of Google Books is an important tool for historical semantics because it enables us to detect texts which might otherwise be overlooked by more traditional research strategies based on library holdings and cross references. As such, Google Books provides a unique tool for tracing both the emergence and diffusion of concepts. However, given that text acquisition in Google Books is dynamic and not entirely transparent to the user, it is difficult to delineate the corpus of books actually contained within its database. Thus Google Books may not be easy to use for scholars interested in exact bibliometric analysis, but it can help researchers gain a rough idea of when certain concepts began to be used and how use of these concepts became more or less common across different periods of time and within different language communities. This is how the current article uses the information derived from Google Books.

Pure Science in the 19th Century: The Natural Sciences and the Philosophical Tradition of Academia

As studies have so far located the concept of basic research in the tradition of pure-science ideals, the following section will deal with the term’s prehistory as a means of tracking continuities and breaks in the way science perceived itself. The notion of pure science and the conceptual opposition between “pure” and “applied” in science can be traced back to the 18th century. The attributes of “pure” and “applied” referred in turn to the much older, classical distinction between theory and practice that had undergone reinterpretation during the Scientific Revolution. Back then, Francis Bacon and his contemporaries had tried to conflate the new empirical and instrumental form of knowledge of nature with the older tradition of natural philosophy and its idea of contemplative understanding (Dear 2005 : 393–397). In the late 18th century, these attributes became important once again for natural scientists positioning themselves within the academic community for the purposes of finding a way into the university system. Although states such as Prussia demanded ever more instrumental knowledge and technical education for their mining industries or other state-owned enterprises (Klein 2010 ), natural scientists had to adjust to the predominant philosophical understanding of science 5 at universities, which, even then, consisted only of philosophical, theological, legal, and medical faculties.

In the case of chemistry, Christoph Meinel has already demonstrated that, in the Age of Enlightenment, chemists labelled their discipline as “pure and applied” so that chemistry could become an acceptable subject at universities, shedding its older status as an auxiliary science of medicine (Meinel 1985 ). Due to its empirical approach and its utilitarian orientation, chemistry was still classified as an “art” rather than as a “science” in the 18th century. Academic teaching had hitherto focused on imparting theoretical knowledge and established theorems, that is pure science. In contrast, the applied sciences represented experience-based knowledge on the epistemic level; at the same time “applied” denoted research with a practical purpose. Both aspects of these so-called applied sciences did not (yet) fit into the philosophical tradition of universities. By striving to become a part of this academic institution, chemists had to stress both the pure scientific and applied aspects of their discipline (Meinel 1985 ; Bud and Roberts 1984 ).

At the very same time, philosophy was engaged in reviving the controversy between rationalism and empiricism that solidified a hierarchical concept of knowledge. As a consequence of the philosophical longing for the wholeness and absoluteness of ideas, a posteriori approaches continually played a subordinate role in contrast to a priori and metaphysical ways of knowing (Ross 1962 : 68–69). The concept of cognition process in science turned out to be one-way: from the general to the particular. This concept of scientific progress implied the possibility of deducing endless applications and specific, context-linked knowledge from universal principles such as the laws of nature. The advancement of knowledge, however, was not supposed to take place the other way around. This distinction between pure and applied science thus corresponded to institutional and epistemic settings in the scientific community of the late 18th and 19th centuries.

The Natural Sciences Face Challenges from Engineering and Technological Success

In the mid-19th century the pure/applied boundary started focusing on the distinction between the natural sciences and technology. The common definition of technology as applied natural sciences represented a special version of this one-way concept of knowledge. This definition was widespread – even economists believed in the one-way relationship between science and technology. They assumed that only scientific discoveries and theories paved the way for innovations: “Technical science may stimulate pure science to a certain extent, but, on the whole, technology is much more at the receiving end. Pure science is always further ahead of applied science, and never the other way round. However, technology finally turns science into a common good” (Rössler 1857 : 179, translation by DS).

It was above all the community of natural scientists that wanted to preserve the hierarchical distinction between science and technology. The scientific foundation and the aspiring academic status of engineering in the second half of the 19th century challenged the scientific profession, in particular physicists (Gieryn 1999 : 51–62). As the natural sciences had only recently assumed their place within the university, the legacy of natural philosophy and its epistemic and moral ideals, such as the unrewarded dedication to science for its own sake, was even stronger than the century before (Dear 2005 : 401–404). Having scarcely ascended to the league of the pure sciences, the natural sciences even adopted the idea of an eternal truth defined by the discovery of natural laws.

The words of German physician Rudolf Virchow represent this adapted concept of pure science, but, more importantly, they also show that this purity discourse was not without contradictions. With the economic success of technical innovations and the growing appreciation of engineers within society throughout the German Empire, Virchow and his colleagues increasingly forged a link between themselves and the promise of technical progress in order to promote the idea of indispensable scientific endeavour:

All the benefits that have emerged from the steam engine, from telegraphy, photography, chemical discoveries, the production of colours and so on and so forth, all these benefits are based on scientific theorems that we men of science have unveiled, and not until we are absolutely sure that they are laws of nature, we pass these truths on to the general public so that others can work with them and create new things that nobody could imagine before, that no one has ever dreamt of, that see the light of day for the first time and transform the character of society and the state. (Virchow 1877 : 8–9, translation by DS)

Compared with the great engineering inventions of the 19th century and their noticeable effects on everyday life and society as a whole, scientific progress was less visible. In a way, this poor visibility was one aspect of the ideal of the pure scientist in its philosophical tradition: a scholar who, in solitude, dedicates life and work to science, driven by the sole motive of finding the truth – or at least contributing his tiny part to the scientific community’s joint effort – even without any prospect of public acknowledgement. In fact, as Peter Dear put it, “the authority of science in the modern world rests to a considerable extent on the idea that it is powerful, that it can do things” (Dear 2005 : 404). Yet, the scientific strategy of technological promise in order to gain greater visibility, support, and acknowledgment appeared risky; the scientific pledge to technological progress needed a show of confidence. Given the uncertainty and contingency of scientific advancement, it seemed even harder to predict if or when discoveries would lead to new technologies. Scientists thus defined their work as a long-term endeavour in contrast to engineering, which they classified as a medium-term project aimed at satisfying immediate need. In any case, the fact that researchers such as the chemist Justus von Liebig felt it necessary to defend the scientific profession reflects the growing pressure the scientific community faced from societal expectations in the course of the 19th century:

Even the most powerful effect of science on the life and spirit of men is so slow, noiseless, creeping and barely perceptible that a superficial observer would be hard pressed to assess its impact. The expert, however, knows that no real progress in this world is currently achieved without science and that the accusation whereby it is not of public benefit preoccupies the general public and not the men of science, who each in their own way, unwaveringly follow their goals. Indeed, they remain untroubled about the future benefits of their work since these accrue neither to them nor to an individual country but to the whole of mankind. (Liebig 1862 : 33, translation by DS)

Blurring Boundaries in the Late 19th and Early 20th Centuries: Scientists in Transition

The fact that scientists felt compelled to do boundary work indicates that scientific practice had already begun to change and that the hierarchical epistemic order no longer applied across the board. It was the birth of engineering as an academic discipline that set off this dynamic process of boundary work. By acquiring the right to award doctorates in the late 19th and early 20th centuries, the German technical colleges enhanced their academic status (König 1999 ). Leading figures of this new group of aspiring engineers such as Alois Riedler, a mechanical engineer and rector of the Technische Universität Berlin-Charlottenburg from 1899 to 1900, persistently stressed that the relationship between science and technology was a two-way process:

Technology has its natural share in the progress of the natural sciences; in many areas technology has even run ahead of the natural sciences until deeper scientific insights in turn paved the way for perfecting technical development; … [T]hrough the magnificence of its tangible achievements, technology has raised the public’s awareness of the natural sciences and has contributed enormously to making science, in general, more popular. (Riedler 1900 : 12, translation by DS)

Conversely, scientists themselves began to overcome the gap between (pure) science and technology. Related distinctions, for instance, between discovery and invention were also blurring. Within the expanding field of the natural sciences in the late 19th century, researchers had to transcend the limits of both established disciplines and methods in order to find out something new. The development of instruments became, more than ever before, an integral part of scientific work; the act of designing new techniques became as relevant as discovering new elements or laws of nature. The instrumentality of science, not only in terms of its methodological role of confirming theories but also in terms of its effectiveness, had finally become part of the image of the truthfulness of science in the modern world (Wilhelm Ostwald 1929 : 21; Dear 2005 : 404; Joerges and Shinn 2001 ).

Scientists such as the Nobel Prize winner and pioneer of physical chemistry Wilhelm Ostwald campaigned for closer cooperation between scientists and engineers. While criticizing the old supremacy of natural philosophy, he emphasized the similarities of scientific and technological endeavour, in particular a systematic approach to research and to the desire to venture into the unknown (Wilhelm Ostwald 1908 : 20). As far as Ostwald was concerned, scientists and engineers nonetheless differed in terms of their motivations (or goals) and their temporal perspective; having discovered a new technology, engineers abandoned scientific questioning, whereas scientists followed the path to its very end, hoping to find definitive explanations to their questions. Although this notion of the advancement of knowledge was less asymmetric than it had been a few decades earlier, the emphasis Ostwald placed on science’s long-term orientation and the continued ideal of human curiosity as a scientific value in itself demonstrated that a sense of the moral superiority of science endured. (Wilhelm Ostwald 1905 , 1911 ).

While the ideals of pure science were in the process of dissolving, by 1900, both the institutional settings of research and research practices in the natural sciences had already undergone significant change. The emergence of professional industrial laboratories with salaried researchers (initially in the chemical and electrical industry), the establishment of special research institutes outside of the universities (both national laboratories in the service of the state and research centres for specific research fields with mixed funding), the beginning of special funding programmes for science, and the more extensive involvement of the administration in science policy issues were some of the developments in science and in the attitudes within society towards science observable in different countries.

Studies into German science emphasize that two new types of institutes, the Notgemeinschaft der deutschen Wissenschaft (Emergency Association of German Science) and the Kaiser-Wilhelm-Gesellschaft (Kaiser Wilhelm Society), concluded an ongoing process of change in science at an institutional level that had come about in response to the limitations of the former university-centred organization of research and to the new expectations of industrialized mass society (Szöllösi-Janze 2005 ; Ash 2002 : 35–38). 6 The Kaiser Wilhelm Society, established in 1911 to promote the natural sciences in Germany, was a reaction to the increased requirements of disciplines such as chemistry and physics as well as a response to increasing industrial demand for scientific knowledge and growing international competition. With the financial support of both the state and influential entrepreneurs, scientists in the institutes on material research belonging to the Kaiser Wilhelm Society were able to concentrate their entire efforts on research, that is “pure science”, without needing to undertake teaching duties. The Emergency Association of German Science largely sponsored research projects at the universities. This fund, derived from a variety of sources and governed by academics, had been initiated by scientists after the First World War.

The funding programme Gemeinschaftsforschung (Collaborative Research), which sought to further public health, the economy, and the greater public good, together with the research areas pursued by several institutes belonging to the Kaiser Wilhelm Society provide evidence that the pure-science ideal was becoming less important. These self-governed academic institutions promoted research that responded directly to industrial and political demands. Collaborative Research, for example, financed projects which promised to either secure the production of raw materials or develop substitute materials, to improve material processing or technological development, and to increase food production.

To sum up the whole section, the historical overview from the 19th to the early 20th century shows that the pure-science ideal prevailed until the late 19th century when the cooperation between university scientists and industry started to become closer. The pure-science ideal was a legacy of the long-standing domination of philosophy in academic culture. Having worked hard to earn the status of academic disciplines, it was difficult for the natural sciences to overturn a notion of science that strove for eternal truth while ignoring the technical and economic fruitfulness of research. The fact that natural scientists continued to cling to the philosophical tradition, however, became a point of conflict in the late nineteenth century because the high social esteem enjoyed by the natural sciences was based primarily on their significance for technological innovation and economic success. German science had already begun to adjust to the new role of science in society on an institutional level, the conceptual distinctions between pure and applied science and between science and technology were set to blur in the early 20th century.

Science in the First Half of the 20th Century: Fundamental Research and the Promise of Utility

The scientific purity discourses lost importance around 1900 and new terms began to reshape the notion of science. This semantic shift suggests that the role of science in society had already changed. The German composite noun Grundlagenforschung (fundamental research), 7 is a relatively young term that first emerged in the early 20th century within a very specific context in the discipline of mathematics (Dingler 1911 : 35; Rulf 1913 ). In the late 19th century, mathematics underwent a disciplinary realignment known as mathematical modernism (Mehrtens 1990 ). German mathematicians played a leading role in this scientific movement, the main goal of which was disciplinary autonomy. The movement’s proponents created a special, self-referential language by freeing the discipline from any metaphysical grounds and providing mathematics with a theoretical framework that denied any reference to reality or other concepts in science and technology and favoured instead an intrinsic, formal logic. Journal articles such as “Mathematische Probleme” by David Hilbert ( 1901 ) delineated a future research programme for mathematics revolving around principal epistemic questions of proof. In summary, modern mathematicians created a new epistemic foundation for their discipline.

Although the role of applied mathematics was an issue for dispute within this reform movement, the term fundamental research was not actually used as an antonym that contrasted to applied mathematics. Within the particular context of mathematics, fundamental research denoted studies that contributed to solving fundamental logical problems like those Hilbert had put on the agenda. Herbert Mehrtens ( 1990 : 149) thus classifies fundamental research as a specific subdiscipline (“ Spezialdisziplin ”) within mathematics. Because this specific meaning was confined to mathematics, the term fundamental research first spread to adjacent disciplines such as philosophy and, in particular, the philosophy of science (Lewin 1922 ). In fact, the German version of fundamental research was not common throughout the 1920s and early 1930s, and the few times the term emerged, it referred mostly to fundamental epistemic questions within disciplines.

In contrast to the German scientific discourse, the English term “fundamental research” emerged slightly earlier and, more importantly, within a different context than in Germany. The English term basic research was initially less prevalent. Roger Pielke has detected a New York Times article from 1919 in which “basic research” emerged in the context of a Congressional hearing on agricultural policy. According to him, the concept was an offspring of the political discourse since its use was restricted to the political arena until the late 1930s (Pielke 2012 : 343). It must be added that “fundamental” and “basic” were, among other things, used as attributes to denote the core academic disciplines, such as physics, mathematics, or chemistry, upon which other disciplines were founded. Thus, fundamental science and basic science meant something completely different to fundamental research or basic research in the English/American context.

The initial use of fundamental research in fields such as plant breeding and technological or industrial research indicates that the term did not emerge from the 19th-century purity discourse. In the 1890s, scientists of agronomy at the American land-grant colleges called for more fundamental research in general aspects of plant physiology in order to continue making progress in plant breeding (Arthur 1895 : 360). Problem- and application-oriented research led them to new questions that “pure” botany had not yet raised. The land-grant colleges were the result of a federal initiative to foster education in agronomy and technology, and to offer higher education to the wider public. As a result of their agricultural focus, these colleges were provided with federally controlled land to establish agricultural experiment stations. Similar to the German technical colleges, the land-grant colleges were not originally on an equal footing with the universities in terms of scientific prestige (Thelin 2004 : 135–137). Yet researchers in these experimental centres faced high public expectations to provide results that could improve farming practices and increase crop yields (Marcus 1985 ).

The demand for more fundamental research expounded one problem: the uncertainty of scientific outcomes, even if a project had a clear task to fulfil right from the start. Given this uncertainty, doing fundamental research meant at least promising to lay a cornerstone for future technologies, new products, or new materials. If research failed to produce new knowledge proving useful, scientists could still legitimise their work via the ideal of pure science, that is the advancement of knowledge as a value in itself. As any reference to the intrinsic ideal of pure science was secondary, it served primarily as a back-up means of legitimisation and only secondarily as a way to claim recognition for applied botany among “pure” scientists. In the end, similar to the German example in engineering, scientists in applied botany declared the distinction between pure and applied science to be invalid: “All science is one. Pure science is often immensely practical, applied science is often very pure science, and between the two there is no dividing line” (Coulter 1917 : 228). Applied botanists called upon science to remain open to everyday needs and problems (Coulter 1919 : 366). Alongside these examples from botany, the term fundamental research can be found very early on in the context of technological and industrial research. Fundamental research denoted any scientific research revolving around basic technical problems with the goal of improving existing technology or, hopefully, developing new technology (Nutting 1917 : 250).

The fact that the concept of fundamental research arose in research fields with an explicit application-orientation reveals that the new term was not a synonym for pure science. Rather, it conveyed the promise that science would produce, sooner or later, useful knowledge. This semantic shift was a response to the growing expectations of science within society and the increasing number of possibilities that scientific research had been able to offer in the development of technology and other societal improvements since the late 19th century. However, researchers and scientists phrased their promise of utility very cautiously; the metaphorical meanings of “fundamental” express the idea that research is the first, but not the only step in a complex process. Hence, the strategic use of the term can be described as twofold: to promise utility and, at the same time, to confine expectations that may be far too high.

With respect to British science policy in the first half of the 20th century, Sabine Clarke ( 2010 ) has already pointed out that fundamental research did not emerge as a synonym for pure science. She shows that in Britain, the new Department of Scientific and Industrial Research, established in 1916, used the term first and foremost to stimulate industrial research. The new ministry was supposed to coordinate and support research that promised economic and social improvement. At first, manufacturers and scientists scarcely welcomed the new grants offered by the Department; according to Clarke, both parties wanted to avoid any kind of governmental interference. Confronted by this industrial opposition, the Department of Scientific and Industrial Research advertised long-term research projects dealing with the basic properties of materials or with technical processes with the new term “fundamental research”. In this particular context, the label pure science would have evoked the image of curiosity-driven research without any practical end.

As Clarke demonstrates, the new term can only be understood within its specific institutional and national setting; thus, we should not be too rash to conclude that the findings of the British study also apply to the German case. Furthermore, Robert Kline’s older study ( 1995 ) on the boundary discourse of pure and applied science in the US, which focuses on engineering and its relationship to the natural sciences, suggests that, even in the English-speaking world, the meaning of the term fundamental research varied greatly. According to Kline, the distinction between “pure” and “applied” had only become common in the 1870s, and so the ideal of pure science was a relatively recent phenomenon in the US. Although the demarcation between pure and applied science was becoming blurred in the interwar period, Kline argues that the majority of researchers in engineering eventually adopted the pure-science ideal in order to underscore their scientific capabilities and their growing professional status. Kline’s main argument is that because engineering was unable to assert an autonomous ideal of itself, technological knowledge continued to be subordinated to scientific knowledge in the 20th century. For Kline the new term fundamental research represented a modified ideal of pure science which could also apply to technology. Where engineering is concerned, Kline admits that he is unable to identify a clear strategy of autonomy forming an essential aspect of the traditional notion of pure science.

Nazi Opposition to the Notion of Pure Science

In Germany, the term Grundlagenforschung only became common in the sciences during the late 1930s. Its meanings certainly deviated from the original use of the concept within the context of German mathematics, as well as from the old semantics of pure science. After the scientific purity discourse ran out of steam in the 1920s, the National Socialist German University Lecturers’ League ( Nationalsozialistischer Deutscher Dozentenbund ), which represented the younger generation of lecturers attempting to bring the universities into line with Nazi ideology in particular, fought against the institutional, epistemic, and normative concepts that characterised the ideals of pure science (Nagel 2008 ). The Nazi discourse denounced the 19th-century humanistic notion of academia as a liberal bourgeois ideal that had permanently estranged science and scholarship from the German people.

On a detailed scale, the Nazi discourse criticized the older concept of science as being a selfish project pursued by scientists. This criticism was levelled at the epistemic norm of objective neutrality and the assumption that the natural sciences were unconditional – in particular in terms of the choice of research subjects – thus exposing the notion of pure science as a concept contrived by the ivory tower. Furthermore, Nazi critics blamed the self-referential concept of pure science for causing institutional fragmentation and disciplinary differentiation in science. Continuing the Weimar policy of collaborative research, the Nazi scientific ideal entailed joint efforts by researchers from different institutional and disciplinary backgrounds aimed at solving the problems of the day; problems that were, of course, defined by the politics of the Nazi regime. It is no surprise that the Nazi counter-concept of science quite openly called for a politicization of the academic world – in particular with regard to staff and research policy – and reinterpreted the ideals of universalism, academic freedom, and unity of science in light of the Volksgemeinschaft ideology (the ideology of the community of German people): academic universalism transformed into social universalism, which sought to overcome individual, institutional, and disciplinary interests. The political interpretation of freedom meant that science was in a position to contribute to the German people’s independence from foreign raw materials, in accordance with the Nazi quest for autarky. And lastly, by invoking the older ideal of the unity of science, they legitimized collaborative science, its different disciplines, and its various institutions in order to fulfil national tasks (Henkel 1933 ; Krieck 1933 ; Löhr 1938 ; W. Schultze 1938 ).

Research in Nazi Germany: Between Four-Year Plans and Long-Term Science Policy

In light of the official campaign against the old pure-science ideal at the beginning of the Nazi regime, the use of fundamental research in the late 1930s can hardly be understood as a new version of pure science presenting the search for knowledge of nature and truth both as an a priori goal of research and a value in itself. The terms Grundlagenforschung and Zweckforschung (goal-oriented research) gained hold as political efforts to acquire control over academic and industrial research increased. In 1937, the Nazi regime established a research council, the Reichsforschungsrat (Reich Research Council), which was responsible for funding research. During the war, the Research Council was directly responsible to the Army Ordnance Office (Flachowsky 2008 : 232–462).

The Research Council’s first president, military general and professor of army technology Karl Becker, defined fundamental research as science that could not be “commanded and accelerated”. He guaranteed, therefore, that “as far as researchers and facilities in the institutions [for fundamental research] in question offer even some guarantee of success”, there would be no interference from the Research Council (Becker 1937 : 26). Becker made particular mention of the various institutions for aeronautical research and the institutes of the Kaiser Wilhelm Society, promising to abstain from exerting any control over these institutions in light of their close relationships to industry. Goal-oriented research, which was meant to be built on fundamental research, was to fit into the schedule of the four-year plan. In this context, goal-oriented research denoted first and foremost industrial research leading to the development of advanced technology. Against the backdrop of the four-year plan, the Nazi regime demanded that industry give complete insight into its research activities (Becker 1937 : 25, 27).

In 1940, the Illustrierte Zeitung , a well-established illustrated magazine published in Leipzig, devoted an entire issue to the topic of German research in the service of the people in order to present Nazi science policy. The magazine included articles from leading scientists such as the biochemist and Noble Prize winner Adolf Butenandt, journalists specializing in scientific topics such as Hans Hartmann 8 , and ministry officials (No. 4956, 22 August 1940). To some extent, the issue was a response to continuing foreign criticism of the way the Nazis had incorporated German academia into National Socialism (Rust 1940 ; Hartmann 1940 ). Completely ignoring criticism of racist staffing policy, the articles presented a concept of science that responded to the needs of society without compromising scientists’ research freedom. “The freedom of research would not be endangered when the state ensures that state-funded institutes are given the task of conducting fundamental research in order to solve problems within the national economy” (Krauch 1940 : 122, translated by DS).

The articles, however, also addressed German scientists on the question of how a more utility-oriented research affected its institutional setting. The issue of organizing science in order to quickly achieve societal and technological progress without duplicating efforts in both academic and industrial research had already been under discussion within the paradigm of rationalisation prior to the Nazi’s seizure of power. From the late 19th century onwards, industry conducted more and more research in its own laboratories, and the good salaries attracted talented researchers. The future role of universities as training and research institutions and the initial division of labour between academic and industrial research thus became a vital question of science policy. Furthermore, the changing research practices also led to an organizational discussion about individual or team research. The terms fundamental research and goal-oriented research were part of these ongoing negotiations (Krauch 1941 : 2; Brüche 1944 : 114–115; Stadlinger 1944 : 227, 229; Verein Deutscher Chemiker 1943 ; Drescher-Kaden 1941 : 10, 16–17).

Overall, the articles in this special issue sought mainly to demonstrate to the public how German scientists, whose work was less visible, contributed to the nation during war time. Authors such as Butenandt tried to explain their ongoing experimental work in terms of both its meaning for society and its potential impact to a wider lay audience (Butenandt 1940 ). Following the initial hostility demonstrated by Nazi ideology towards the academic elite and elitist institutions such as the academies of sciences, this issue of the Illustrierte Zeitung promoted science wholesale by emphasizing that it was necessary for society to support research.

Within the natural sciences up to 1939, the new term “fundamental research” was rarely used and did not yet have an established, fixed set of meanings. In physics, for example, fundamental research could denote theoretical physics or, alternatively, it referred to the older distinction between the natural sciences and technology (Reichenbächer 1937 : 285; Hiedemann 1939 : V, 1). Despite this semantic variation, the strategic uses of the new term in most of the disciplines bore some resemblance to one another when it came down to combining the term with goal-oriented research. It is striking that as the term fundamental research became more widespread after 1939, scientists tended to mention Grundlagenforschung and Zweckforschung in the same breath (Witzell 1944 : 212–217). In fact, the term fundamental research emerged in the natural and technical sciences mainly when the individual field of research was close to application or demonstrated promise for military, economic, and political aims. This was the case, for instance, with innovations in weaponry and military strategy, maintaining public health, ensuring food supply, rationalizing the production and use of raw materials, inventing substitute materials, and encouraging industrial production. In the humanities, the term fundamental research was still less common. This observation leads us to the question of whether the use of the two terms really worked as a boundary discourse.

Interdisciplinary research fields, such as forestry, represented a utility-oriented notion of science in the first place. In the case of forestry, research promised more profitable cultivation and effective technical treatment of the raw material wood. Germany’s rise as a colonial power in the late nineteenth century had already transformed forestry into a politically and economically significant discipline, fostered since by the German state. In the Nazi war economy, the issue of raw materials, and with it the supply of wood, gained even greater importance (Steinsiek 2008 ). In this disciplinary context, fundamental research and goal-oriented research represented two equivalent sub-areas of forestry: one that studied the nature of the substance wood, and one that analyzed its material properties and the effects of technical treatment. The overall goal of both research fields was to acquire knowledge about the optimal use for the raw material wood (Runkel 1942 : 305–306). 9

The majority of scientists defined fundamental research as pursuing fundamental questions of nature, its substances, and its processes. This contrast to goal-oriented research still adhered to the old demarcation between nature, on the one hand, and society and its relationship to natural resources, on the other. But questions about nature, labelled by scientists as fundamental research in the late 1930s and 1940s, arose within the context of technical and practical problems (Kaiser Wilhelm-Gesellschaft zur Förderung der Wissenschaften 1939 : 322; Hoffmann and Suhr 1944 : 550), that is in applied science disciplines such as aeronautics, armament, forestry, plant breeding, and nutrition.

In the majority of cases, scientists simply stressed the necessity of both fundamental research and goal-oriented research, in other words, the general necessity of research for any kind of progress. This is where the views of the scientific community converged with the goals of Nazi economic policymakers, who were aware that the US and British governments were providing massive support to research for economic and military purposes (Krauch 1939 ). When it came to clearly defining terminology in this period, scientists surprisingly described fundamental research as the study of nature, devoid of any concrete notion of how it might be applied in terms of technology or societal utility. Yet having just drawn a distinction between utility-oriented research and research driven simply by the urge for knowledge, scientists immediately strove to emphasize that limiting fundamental research was not possible in terms of research practice and its institutional settings, whether in industry, in universities, or in other research institutions (Bauermeister 1938 ; Wolfgang Ostwald 1942 : 130–131; Niemeier 1944 : 106–107). Moreover, the distinction between fundamental research and goal-oriented research was often criticized as misleading because it suggested that fundamental research was far removed from any notion of useful application (Zenneck 1944 : 10; Endell 1942 : 113; Wolfgang Ostwald 1942 : 130–131).

These definitions must be seen as a vestige of patterns characterizing scientists’ former understanding of science. However, one question remains unanswered: if this differentiation of research types appeared to have little consequence for the scientific community, why did scientists introduce new terminology that could be understood as part of a dichotomy and that, moreover, was reminiscent of former boundary discourses? As the use of the term fundamental research was prevalent in engineering as well as in those research fields in chemistry, physics, biology, and geography that responded, in particular, to the concrete needs of the economy and the political regime, the intention was hardly to reactivate either the old demarcation between science and technology or the ideal of science for its own sake. The fact that new terms emerged reveals two things. Firstly, under the Nazi regime the scientific community felt the need to renegotiate the conditions under which science and research were conducted. Secondly, the old concepts of science no longer fit with existing practices in science.

The term fundamental research was fresh; Zweckforschung , which was highly unusual in the natural sciences until the mid 1930s, was even more so. 10 In fact, the latter only gained importance during the Nazi period. Some scientists explicitly considered goal-oriented research as a temporary focus of science responding to a situation of national emergency. In 1936, the chemist Wolfgang Ostwald, son of Wilhelm Ostwald and former president of the Kolloid-Gesellschaft (Colloid Society), stated that “[o]ver the last years, much has been said about so-called ‘Zweckforschung’. It means the entirety of efforts to draw more extensively than usual on scientific research for solving major economic problems” (Kolloid-Gesellschaft 1936 : 159, translation by DS). To be precise, the term goal-oriented research was spreading at the very same time that the creation of the new government administration in 1937, the Reich Research Council, institutionalised the four-year plan. This new authority and the second four-year plan that ensued broadened the field of activities in which science henceforth was understood as an important prerequisite for economic progress. Thus, in contrast to free research, goal-oriented research meant target-oriented research according to the goals of the four-year plan (Bachér 1937 ; Willing 1937 ). 11

In order to explain the emergence of this new scientific nomenclature, it is most revealing to look at the chronology governing the spread of new terms. With the foundation of the new Reich Research Council ( 1937 ), which claimed to bring German research efficiently into line with Nazi policies, some scientists were concerned about the future funding of fundamental research (Bauermeister 1938 : 476). It would be misleading to interpret this plea for fundamental research as a struggle for freedom of science that ignored the expectations of society in favour of absolute professional independence; the concerns expressed do not reveal an objection to the idea that science should serve political aims or national tasks. Yet the scientists’ worries certainly revolved around the question of how to govern science. The concern was, in fact, that scientific knowledge as a resource for innovation might dwindle in the long run. It can be described as an argument of knowledge sustainability meaning that knowledge will run short if scientists and policymakers align knowledge production exclusively with immediate needs. Within this sustainability discourse, fundamental research represented the experiences that, firstly, scientific progress was often unexpected and, secondly, that even the research output that sought practical solutions was unpredictable and needed time before its application was possible. According to these researchers, science had to conquer new ground deemed necessary for the long-term advancement of technology. Only a few scientists actually recognized the semantic shift in scientific nomenclature and criticized the new term fundamental research for constraining science to technological ends (Richter 1943 : 207).

The reference to the long-term and unpredictable nature of scientific research was, of course, not new. Back in the 19th century, this had already served as an argument in the science-technology boundary discourse. Yet it was not until the 20th century that this aspect of scientific and technological progress became an everyday experience in many fields of research. The problem of how to find the right balance between venturing into the unknown and, at the same time, abiding by a research policy that sought to keep the aims of research in its sights had been under discussion in the 1920s, in particular within the context of industrial research. Faced by the Nazi Regime’s four-year plan and the increasing pressure of the expectations on science during the war, it became even more important for researchers to communicate to the regime that their work contributed to political aims, even if they were unable to guarantee any immediate success.

The argument that research had its own temporal logic was also present in research fields devoid of a science-technology nexus. In 1943, Joachim H. Schultze, professor of geography in Jena, expressed the belief that science ought to be one step ahead of the demands of the day. He defined fundamental research as “general research regardless of its practical application and regardless of the benefits of everyday life” (J. H. Schultze 1943 : 197). He described research in geography as the task of depicting the overall research areas in the discipline, which included topics as diverse as the earth’s surface, landscapes, and the cultural and demographic depiction of countries. The central aim of Schultze’s article was not, however, to protect a self-referential concept of science, but rather to praise the utility of geography in general as well as the research carried out thus far for the purpose of warfare. Referring to historical examples of the huge political and economic interest in geography, Schultze argued that science, rather than being left to its own devices, needed both a societal mission and interest from the public. He advocated the idea of a central German geographical institute which would carry out fundamental and goal-oriented research for the state and for economic purposes. Schultze called for the combination of fundamental and goal-oriented research for an epistemic reason: research needs time and the future utility of scientific outcomes is not foreseeable as readily as future societal needs (J. H. Schultze 1943 : 201). Thus, the term fundamental research stood for sustainable knowledge with potential benefit, or a sort of stock of knowledge (Ziegelmayer 1936 : 253; Stock 1938 : 150–151; Brüche 1944 : 113).

The Discursive Strategy of Fundamental Research and the Reassessment of German Science in the Nazi Period

Over the last decade, German science and its research endeavours under National Socialism have undergone a historical re-evaluation, namely within two major projects on the history of the Kaiser Wilhelm Society and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation). The focus has shifted to some extent from the effects of Nazi ideology and the participation of the humanities, anthropology, and medicine in racist and eugenic policies, to the hard and technical sciences that contributed to the military and economic goals of the Nazi regime. Whereas former studies stressed the negative effects of Nazi science policy, such as, the international isolation of the German scientific community, the experience of being cut off from raw materials required by the experimental sciences and the focus on substitute research as a result of a policy of autarky, and, since 1933, the incredible loss of excellent researchers as a result of racist science policy, recent studies present a more differentiated picture of science under the Nazi regime when focusing on research output and technical innovation.

Despite the regulatory claims of the Reich Research Council and the German Research Foundation’s loss of autonomy, recent studies show that researchers were still able to shape research policy according to their own interests. Provided that researchers showed a political affinity to the Nazi regime, scientific reputation and peer review continued to define the allocation of research funding (Flachowsky 2010 ). In particular after 1942, the year in which the Reich Research Council was reorganized and military technical equipment assumed greater importance in the German war effort, it appears that the regulatory claims of German research policy finally gave way to a more efficiency-oriented policy. As Mitchell Ash puts it, normal science existed throughout the Nazi period (Ash 2006 : 34–35).

In this reassessment of German science and scholarship, the question of whether Nazi science policy led to a shift in focus from Grundlagenforschung (fundamental research) to angewandte Forschung (applied research) plays a crucial role. Recent studies provide evidence that fundamental research and applied research did not work as clearly demarcated, transdisciplinary, and supertemporal categories. Current studies on the history of the natural sciences during the Nazi period attest to a continuity of – what they call – fundamental research. Some studies suggest that German professors adjusted to the new conditions by combining applied research that accorded to political and economic requirements with fundamental research that earned greater appreciation within the scientific community in their projects. Although full professors apparently still honoured the ideal of pure science, most of them had contact with industry as individual consultants and/or via collaboration. Other studies identify fundamental research in especially applied contexts such as armament and defence research, but also in economically promising research fields such as metals research and polymer chemistry (Luxbacher 2010 ; Erker 2010 ; Flachowsky 2010 ; Epple 2002 : 318–322). In the case of metals research, Günter Luxbach differentiates between research on the composition of metal, which was labelled as fundamental research, and research that tested the technical properties of metals, which was known as applied research. In contrast to this classical distinction between the quest for knowledge of nature and the quest for technological progress, Paul Erker describes polymer chemistry as a discipline that strove to combine these two motives. Erker employs the label of basic research for a heterogeneous and innovative research policy. Thus the meaning of fundamental research differs in historical studies on the natural sciences, not least because these studies investigate different disciplines.

By countering older historical interpretations that see German science in decline since its political instrumentalization in 1933, the main thrust of these recent contributions is, of course, that the Nazi’s war and policy of autonomy did not cause the profile of academic research to change overall. The insight that fundamental research went hand-in-hand with goal-oriented research is a novelty only if we analyse science on the premise that basic and applied research constitute two fundamentally different forms of research. Most of the historians quoted above still do not question the distinction between basic and applied research. The long-established categories still appear to be so self-evident that these authors do not feel obliged to define them explicitly for the specific research fields upon which they focus. Moreover, most of them still fail to reflect on how scientists employed terms such as Grundlagenforschung and Zweckforschung during the Nazi period.

Only a few of these historians have reconsidered their analytic vocabulary in light of new evaluations of the Nazi period. Moritz Epple, for instance, no longer believes in the opposition of the terms basic and applied. As in recent propositions in the philosophy of science, he suggests that we should speak of application-oriented fundamental research within the context of Nazi science (Epple 2010 : 213). Another interpretation suggests that as German professors were increasingly involved in applied research, the use of the term fundamental research was merely symbolic, for the purpose of scientific reputation (Wagner 2010 : 26–27, 33). Surprisingly, semantic sensibility is on the rise when it comes to discussing the aftermath of the Second World War. Within this context, the use of the term fundamental research is more often identified as a simply rhetorical strategy deployed by German scientists in order to retrospectively downplay their involvement in the Nazi system. Carola Sachse argues that this strategy of moral relief also worked in the American context: it was supposed to dispel fear of German post-war science (Sachse 2010 : 480).

So far, this analysis of the first half of the 20th century has shown that the new terms fundamental and basic research initially emerged in mission-oriented or technical research fields. In Germany the concept only gained importance since the 1930s when research had to meet high political expectations. With regard to the historical context of the Nazi regime, the results suggest that the interpretation of a simply rhetorical strategy, whether as a strategy of individual moral relief or as a professional strategy for protecting a scientist’s guaranteed freedoms, is not entirely convincing. Because many German scientists demonstrated their commitment to the Nazi regime by offering their research services, the terms fundamental research did not serve to protect the old intrinsic ideal of science. In a period when the political expectations placed on science were high, the terms expounded instead the experience that scientific progress and procedures leading to exploitable results were difficult to predict.

From Knowledge Sustainability to Purity Discourse: US Science Policy Between the Second World War and the Cold War Period

As the rise of basic research as a pivotal keyword in science policy during the post-war era was not peculiar to Germany, it is now time for a more comparative perspective. Although the two terms fundamental and basic research had gained greater currency throughout the 1930s in US science and, more generally, in science throughout the English-speaking world than had the term Grundlagenforschung in German science, they had not yet spilled over into all the different disciplines. 12 Analysis of the journal Science demonstrates that, at that time, the use of these terms was still limited to biology (agriculture as well as studies on vitamins and proteins, which attracted pharmaceutical companies, also employed these terms), industrial research, and engineering. Once again, the terms denoted long-term studies focusing on fundamental problems in biology, chemistry, or physics emerging within the context of technical and application-related questions. The term fundamental research did not constitute an antonym to applied research; it was not part of a boundary discourse. In 1942, the research administrator of the US Department of Agriculture described basic research as follows:

In all these cases, either basic research precedes the practical applications of science, or a certain amount of this kind of research is found to be necessary somewhere along the line to clear-up obscurities that block further progress. … the point I am making is that in research there is no single road to practical results. If we keep our eyes constantly and exclusively on what seem to be immediate needs, we miss some of the richest fruits of scientific work – the fruits that grow from the discovery of important fundamental facts. … The emphasis I have given to basic research and freedom of inquiry does not mean that we should pay any less attention than we do to homely experimentation directed toward solving everyday problems. (Auchter 1942 : 287, 288)

In the case of engineering, the concept of fundamental research largely represented the ongoing process of the scientification of technology (Gibb 1937 : 233–234; Jewett 1944 ). Institutes such as the Mellon Institute of Industrial Research at the University of Philadelphia, which defined itself as a link between science and technology (or in the words of Edward Weidlein “between the world of science and the industry”), used the term fundamental research as a general label for their projects and training (Weidlein 1935 : 562).

In light of these examples, the argument that the new concepts reflected the increasing expectation that science should be beneficial to the economy and to society as a whole also applies to the US case. Scientists were aware of the epistemic and institutional challenges to research that the 20th century brought forth. As a result of the increasing commercial potential of fundamental research, patents became a major issue at US universities quite early on. In contrast to the German universities, where the right to hold patents appeared to be considered part of the individual academic freedom of German professors (at least until the rise of the Nazi reign), 13 the American land-grant colleges introduced patent regulations as early as the 1920s. Nevertheless, the administrative, legal, and ethical problems of patenting within institutions of higher education remained a controversial issue over the next few years (Potter 1940 ).

After dealing bit-by-bit with the shifting situation of science in the early 20th century, the Second World War marked an incisive and formative experience for the scientific community. When the US entered the war, the national mobilisation of science acquired the same level of importance there as in the other warring countries. In the early 1940s, the US debate on the effects of wartime revolved first and foremost around financial redistribution in science. The US universities, which depended mostly on private donations, were considered to be the losers in this process. In 1941, the long-standing idea of a federal fund that aimed to guarantee research funding on a regular and permanent basis was reignited. Although the financial crisis of the American universities had begun earlier during the Great Depression and bore several failed attempts to secure federal support for academic research (K. T. Compton 1934 ; Geiger 1986 : 246–255), proponents of this initiative blamed the war for worsening the financial situation of the universities and diagnosed a crisis in fundamental research (Blakeslee 1941 ).

Those advocating federal support argued that a new form of funding was necessary because research in the basic sciences, that is in basic disciplines such as physics or chemistry, laid the indispensable foundation for future benefits: “We are all familiar with the material conveniences and comforts which science has given us, but we often forget the original patient, fundamental research which made them possible and will be the basis for future advances” (Robbins 1941 : 8). As the concept of fundamental research had thus far denoted only research with a clear reference to application, the novelty here was the fact that the supporters of such a fund classified the entire endeavour of academic research at universities as fundamental research. Moreover, the lack of financial support for the universities was in opposition to the better funding of industrial and governmental research, which was only supposed to favour research that could demonstrate the prospect of immediate benefits (Robbins 1941 ).

From Wartime to Peacetime: Vannevar Bush’s Plans for Transforming Science Policy

During the war, scientists had discussed the future conditions of science (Science 1942 ). At the end of the war, plans for a new science policy were already on the table. In the literature on research and development policy, funding for basic research and the dissociative model of basic and applied research in the post-war era are still inseparably linked to the name Vannevar Bush (Braun-Thürmann et al. 2010 : 17). The MIT professor for electrical engineering served as presidential science adviser and, in particular, as chairman of the National Defense Research Committee and director of its successor organization, the Office of Scientific Research and Development. While coordinating the American military research programmes, including the Manhattan Project (the project devoted to constructing the atomic bomb), he began to make plans for a federal peacetime science policy. Based on the negotiations of four scientific committees (a Medical Advisory Committee, a Committee on Science and the Public Welfare, a Committee on Discovery and Development of Scientific Talent, and a Committee on Publication of scientific Information), in July 1945, Bush presented guidelines for future governmental promotion of scientific activity in the natural sciences and in medicine to the public. As well as providing financial support for academic research and junior scientists in the natural sciences, the proposals encompassed a reform of patent law and tax incentives for industrial research, the promotion of medical research, the plea for open science by fostering international exchange and strategies of declassification, and, finally, the sponsorship of basic research on military matters. Bush’s report “Science—The Endless Frontier” essentially sought to institutionalize federal science policy on a permanent basis (Bush 1945 ).

We should interpret his draft against the background of the war experience. The Second World War had demonstrated, once again, the importance of research for society and the fast-growing need for scientific knowledge. During the war, scientists and engineers had found that the search for technical innovation in the service of national defence spawned new questions and new problems for the natural sciences, the implication of which was long-term research. Given the immense expectations concerning immediate results within the context of warfare, some scientists feared that researchers would no longer be able to meet the demand of new knowledge for technical development (Simons 1943 : 391). Despite the achievements made during the war, researchers warned of an exhaustion and future shortage of scientific knowledge: only by exploiting existing knowledge, they claimed, had it been possible to invent penicillin and radar, two of research’s major wartime success stories. In other words, there was a fear that the equilibrium between the production of scientific knowledge and its application would be disturbed (Bush 1945 : 5, 8). The argument of knowledge sustainability thus became also important within the US community of scientists facing the war-time conditions of research.

This scarcity anxiety also applied to personnel resources in science (Barton and Burnham 1943 : 176; H. S. Taylor 1944 : 250). Bush’s report criticized the fact that, due to radical recruitment practices, the shortage of scientific personnel in the US was greater than in other countries (Bush 1945 : 19). Bush’s colleagues, such as the Nobel Prize winner Arthur H. Compton, believed that the training situation and the support afforded to fundamental research 14 at the universities were even worse in the US than in Germany (A. H. Compton 1945 : 208). A lack of scientifically trained researchers also posed a problem for science-based industry. Thus the four committees suggested programmes for fostering scientific talent that included the generation in uniform returning from the war, particularly through doctoral fellowships for basic research.

In spite of this crisis and the discourse of epistemic and personnel shortage, wartime research efforts had, after all, strengthened the position of science in society. As the US government had spent more money on science throughout the course of the Second World War than ever before (Bush 1945 : 82), scientists had a particular interest in perpetuating this federal commitment to science in peacetime. Since the US were traditionally characterized by less state intervention and a scientific infrastructure based largely on philanthropy and private donations, the federal support for academic research and training had been much lower than in Germany or in other European countries. It thus became necessary to legitimize the regular government funding envisioned through science’s role in the overall welfare of the nation. The Bush report justified the government’s obligation to support basic research in three ways. Firstly, medical research would improve public health. Secondly, research would advance the overall public welfare, which was almost synonymous with economic growth and job security due to innovations and new products. And finally, long-term civilian research promised to give the US a technological edge in armaments which was supposed to guarantee national security. Only then did basic research become a real keyword in research funding. And the metaphor of “basic” did the trick; by laying the basics for all kinds of future benefits, the federal government financed basic research as for the common good.

Bush’s proposal also reacted to the organizational conditions of wartime research, in particular with regard to security restrictions. The problem of secrecy policy had already been discussed openly during the war (K. T. Compton 1942 : 28). Bush’s report called for the prompt release of classified research after the war. This request also involved a secrecy strategy in which projects were split up into small, isolated research groups, each of which worked on a specialised problem without the opportunity for any kind of exchange between them. 15 It is worth mentioning that the work on these specialized problems within isolated research groups was sometimes denoted as fundamental or basic research during the war (Simons 1943 : 392), which indicates that Bush’s use of the term deviated from the former understanding. In order to near the ideal of open science once again, Bush believed that the federal government was also obliged to encourage publication, international communication, and cooperation following the war. In general, the report restricted the role of federal science policy to financial support and the provision of coordinating infrastructure (Bush 1945 : 22–24). Bush sought to prevent the government and the military from continuing to pursue the managerial approach to science policy they had applied in wartime. Bush criticized the military leadership for being too narrow-minded, a characteristic that did not fit with his understanding of the speculative and multidimensional nature of research (Reingold 1987 : 338–341). Failed attempts to establish federal research funding had already demonstrated that the majority of the scientific community disapproved of any governmental intervention in science (Geiger 1986 : 255).

Other scientists shared with Bush the rising concerns over free scientific exchange toward the end of the war (H. S. Taylor 1944 : 255; Jewett 1944 : 3), but Bush’s report was the first to link the relatively new notion of basic research with an institutional guarantee of scientific autonomy in such a close fashion. This also included his contrasting juxtaposition of basic research with applied research and development. By then, applied research had not been an antonym to basic research. Basic research thus not only meant that science should be freed from the burden of high expectations tied to immediately exploitable results; it also entailed the freedom of both inquiry and scientific communication. The different agendas and arguments – the strengthening of US universities as research and training institutions, the switch from war to peace, and attracting federal support for science in the name of national welfare – converged in the report’s recommendation to enhance the universities and non-profit research institutions as centres of basic research.

Following the release of the report, US scientists, politicians, and industry representatives entered into controversial discussions on various aspects of Bush’s proposals, which delayed the establishment of the National Science Foundation for nearly five years. The patent issue, the suggestion that the social sciences be excluded from federal support, the uneven distribution of excellent research universities in the individual states coupled with the corresponding problem of how to fairly allocate federal funding, and, finally, the envisioned scientific expertocracy within the federal foundation proved to be particularly delicate subjects. Bush’s proposal faced opposition, in particular from the military, liberal and democrat activists, and even from scientific colleagues. At the universities, which stood to benefit most from the funding, some scientists considered the plea for basic research as restricting their funding and research habits, which included contract research for industry or the army. There is no need to go into the details of this debate here since a mass of literature has already revealed these conflicting institutional interests and the political dimension of the controversy on science policy in the early years following the Second World War (Kevles 1977 ; Reingold 1987 ; Owens 1994 ; Zachary 1997 : 218–239, 249–260; Guston 2000 ; Dennis 2004 ). Most interpretations allude to Bush’s political conservatism aiming at the restoration of the pre-war political order. More generally speaking, studies on post-war research policy have so far presented a mainly political reading of these debates, which essentially revolved around the issue of more or of less intervention of the federal government into research. Even Roger Pielke’s ( 2012 ) current interpretation confines the concept basic research to this political dimension: in his view, the symbolic capacity of the term accommodated the conflicting parties, striving for the organization of science by the federal government on the one side and the autonomous organization of research by scientists on the other side, by promising potential utility.

What this analysis can add to the previous literature is a more nuanced interpretation of the conflicts within the scientific community. I argue that the scientific controversy over Bush’s “Endless frontier” partly stemmed from differences or even misunderstandings in the semantics of basic research. Although Bush developed the concept from the common discourse on knowledge sustainability, he added new semantic dimensions that had to meet multiple requirements of a new funding programme. In what follows, I demonstrate how the various problems in research organization and the overall political climate during the Cold-War period were turning this sustainability discourse by and by into a purity discourse.

Bush’s Definition of Basic Research: The Beginning of a History of Misconceptions?

Bush’s report marked a semantic shift in basic research that made a clear break with existing practices and notions in order to reorganize research in the post-war period. His specific use of the concept of basic research thus gave rise to misunderstandings and confusion. Moreover, the history of basic research in the second half of the 20th century has been characterized, in part, by these misunderstandings, which in the long run evoked anachronisms over which science and technology studies are still puzzling today. Bush’s short definition of basic research as “research performed without thought of practical ends” (Bush 1945 : 13), still singled out by most studies (Stokes 1997 : 116; Godin 2005b : 265; Popp Berman 2012 : 21), definitely contradicted the original understanding of basic/fundamental research in the context of application. Even more so, this reduction fails to represent the conceptual range of basic research in the report and the wider debates that took place right after the war.

Bush’s definitions of research, science, and applied science confused his peers and even one of his closest companions James Conant, who reflected critically on the new conceptual boundaries (Conant 1948 ). Representatives of national or military laboratories felt particularly compelled to argue against an institutional separation of basic and applied research. A member of the Naval Ordnance Laboratory, for instance, argued that “the naval laboratory programmes make it necessary for us to carry on basic research in certain parts of certain fields simply because no other agency is interested in, or has the facilities for, doing this work” (Bennett 1946 ). Bush’s peers in engineering were certainly confused by the different ways of denoting basic research. Universities specializing in the applied sciences and engineering, such as the California Institute of Technology (Caltech), understood basic research – inseparable from the overall pragmatic goal of inventing new technology – as an integral part of modern engineering (DuBridge 1959 : 109–110).

Following the publication of “Endless frontier”, scientists and other policymakers tried to differentiate and redefine the concept of basic research, which indicates that Bush’s understanding of basic research was not taken for granted and that researchers struggled with it because it did not fit the existing research landscape. John Steelman, science adviser to President Truman, for instance, divided basic research into two subcategories: firstly, fundamental research defined as “theoretical analysis … directed to the extension of knowledge of the general principles governing natural or social phenomena”, and secondly, “background research” defined as “systematic observation, collection, organization, and presentation of facts using known principles to reach objectives that are clearly defined before the research is undertaken to provide a foundation for subsequent research” (Steelman 1947a : 6). Others tried to introduce a distinction between “fundamental research, which leads to an understanding of the laws of nature, the discovery of new facts and laws, and the theoretical development”, and “basic research as it applies to industrial or military development involving basic studies of the fruits of fundamental work to determine their potentialities antecedent to application” (Leob 1946 : 540). An industrial researcher defined basic research as an intermediate category, which he located between pure research as “inquiry after knowledge for its own sake” and applied research as “the investigation carried out in response to immediate, direct, and obvious needs” (Spaght 1955 : 785). The gradual emergence of new variations such as “mission-oriented basic research” indicates, at least, that the criterion of intention, whether utility-oriented or not, became problematic in the long run (Tuve 1959 : 174; Kistiakowsky 1966 : 18).

While all these redefinitions can be interpreted as a claim to reintegrate application goals into the concept of basic research, Bush himself actually did not exclude the idea of mission-oriented research. Nathan Reingold sees “the pursuit of new knowledge” – and not the pursuit of knowledge for its own sake – as the real core of Bush’s notion of basic research. Reingold further refines his interpretation by quoting Bush’s argument that “there is no specification as to whether the knowledge is or is not of direct utility” (Reingold 1987 : 305). That sheds a very different light on the story of basic research. The importance of new scientific knowledge becomes even clearer if we take into account Bush’s metaphor of “the endless frontier”, which emphasized the cutting-edge role of scientific research. It thus placed scientific problems at the border of the unknown, reflecting the uncertainty of scientific outcomes and their long lead time in a world increasingly reliant upon scientific progress. Not least, Bush’s outline for a federal funding programme entailed financial support for basic research (long-range scientific research) on military matters.

Nevertheless, some of Bush’s contemporaries interpreted the report as an attempt to return to the old intrinsic ideal of pure science. They criticized the report for favouring a selfish notion of scientific autonomy that did not respond to any societal or economic needs (Shepard 1946 ). 16 My analysis thus far reveals that the discursive strategy of basic research initially aimed to acquire regular financial support from the government by promising utility in spite of the uncertainty of scientific research. This happened to conform to the interests of the scientific profession, so long as this support did not affect the ideal of open science. In the long run, however, the allegation of a return to an ancient pure-science ideal proved to be true. As the promotion of basic research continued, the sustainability discourse was transformed into a purity discourse, which revealed aspects of a past notion of science deemed to have been superseded in the 20th century.

After having become the spearhead of scientific endeavour, surprisingly enough American researchers looked back towards continental Europe. Post-war proposals for higher education in the US idealized the European university system and its humanistic tradition by associating it with democracy (Bender 1997 : 4–5). The old model of the European research university and its success story in basic disciplines such as physics and chemistry became a role model. According to Bush’s report, the arguments for reinvigorating the university within an increasingly pluralistic research landscape were twofold: immense need for scientifically trained researchers and the demand for scientific autonomy.

American scientists had repeatedly commented on the short-sighted focus of armaments research and on the threat Hitler’s regime implied for the freedom of science, presenting the liberal democratic order as the only safeguard for the fundamental principles of science (Fosdick 1934 : 380; Simons 1943 : 392; Goudsmit 1947 : XI). The debates on lessons to be drawn from the war experience, however, demonstrate that American scientists were chiefly worried about the threat to scientific freedom with regard to their own national conditions (H. S. Taylor 1944 : 255; Goudsmit 1947 : 232–246). Bush’s report blamed previous federal policy – from the Morill Land-Grant Colleges Act to the more recent practice of contract research – for being primarily interested in immediate benefits. The report argued that, due to an alleged inclination towards more utility-based research, the American nation depended entirely on the European production of new scientific knowledge (Bush 1945 : 2; see also Astin 1959 : 146–147).

By reproaching the societal and governmental focus on the utility of science, Bush’s sustainability argument was just about to tilt over towards the purity ideal of science. This explains why some scientists initially reclaimed the application aspect of research. The related aspect of academic autonomy, however, met with the approval of most scientists. Aside from the politicians supporting a science policy agency like the Democrat Harley M. Kilgore ( 1945 : 636), only a few scientists argued frankly against the anxiety about governmental interference by pointing out that the increasing social and economic demand for scientific research and the necessary political coordination of research in response to these needs were facts which scientists in the 20th century had to accept (Dunn 1945 ). Although the final establishment of the National Science Foundation turned out to be a compromise for all parties having negotiated this new form of federal science funding, Bush’s altered definition of basic research, in the end, became accepted.

At the end of the 1950s, after the National Science Foundation had been operating for several years, scientists continued to criticize the low federal base rate for basic research in comparison to that of contract research in the Department of Defense, which was twice as high (Elvehjem 1959 : 94; Waterman 1959 : 26–27). Some deployed the sophisticated argument that many projects were not truly basic research, but actually mission-directed basic research. In fact, the Korean War had meanwhile intensified the Cold War conflict and the Soviet’s launch of the Sputnik satellite turned the ideological competition between West and East into a science and technology race (Tuve 1959 : 173–176). As a consequence, basic research stood primarily for federally financed academic research – with or without any concept of practical use.

From the outset, the various drafts of the documents formally establishing the National Science Foundation included fellowships for graduates and junior scientists, so that the concept of basic research was closely linked to training scientific talent (Steelman 1947b : 29–30). From the late 19th century onwards, the modern research university inevitably moved further and further away from the traditional concept of a university as a specialized institution of higher education that excluded any notion of material benefit or practical aims. Yet the post-war debate on support for basic research led to a new version of the old boundary discourse of pure versus applied and theory versus practice. At a major symposium on basic research in May 1959, one representative of a private technical university reasoned that the

most difficult questions arise as to what is fundamental research, what is practical development, and which projects could be more appropriately done in commercial laboratories. … One useful criterion which helps many decisions in this field is that to be acceptable in any area a research program must be one which is consistent with and contributes to the educational program. This means it must be one in which graduate students can participate. This means, among other things, it must not be ‘classified’, either for reasons of trade secrecy or military security. (DuBridge 1959 : 109–110)

In the discourse among academic teachers, the ideal of training “good scientists” was not compatible with military or other contract research (Elvehjem 1959 : 94). Even engineering sciences felt compelled to adopt pure-science ideals whereby profit and research projects with self-serving interests should be taboo in institutions of higher education as long as they were part of scientific training. Given the fact that the growing number of military-related research projects at universities during the Cold-War years often included doctoral students, these statements certainly did not mirror the actual practice in the higher education of engineers (Dennis 1994 ). They rather seem to reflect the increasing uneasiness with the security guidelines related to contract research for the armed forces and the increasing number of military-related research projects.

The call for new knowledge through basic research in the post-war era also reached industry. Big companies such as DuPont or the Bell Telephone Company, which could afford their own well-equipped laboratories, intended to expand their participation in basic research after the war had ended (Fisk 1959 ). However, since economic rationales entailed selecting projects that were most likely to lead to innovation, these companies welcomed the idea of the federal government funding riskier projects to be carried out at the universities (Greenewalt 1959 : 130). After all, failures and deadlocks – all more or less inevitable parts of the scientific production of knowledge – would cause costs they wanted to avoid. Furthermore, industry representatives appreciated federal support for training the future generation of researchers they needed. This division of labour was financially promising for companies as “a technological savings account” (Greenewalt 1959 ).

While the amount of research carried out in direct response to economic and military demands had increased tremendously since the Korean War (Killian 1959a : 122), the university was meant to become a sort of reservation for long-term basic research within a changing research landscape. Academic freedom in the second half of the 20th century largely sought to liberate science from over-the-top societal expectations. Protecting scientific research “from the insistent demands of applied research” became a central argument deployed by scientists as well as industry and politics (Weaver 1959 : XIV; see also Greenewalt 1959 : 128). Yet what was initially intended to protect scarce knowledge resources could, in the long run, transform into an ideal of purity. The university was granted the status of a reservation in the midst of a rapidly changing research landscape in order to protect science against excessive expectations and thus guarantee the open development of scientific knowledge. With the status of reservation, however, also came the danger that research conditions be artificially conserved, making it difficult to respond to changes in scientific practices.

The Revival of 19th-Century Epistemic Norms and Virtues

The shift from a discourse of knowledge sustainability to a discourse of purity affected the epistemic concepts of science in particular and, in so doing, appeared to hark back to ideas coursing in the 18th and 19th centuries. First and foremost, this shift concerned the relationship between the natural sciences and technology. Although Vannevar Bush himself dealt with basic questions in mathematics – a central basic discipline in engineering – as well as with construction design in his own research, his proposals ended up reviving the old distinction between nature and technology because they made the distinction between engineering, on the one hand, and the natural sciences, on the other.

Historians have explained this distinctive position on the natural sciences with Bush’s personal concepts of administration and his ideas about achieving excellence in science through specialized researchers, based, of course, on the premise that the rationale of open science would guarantee the unhindered diffusion of knowledge for the benefit of technical progress (Reingold 1987 : 306–307). This relapse into outdated concepts of science, however, cannot be reduced to the personal preferences of Vannevar Bush. It should instead be seen as a broader academic phenomenon, which began as a move to counter the increasing demand on science for immediate benefits that reached its height during the Second World War, before finally turning into a political programme in the West, nestled within the ideological competition of the Cold War.

In order to protect basic research in the natural sciences, academic experts wanted these disciplines to steer clear of any kind of technical development. As Alan T. Waterman ( 1959 : 28) proclaimed in 1959, “the growing applications of physics, chemistry, and mathematics should be shifted to engineering departments and kept out of the regular science departments”. In other words, from the point of view of the natural sciences, applied research primarily meant research that sought to yield future technology.

An oceanographical study carried out within the context of naval research in the late 1940s and early 1950s reconfirms this one-dimensional understanding of applied research in contrast to basic research. The US Office of Naval Research was a staunch supporter of basic research in oceanography, yet the question of secrecy revealed that the Navy and scientists differed in their classification of basic and applied research and in their notion of utility. Oceanographers defined their investigations of the topographical features or meteorological conditions of the ocean as basic research as long as it did not expressly serve the development of technology destined for use by the Navy. The Navy, however, developed “a more sophisticated definition of basic research that would take its operational nature into account” and demonstrated strategic utility of geography for military purposes (Hamblin 2002 : 27).

This purification of the natural sciences even affected the existing research vocabulary. Science policy experts tried to find new labels for research fields in engineering formerly classified as fundamental or basic research. The term “analytical engineering” is a good example of this renaming practice (Killian 1959a : 122). Moreover, in the debates revolving around basic research in the post-war era, the whole attitude towards technology appeared to become more ambivalent. In the 1950s, the National Science Foundation still justified the support for basic research primarily by the goal of enhancing technical progress. At the same time, it became ever more common for statements on science to conclude with a declaration bearing the motivating force behind scientific endeavour; the pursuit of knowledge for its own sake and the quest for truth became the appendix of federal science policy (Waterman 1959 : 37–40; Astin 1959 : 154).

Researchers in innovation studies have associated post-war research policy with the “linear model”, that is with a linear trajectory from basic research in the natural sciences to technology (Edgerton 2004 ). Implicit in the new policy of basic research was a renaissance of the older epistemic notion of an asymmetry of knowledge and, by association, the scientific preference for research led by theoretical questions. Particular support for basic research in the natural sciences was grounded in the hope that a few basic discoveries would be sufficient to significantly broaden the potential for technological application (Elvehjem 1959 : 98). In the process of striving for the endless frontier of the unknown, the idea of major theories in the natural sciences came to be the ultimate driving force of scientific progress and thus a further argument for supporting basic research.

Even representatives of industrial research endorsed the orientation of academic research towards theory in order to provide mutual benefit:

[T]he existence of even a crude and preliminary physical theory and the heeding of it in the expectations and patterns of operation of scientific work would permit coupling of the individual, uncommitted, undirected researcher to the general objectives of economic and social programs. … In the still regrettably small list of findings from basic scientific research which have been quickly and directly connected with large advances in technology and useful operations are several important examples. In these, the really new idea came out because a unifying theory had displaced the true possibilities – the wide range of means rather than simply the ends themselves … (W. O. Baker 1959 : 54).

This hierarchical and linear notion of knowledge production contrasted with a more dynamic understanding of the relationship between fundamentally theoretical questions and approaches that started out from a concrete problem of application. Although the professional self-image of academic superiority certainly continued to have an effect on epistemic ideas and norms in the late 19th and early 20th centuries, shifting research practices had already begun breaking up this static epistemic model. As the special support of basic research and its distinctive position within the different research activities was beyond dispute in the late 1950s, representatives of industrial research or national laboratories only casually mentioned the mutual reinforcement of theoretical and application problems they encountered (Astin 1959 : 145, 151; Fisk 1959 : 160–161).

Debates on basic research eventually revealed another old epistemic ideal referring to the intellectual qualities of researchers and to research conditions that encouraged scientific creativity. New (federal) support for basic research initially focused on individual researchers in order to foster “the development of the individual scientist” (Waterman 1959 : 34; see also Weaver 1959 : XI; Greenewalt 1959 : 128–131; Morison 1959 : 230). Experts esteemed individual creativity as the main property of outstanding scientists, enabling them to move forward into the unknown. The free flow of unconstrained intellectual creativity was thus defined as basic research. Not least, the financial relief stemming from regular federal funding was well received as a guarantee of intellectual freedom (Tuve 1959 ).

This particular position was backed up by the revival of old academic virtues. “[T]ruly ‘basic research’ was driven by a passionate love for knowledge. Basic research thus meant ‘support for ideas’ in the first place” (Tuve 1959 : 174, 175; see also Waterman 1959 ). This definition of basic research tended to be averse to technology. Furthermore, the hierarchy of basic and applied research implied the moral superiority of academic research over benefit-oriented industrial research, even on the personal level of researchers (Elvehjem 1959 : 94–96). In the end, the epistemic virtue of disinterestedness – according to Robert Merton one of four imperatives of modern science – got mixed up with social and moral values.

This deep appreciation of individuality was partly a reaction to the growing experience of scientific teamwork, which had become common within large military or industrial research projects. Individual creativity contrasted with the conservative atmosphere of research groups, which tended to object to fresh, radical ideas (Waterman 1959 : 30; Tuve 1959 : 176). Even those involved in industrial research highlighted the advantage of academic research because companies were only able to offer limited space for the individuality of their researchers. Furthermore, the freedom of investigation was supposed to be a special incentive for academic research – an incentive that had to compete with the high salaries and the technologically well-equipped laboratories in industrial research (Elvehjem 1959 : 96–97). Praise for individuality in science, however, derived partly from the ideological value of individualism in Western civilization. The first director of the National Science Foundation, Alan T. Waterman, put it like this: “Surely one of the great assets of democracy is the encouragement of individual initiative” (Waterman 1959 : 25).

Democracy at Risk: The Ideological Role of Basic Research in the Cold-War US

The ideological potential of the basic-research concept contributed significantly to the shift from a discourse of sustainability to one of purity. Politicians, for example US President Dwight D. Eisenhower, translated the new science policy directly into political slogans such as “Science: Handmaiden of Freedom” (Eisenhower 1959 ). Politicians still placed great hopes and expectations on science as the pacemaker of technical progress, capable of securing national security, national welfare, and prosperity. At the same time, their support of basic research enabled politicians to praise academic freedom as an overall value of liberal Western society. In addition to this, federal funding for basic research, defined as support for individual initiative and creativity, symbolized the individualism within democracy (Waterman 1959 : 25). As a collective symbol bridging the gap between scientific and public discourse by the polysemy of metaphors, basic research offered a true ideological surplus. Politicians further contrasted the “limited or local application” within mission-directed research with the universality of basic research designed to “benefit all mankind” (Eisenhower 1959 : 137). Leading the technological race with the launch of its Sputnik satellite, the Soviet Union then stood for an application-oriented understanding of science in the service of communist goals, whereas the Western argument pertaining to the universality and openness of basic research claimed ethical superiority.

During the 1950s, this high praise for free basic research stood in opposition to the high percentage of projects funded by the military and the increased demands for secrecy imposed on large areas of research in physics or other fields relevant to military projects by US security policy. It is telling that, in 1951, Alan Waterman, first director of the National Science Foundation and former technical director of the Office of Naval Research, emphasized the role of science in the situation of national emergency in the wake of conflict with the communist world; in spite of the National Science Foundation’s basic research programme, he underlined the need for science to focus on urgent application problems (Waterman 1951 ). According to the literature (Forman 1987 ; Westwick 2000 ), patriotic mobilization among scientists was still high. Many classified their research voluntarily, or adjusted to political pressure for security by compartmentalizing research and forming classified communities. Although these strategies were supposed to guarantee as much scientific exchange as possible, secrecy meant that research largely took place within a national context.

Moreover, the debates in Science during the 1950s demonstrate that the secrecy policy and the effects of a dominating military grip on science gave more and more cause for concern within the scientific community. Scientists criticized the idea that the military had a “sophisticated understanding of the needs of basic research”, arguing, moreover, that “those branches of pure science that lack military appeal are as badly off financially as they ever were” (Phillips 1952 : 440). In the early 1960s, military or military-related institutes, such as the Office of Naval Research, were still financing most academic research, in particular at prestigious universities (Leslie 1993 ). Against this backdrop, the political reading of basic research was not merely an aspect of portraying the US as a liberal society to the outside world. The debate on basic research also reflected, more controversially, the internal effects of the cold war on research. The debate was embedded in a more general intellectual discourse on the consequences of the predominant security policy and the growing power of the military for democratic society (see, for example, Shils 1956 : 176–191).

Eisenhower’s statements demonstrated this growing ambiguity. In his well-known “Farewell Address” from 1961, the departing president, former supreme allied commander and president of Columbia University, warned against the growing power of a “military-industrial complex”:

[W]e must guard against the acquisition of unwarranted influence, whether sought or unsought, by the military-industrial complex. The potential for the disastrous rise of misplaced power exists and will persist. We must never let the weight of this combination endanger our liberties or democratic processes. We should take nothing for granted. Only an alert and knowledgeable citizenry can compel the proper meshing of the huge industrial and military machinery of defense with our peaceful methods and goals, so that security and liberty may prosper together. (Eisenhower 2003 : 414)

Eisenhower construed financially attractive contract research as a threat to the academic “fountainhead of free ideas”. More importantly, he warned against the menace to public policy and civil society of a new “scientific-technological elite” (Eisenhower 2003 : 414–415). Although Robert Merton had already stressed the similarity or affinity between open science and Western democracy, in the late 1950s and early 1960s Eisenhower and other politicians identified science as a threat to democracy when a close connection between science, the military, and the economy remained intact (Wang 1999b ).

Along with the attribute of universality, another of Merton’s four imperatives of modern science, the notion of truth also gained importance in this ideological discourse (Waterman 1959 : 39). The ideal of truth had already been part of the ideological fight against fascism during the Second World War when researchers emphasized that science offered more than technical applications: “American science therefore has an especial duty to keep aflame the torch of free research for truth, which is dimmed or gone out in so many lands” (Blakeslee 1940 : 592).

As the natural sciences had needed a long time to set themselves apart from an understanding of science dominated by natural philosophy, the revitalization of the idea of universal truth appears anachronistic. In the 19th century, the natural sciences developed a mechanical and structural understanding of objectivity based on methodological processes that sometimes even stood in contradiction to the quest for truth and certitude (Daston 2000 : 32–34). At the beginning of the 20th century, the quest for truth had something old-fashioned about it in a scientific era in which research was constantly doing away with established certainties.

Coping with Ethical Dilemmas in the Cold-War Era

During the Cold War, however, the attributes of truth and universality were revitalized and became part of an effort to present science as a politically and ideologically independent authority in society. From the viewpoint of politics, science was able to act as a neutral authority upon which decision-makers could rely (Price 1962 : 1105). Scientists themselves praised the idea “that science has something more valuable than its material gifts to offer. … Science can have no dogma, no arbitrary authority, no ‘party line’” (Sinnott 1950 : 125). Scientific virtues of “objectivity, tolerance, reluctance to distort or suppress evidence, and willingness to accept sound logic and demonstrable fact” were transformed into political virtues (W. P. Taylor 1953 : 449). At the same time, however, the position of impartial experts tended to be morally overloaded when scientists were meant to become missionaries of “reason and good will” in the fight against “falsehood and hate” (Sinnott 1950 : 126; see also Szent-Györgyi 1957 ; Rapoport 1957 ; Weaver 1961 : 259). In fact, the democratic framing of basic research and the revival of knowledge ideals in the tradition of Humanism led to a politicization of science and, as a result, basic research itself became part of ideology, namely Western ideology.

Historians have already pointed to the various ideological dimensions of science in the post-war period (Wang 1999a ; Ash 2006 : 30; for the social sciences and humanities, see Bender 1997 ). Some scholars from science and technology studies blame Robert Merton’s comparison of science in democracy with science in fascist and communist regimes for the misconception of scientific ideals such as autonomy and universality, a misconception that they have been trying to correct ever since (see the overview in Daston 2000 : 18–20). But the societal, political, and ethical implications of the basic-research concept were embraced by the scientific community, even without sociological mediation.

After the atomic bomb was dropped on Hiroshima, the role of science in society certainly became more contradictory (Conant 1961 : 6–13). While researchers had wholeheartedly praised the salutary benefits of science before Hiroshima (A. H. Compton 1940 : 56), contemporaries noted afterwards that the “atom bomb once and for all explodes the ‘neutrality’ of technology” (Shepard 1946 : 66). The promise of progress was only one side of the coin. Scientists became increasingly aware of the burden of responsibility in their own research. Some of them hoped to avoid this problem by pursuing more theoretical research topics. Others tried to take political action, such as the atomic physicists’ movement, which fought for civilian use of scientific knowledge and technological invention. But the anxious atmosphere during the Cold War period – anti-communist harassment and the increasing public fear of a new scientific-technological elite – aggravated the ethical dilemmas of post-war science.

A statement made by the physicist Julius Robert Oppenheimer, a leading figure in the Manhattan Project, about the debate on basic research indicates scientists’ uneasiness when they were faced with these dilemmas: “The argument that the quest for new knowledge, which is basic science, is ennobling, and the argument that the quest for new knowledge produces new knowledge which is useful to technology and thus to practice, are disturbingly separate and unrelated arguments. … Yet science and technology are symbiotic” (Oppenheimer 1959 : 9; for a similar argument, see W. O. Baker 1959 : 43–47). Oppenheimer seemed to suspect that the debate on basic research simply reflected these modern dilemmas. It is striking, but also telling, that he tried hard to avoid the dualistic semantics that characterized science policy at this time. Oppenheimer explicitly raised the political problems brought about by the powerful scientific culture of the 20th century. Taking the growing criticism toward scientists into account, the physicist believed that making the public understand research goals had become difficult. While the impact of science on society had increased tremendously, the fast growth of scientific knowledge and technical innovations made it hard for laypersons to judge issues in science policy. Oppenheimer feared that this asymmetry of knowledge between experts and the lay public weakened democratic political decision-making (Oppenheimer 1959 : 12–13). 17

The charges brought by the McCarthy Committee in 1954 against Oppenheimer relating to his opposition to the hydrogen bomb illustrate that scientists who were willing to assume responsibility for their research by taking political action had to learn the hard way that there was little room in the political climate of the Cold War to deal openly with these dilemmas of modern science (Bird and Sherwin 2005 : 462–550). With regard to scientists of the progressive left advocating a more utility-oriented notion of science, Jessica Wang notes that “[a]lthough their views on the structure of postwar science were not directly responsible for their political difficulties in every case, these scientists and others who embraced a liberal-left politics of science were likely to hold other views that made them vulnerable to anti-communist attacks and excluded them from political influence” (Wang 1995 : 166). In the mid 1950s, the National Science Foundation and the Academy of Science included the criterion of national loyalty into their peer-review system for unclassified research. Both organisations thus sought to avoid allegations of supporting researchers who were suspected of sympathising with communist ideas (Waterman 1960 : 127; Committee on Loyalty in Relation to Government Support of Unclassified Research 1956 ).

The question of loyalty arose especially when it came to discussing technological application, as an official statement by the President of Associated Universities addressed to the Committee on Government Operations confirmed:

If a scientist expresses a strong view on some technological matter that may be contrary to the application of technology to current or to subsequent policy, he is open to the accusation of taking this view with the intent of deliberate subversion. … Moreover, secrecy prevents him from stating the essential technical grounds on which his view is based. Therefore, in the simple process of doing his job for his country well, he is open to damaging criticism against which he is permitted to produce little defense. (Berkner 1956 : 784–785)

Given this pitfall, the discursive separation of science from technology provided a strategy to avoid the risk of being forced to go “politicking”, which gradually came to be considered as the “disease” of the project research dominating American universities at that time (Gates 1958 : 234).

In this particular situation (the ethical dilemmas of the techno-scientific world, the fragile relationship between science and the public in democracy, and the ideological antagonism during the Cold War), the dissociation of the natural sciences from applied research and any practical application of scientific knowledge was thought to offer a strategy of individual, professional, and institutional relief: Firstly, a strategy that avoids assuming ethical responsibility for the changes caused by scientific knowledge. Secondly, a sort of self-protecting strategy that sought to avoid the direct line of political fire in a society entirely concerned with national security, the latter which produced an atmosphere of suspicion. And thirdly, a strategy of political neutrality and independence from any self-serving interests as a means of guaranteeing the institutional freedom of academic science and a self-regulating scientific community which, from a scientific point of view, was best capable of dealing with the open and often unpredictable process of epistemic progress. The scientific community retreated into a “satisfactory philosophy of ignorance”; as long as science was defined as institutionalized scepticism, it was still possible to maintain the belief in science or scientific knowledge as an indispensable value of modern civilization (Feynman 1955 : 15).

Conflicting Promises and Their Effects on the Public Image of Science

This neutral position secured the federal funding of research at universities in the US – something the universities had longed for since the 1920s. In return, academic researchers promised simply that science would lay the foundation for progress. They also offered their expertise to politics, thus acting as an independent authority over truth in a pluralistic, democratic society. The certainty academic scientists offered appeared to be especially welcome at a time in which society was driven by great anxiety. With regard to the outside image of the US during the Cold War, the universities’ role as reservations devoted to autonomous science served as a symbol for Western liberal society in the tradition of Humanism amid the great ideological competition, while simultaneously providing fig-leaf camouflage for the technology-based arms race. The post-war understanding of scientific autonomy was, in fact, the result of a broad process of the politicization of science arising from the growing importance of scientific knowledge for society.

Since there is, by definition, no clear solution for dilemmas, the strategy of basic research inevitably caused problems for the relationship between science and the public in the long run. Articles on this relationship and on topics such as the responsibility of science in the late 1950s show that public mediation between the needs of science and those of society became increasingly problematic (Killian 1959b : 136; Sayre 1961 ; Price 1962 ). According to Bender, this understanding of the autonomy of science, in particular the position of elitist experts and how they neglected their responsibilities, alienated science from society, evoked the impression of an academic ivory tower, and, finally, ended in federal budget cuts for academic research (Bender 1997 : 8–12).

Moreover, I argue that the simple promises of truth and progress scientists had avowed to society covered the complexity and uncertainty of research dynamics as well as the tentativeness of contested scientific knowledge. Moreover, the authority of scientific objectivity and methodologically certified knowledge revealed its limitations during political negotiations on values and societal goals; the position of moral neutrality might bewilder the public. It could thus lead to disappointment, misunderstanding, and even to the loss of science’s integrity in the public sphere. Furthermore, the increasing interlocking of technology and the natural sciences was also hidden behind praise for basic research. Since technological innovation had become part of the natural sciences, questions of risk and utility had inevitably arisen and transformed themselves into political and ethical issues: Who will profit from the results? How do we manage risks?

Only few researchers at that time anticipated that the excessive expectations of and contradictory demands on research might turn the public against science (W. O. Baker 1959 : 48; Dryden 1954 ). The shift from a discourse of knowledge sustainability to one of purity meant that the concept of basic research itself sent contradictory signals to the public: “The uneasiness of scientists on this score is revealed by the observation that, whereas they claim among themselves that their primary interest is in the conceptual, not in the applied, aspects of science, in public they justify basic research by asserting that it always leads to ‘useful’ results” (Dubos 1961 : 1209; see also Daniels 1967 ).

In fact, the concept of basic research and the underlying linear model of innovation had already come under attack in the late 1960s and early 1970s. The long-term and highly speculative nature of scientific research was difficult to communicate to a public that expected economic prosperity and welfare here and now. Society’s disappointment backfired on the scientific community and stimulated a debate about the appropriateness of dissociating basic from applied research (Abelson 1966 ; Reagan 1967 ). Yet this crisis is another chapter in the conceptual history of basic research and goes beyond the scope of this paper.

Despite recurring crises, the concept of basic research functioned as a collective symbol for science policy over quite a long period of time. Moreover, the semantics of the new US science policy spread across the entire Western world. Ever since the National Science Foundation established a periodical survey of overall research in the US based on the categories basic research, applied research, and development (the final stage of innovation, when technologies or ideas are turned into marketable products), nearly all countries in the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) adopted this classification (OECD 1976 ). Basic research and its corresponding categories were converted into enduring statistical realities that played a crucial role in budget planning within industry and in funding allocation undertaken by government bodies (Godin 2005b ).

Fundamental Research in the Federal Republic of Germany: A Brief Overview

In the Federal Republic of Germany (hereafter referred to as West Germany), fundamental research also became a key concept in science policy. The impact of the American role model on West Germany is quite obvious. Within the context of re-education and development programmes, those representing US science promoted their concept of science in democracy with its special focus on fundamental research in West Germany (Conant 1953 ; Bush 1954 ). However, the national characteristics of the German research landscape coupled with the historical burden of the Nazi past meant that the way fundamental research and its corresponding discourses were implemented differed to a certain degree from the American experience. I will briefly mention some of these Germany-specific characteristics in order to maintain a balance between the two national perspectives.

After the Second World War, the Allies assumed control of science in Germany with the intention of suppressing all further research activities relevant to the development of armaments. Allied Control Council Acts and the ensuing executive regulations specified by each of the Western occupation zones forbade any fundamental or applied scientific research with military relevance (Frowein 1949 , 1950 ). 18 It is remarkable that the crucial criterion for prohibition was the military potential of research projects rather than the difference between fundamental and applied research.

Similar to the American reaction to Bush’s proposals, discussions within the German scientific community over the dissociation of basic from applied research were quite controversial in the initial post-war years. Those from engineering or the applied sciences were particularly confused by this distinction and felt insecure about their future position and status within academia (Vieweg 1950 : 731–732; Sörensen 1952 : 158). The creation of compounds such as “applied fundamental research” ( angewandte Grundlagenforschung ) was a further German strategy designed to overcome this confusion in engineering (Heiss 1950 : 121, 127; Wever 1952 : 1053).

In order to cope with the Nazi past, the concept of pure science was initially more attractive because of its moral connotation in the sense of innocence. Many scientists labelled their research activities during the Nazi period retrospectively as pure science in order to avoid being accused of complying with and supporting the former fascist regime (Mehrtens 1994 ). In general, the revival of ideals belonging to the 19th-century concept of pure science was more extensive than in the US. Reference to the Humanist notion of education became part of the programme to democratise society. This notion thus shaped the self-understanding of German universities, which culminated in a re-glorification of the Prussian university reformer Wilhelm von Humboldt, who had emphasized the educational function of science. 19 German professors embraced the older scientific ideal of truth-seeking as the ultimate motive for research. 20

Right after the war, German academics tried hard to avoid the impression that their research was driven by any political or economic interests. The US occupying forces certainly wanted to keep science at a great distance to politics, but they did not seek to suppress economically and technically promising research (Cassidy 1996 : 200–206). In fact, the growing tension with the Soviet Union meant that the Marshall Plan’s aim was speedy economic recovery in both Germany and Western Europe. John Krige has already pointed out that the basic-research concept played a key role in reconstructing European science under “American hegemony”. Firstly, the concept was important for communicating the US financial support for the former wartime enemies towards the American public. Secondly, it transported the Western ideology and was therefore part of the envisioned democratization process in central Europe. Finally, the US promoted basic research as unclassified research in the allied countries in order to increase its stock of scientific knowledge and thus to secure the American technological leadership (Krige 2010 ).

The German discourse on the general role of science in society defined scientific knowledge primarily as a cultural good in order to strip off the Nazi past: science was given a religious appeal (Walden 1946 ; Rein 1946 ; Reppe 1950 : 1; Erbe 1954 ). However, even scientists such as the physicist Otto Hahn, who argued strongly that research in the natural sciences should abstain from any economic or technological considerations, advocating instead that science ought to be driven by the thirst for knowledge, campaigned for research funding by highlighting examples of scientific discoveries that eventually led to successful products or innovative technology (Hahn 1949 , 1954 ).

Overall, the German concept of fundamental research resembled the US one in many ways. It encompassed the idea of the long-term perspective and that of the scientific knowledge reservoir or resource, the demand for scientific talent, individual creativity in research, and the belief in disciplinary specialisation (see, for instance, Reppe 1950 ). Eventually, fundamental research also became the key concept in the public funding of research in West Germany. However, the Germans’ attempt to institutionally dissociate academic research from research promising primarily economic utility was much more radical than in the US. West Germany founded the German Research Foundation in order to fund academic research and the Fraunhofer-Gesellschaft (Fraunhofer Society) as the funding body responsible for economically relevant research.

The German Research Foundation’s crucial criterion for defining fundamental research was the institutional autonomy of academic research. According to a number of historical studies, this particular focus on fundamental research implied a restoration of the power held by full professors (known in Germany as the Ordinariensystem ). As the demands for the applied and the technical sciences were growing continually, in 1956, the German Research Foundation also established a special commission for funding applied research. However, this commission failed to gain importance as an instrument for promoting research in the technical sciences (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1956 ). Technical universities therefore had to look for financial support from another quarter. In the end, the ideal of fundamental research in West Germany seemed to slow down the institutional emancipation of technical colleges from universities. Furthermore, academic research lost contact with expensive, major scientific projects carried out in publicly funded research institutes (Orth 2011 ).

All in all, the shift from a discourse of sustainability to one of purity after the Second World War appears to have been a transnational process, although both the background and the intensity of the purity ideals in the US and in West Germany differed from one another. In both nations, the purity discourse implied a revival of scientific ideals dating back to the 19th century. West Germany adopted the American imperative of basic research, but German scientists referred more extensively to the Humanist tradition of academia because they had to dissociate themselves from their Nazi past. As universities were supposed to play an important role in Germany’s effort to progress towards democracy, academic science was defined by primarily educational ideals. The call for basic research after the Second World War in the US initially sought to maintain federal funding for academic research in order to enable scientific talent to flourish without it being subject to pressure from the expectation of benefit held by society. Basic research became a key concept in US federal science policy because the latter defined science as a common good with a long-term perspective. However, the fact that this key concept became crucial in the long run can be explained only by the fact that it functioned as a discursive strategy designed to cope with the political and ethical dilemmas of science during the Cold War.

Conclusions

This article has sought to demonstrate the importance of an historical approach in order to, firstly, understand the complex meanings of basic research and, secondly, answer the two questions of why science policy revolved around the concept of basic research and its dissociation from applied research for such a long time, and why this is still such a hot topic in science and technology studies today. If we continue to describe basic research as a timeless, clearly definable mode, even as an ideal type of research in contrast to applied research, we completely overlook the reason why this key concept in modern science policy emerged at all. In fact, this study has shown that the term basic research cannot be seen as a simple synonym for the older notion of pure science. As a consequence, the assumption made in social-scientific studies that the ideal of basic research structured modern science continuously up until the postmodern era, when application-oriented research was thought to gain predominance, needs to be corrected.

As the term basic research emerged in the early 20th century and became more common only in the late 1930s, it is actually quite young. Basic research is best described as a collective symbol of science policy designed to bridge the gap between the desire to support research, despite the fact that scientific output is unpredictable and that the expectations placed upon science by society have been growing constantly during the 20th century. For the history of basic research, it is crucial to note that the concept itself (as well as similar terms such as fundamental research) initially emerged in both the natural sciences within research fields that pursued explicitly practical ends and subdisciplines of engineering that targeted technological innovation and improvement.

While science profited financially from society’s growing demand for research, researchers simultaneously faced pressure from society’s expectation that science should produce immediately exploitable knowledge. In deploying the concept of basic research, scientists promised the public that research would lay the ultimate foundation for all sorts of progress and innovation, while at the same time conveying the experience that scientific research was time-consuming and its outcome and technical applications were hard to predict. Until 1945, basic research primarily meant long-term research in the natural sciences that was ultimately expected to solve practical problems.

After the Second World War, basic research became a central concept of US science policy, which particularly promoted research at universities and non-profit research institutes. Although the scientific promise of progress remained an important message in this concept after 1945, the discourse revolving around basic research shifted considerably in the post-war period from a discourse of knowledge sustainability to a discourse of purity. During the war, scientists had learnt to value massive governmental support of research, but they were concerned that the short-term planning of war-related research and its security restrictions would put the sustainability of both scientific knowledge and manpower at risk in the long run. Believing that scientists knew best when it came down to making science flourish and knowing what it took to explore the unknown, the challenge for scientists was legitimizing the continuance of federal science funding while at the same time advocating the institutional autonomy of science.

To this end, science policy advisers such as Vannevar Bush revived a long-lasting semantic reservoir of scientific ideals. By dissociating scientific knowledge from its potential applications, it became possible to define academic research as a common good capable of laying claim to federal protection, just as the older concept of pure science had done before. Bush’s proposal focused on the natural sciences, whose studies in the fundamental principles of nature were thought to offer nearly endless possibilities for technical innovation. Moreover, this new definition responded to the educational tasks undertaken by universities in which research projects were part of scientific qualification. The self-concept of higher education institutions traditionally kept their distance from any utilitarian aspects of scientific knowledge.

The distinction between basic and applied research thus served, first and foremost, as a criterion governing the allocation of federal funding, implemented through the newly founded National Science Foundation. Although the majority of researchers were grateful for the new federal support for research, the concept of basic research became the subject of controversies in the late 1940s because it reanimated ideals and norms of the older, European discourse of pure science. With these semantic references, basic research evoked older epistemic and social hierarchies. Research was seen to be more theory- than problem-oriented, the natural sciences assumed moral superiority over the technical sciences, and academic researchers were considered morally superior to industrial researchers. The individual pursuit of knowledge ennobled academic researchers, who became detached from immediate demands so that scientific creativity was given free rein.

To a certain extent, the re-establishment of older scientific ideals was a reaction to the exceptional conditions of wartime research. In many research fields, however, the ideals belonging to a former notion of science contradicted the changed practices in and demands placed on research in the 20th century. In particular, the idea of keeping technology apart from the natural sciences, which derived from an artificial funding demarcation, appeared anachronistic. These social and epistemic attributions of basic research looked like a cultural lag in modern science.

The reason why the concept of basic research, with all its reminiscences to former purity discourses, finally prevailed was that it functioned as a discursive strategy to cope with the difficult relationship between science and the public, the ideologically charged atmosphere of the Cold War, and the ethical dilemmas in science during the second half of the 20th century. When it comes to the political dimension of the concept of basic research, there are usually references to Robert Merton or Michael Polanyi, who stated that only democracy guaranteed full scientific autonomy and that, vice versa, scientific independence was a prerequisite of democratic pluralism because it presented a disinterested authority of truth (Merton 1942 ; Polanyi 1962 ). This self-image of science as being autonomous and disinterested was partly a result of Western ideology competing with the Soviet Union during the Cold War.

The effects of the Cold War on domestic politics were a major challenge to the scientific community. The first use of the atomic bomb rendered discussion of the goals of science unavoidable. Scientists who took part in the debate about the application of scientific knowledge for good or bad discovered that there was little room for negotiation in Cold-War America. At the same time, the relationship between the scientific community and the public became ever tenser as cooperation between science and the military increased. American intellectuals perceived this military-science nexus as a threat to US democratic culture. As a consequence, resorting to basic research was part of a strategy of relief – not only relief from society’s expectation of science to produce immediate benefits, but also from political controversies that might affect a researcher’s reputation and put his or her chances of acquiring funding at risk.

The success of this key concept in science policy lay in the polysemy of “basic”, which functioned as a kind of self-reassurance within the scientific community and could be used to signal societal utility when communicating to the wider public. The concept of basic research thus worked as collective symbol linking the public discourse to the scientific discourse. The label “basic” signified that research was a precondition for future scientific progress. At the same time, it communicated the fundamental importance of research for societal or economic and technological progress.

In the end, the concept of basic research could not solve the dilemmas of science in 20th-century societies. In fact, it produced its own confusion and misleading expectations. The simplified promise of progress depended on society’s confidence, which dwindled during the economic crisis of the 1970s. Particularly after the discourse on basic research referred to the intrinsic ideal of science for its own sake, society’s trust was put at risk. In addition, the simplified promise of scientific objectivity – the alleged neutrality of facts – obscured the actual complexity of research, where scientific truth is always contested.

Which lessons can we draw from this analysis for the current theoretical debate in science and technology studies? Bruno Latour’s argument against basic research is well taken in so far as the semantics of the concept do not represent the actual research practices and their institutional settings. As the above analysis has shown, the same type of criticism was already voiced by contemporaries of Vannevar Bush. Bush’s definition of basic research, especially his dissociation of the natural sciences from the technical sciences and its purity ideals, however, were more than just a simple misrepresentation: they had a long-lasting effect on the Western notion of science and research policy.

The resort to purity ideals can be blamed for retarding or interrupting this reflective process within the natural sciences. Moreover, there are hints that the reference to older scientific ideals led to taboos being placed on research topics leading to technical innovations, at least in some disciplines within the natural sciences. Eric J. Vettel has demonstrated how the revival of the policy of pure science in the 1950s altered research topics and institutional organization in microbiology and how the turn toward an application-oriented research policy during the 1960s led, in the long run, to biotechnology (Vettel 2006 ). The discipline of biology is thus a good example for demonstrating that parts of this purity discourse have, once again, faded away. The self-image of researchers like Craig Venter, geneticist and entrepreneur, no longer corresponds to the old image of the quiet academic scholar (Venter 2007 ). These recent historical shifts have indeed been noted by science studies. But we need more long-term historical studies on individual research fields and disciplines – studies tracing the historical development of research topics as far back as the late 19th and early 20th centuries – in order to understand the effects of the return of the purity discourse after 1945.

Bruno Latour has described purity discourses as a typically modern phenomenon that has since become less important. The results of this analysis, however, show that the concept of basic research had many functions. Aside from its role as a criterion for distributing research funding, the concept of basic research mainly served as a strategy for coping, firstly, with society’s increasing expectations of science, secondly, with the ethical dilemmas associated with the debate on the overall purpose of science, and, thirdly, with the political implications of science’s role as an increasingly powerful force in society. The case of the US shows that, despite the initial criticism of anachronism, the revival of the purity discourse succeeded because the concept of basic research became a strategy to cope with the uncertainties and dilemmas of the Cold-War period.

In fact, many of these dilemmas will continue to challenge science policy in the 21st century. As science and technology have become powerful forces in our societies, they will be subject to both conflicts of interest and political and ethical controversies. Whether the strategy of dealing with these controversies will continue to characterize basic research is, however, an open question. On the one hand, some representatives of science and technology studies believe that previous strategies have failed in coping with these dilemmas (Jasanoff 2005 : 6; Shapin 2010 : 387–391). On the other hand, new concepts like the European Research Council’s “frontier research” seem to revive the basic-research concept – at least with regard to its original function as part of a discourse on knowledge sustainability, which the current article has sought to reveal (High-Level Expert Group 2005 : 16). 21

Analysis of these discourses provides us with insights into expectations placed upon future research and into societal and scientific experiences from the past, which, among other things, frame decisions about what kind of research society wants and what kind of research should be funded by the public purse. Historical semantics can help to elucidate scientific taboos, which are taken for granted because they are the outcome of specific political or societal situations. All in all, historical semantics could be one useful approach among many in science and technology studies. It provides a critical perspective on the complex relationship between science and society. Moreover, it helps to reveal the historical legacy of our notions of science and technology, including their multiple attributes, which are still very present, although this seems to have gone unnoticed by many contemporary observers. This is why the analysis of concepts such as basis research is (still) an interesting, worthwhile subject for science studies. However, with regard to the legacy of the concept of basic research, this article suggests that it should not be used as a technical term whose meanings can be taken for granted. Scholars in the field of science and technology studies are thus well advised to explicate which of the many facets of the term they allude to when using the concept of basic research.

1 See the subsection “The discursive strategy of ‘fundamental research’ and the reassessment of German science in the Nazi period” below for more details.

2 Given the recently growing interest in the relationship between science and technology, the reflection on terms like “applied science” seems to have started a little bit earlier (Bud 2012a , b ; Gooday 2012 ; Karns Alexander 2012 ; Lucier 2012 ; Schatzberg 2012 ). This literature, however, focusses primarily on the 19th century.

3 In contrast to older approaches of historical semantics like intellectual history or the history of ideas, which cling to the idea of authorship and reception theory, discourse analysis does not aim at identifying the pedigree of new concepts or arguments in detail. Instead of singling out individual statements or discussing personal rationales, discourse analysis rather tries to find common patters of arguments that are shared by many discourse participants.

4 The cited sources and documents do not represent the whole sample of texts. The texts I refer to in this article were chosen because they exemplify broader discursive strategies revolving around the concept of basic research in a particularly typical way.

5 In the German-speaking context, the term science is not restricted to natural sciences. This also applied to the English-speaking academia in early modern times.

6 The history of both scientific organizations was the subject of two major research projects, which have recently been concluded. See the wealth of research published in the academic series Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft (Stuttgart: Franz Steiner Verlag, four volumes published so far) and Geschichte der Kaiser-Wilhelm-Gesellschaft im Nationalsozialismus (Göttingen: Wallstein Verlag, seventeen volumes published so far).

7 In this article, I will consistently translate the German term Grundlagenforschung as “fundamental research”. Unlike the Anglo-American science discourse, the German science discourse does not accommodate conceptual variations such as that between “basic research” and “fundamental research”.

8 Trained as a theologian, Hans Hartmann worked as an author specializing in writing about scientists and science policy issues.

9 In other disciplinary contexts, the study of material properties was also denoted as goal-oriented research (Bauermeister 1938 : 479).

10 In the natural sciences during this period, the term only appeared in a very specific context. In the 1920s, a group of biologists and psychologists discussed the question of the functional usefulness of organs or mental processes within organisms, an approach they also labelled as Zweckforschung (E. Becher 1921 : 296–304; H. Becher 1923 ).

11 It took a while before the new term Zweckforschung (goal-oriented research) was established. Confusingly, some Nazi partisans used the term goal-oriented research as a synonym for pure science, criticizing the idea of research as an end in itself (W. Schultze 1938 : 5; Löhr 1938 : 20).

12 In order to get a better idea of conceptual dissemination, I provide here some results of a statistical analysis of the electronically accessible journal Science (including the supplement The Scientific Monthly ). Between 1921 and 1930, the term basic research was used 14 times and fundamental research was used 121 times. Between 1931 and 1940, the term basic research showed up 51 times and fundamental research 154 times. Just five years later, the term basic research had been employed 69 times and fundamental research 182 times.

13 See, for instance, the decision of the Senate of the Ludwig Maximilian University in Munich in the early 1920s (Archive of the University, Sen. 523).

14 Bush consistently referred to “basic research” in his report. At the time, however, the term “fundamental research” was more widespread.

15 As the broader research context was hidden to scientific peers and to the researchers themselves, this strategy at least allowed the publication of detailed results.

16 In 1944 and 1945 there was already an ongoing debate in letters to the editor of the journal Science that critically reflected the re-idealization of the notion of pure science (Stern 1944 ; Pearson 1944 ; Robin 1944 ; Feibleman 1944 ; Alexander 1945 ; Stern 1945 ; Moore 1945 ; J. R. Baker 1945 ).

17 See also James C. Connant’s contribution to the debate on the relationship between science and the public ( 1951 ). For more details, see Hershberg ( 1993 : 554–577).

18 The exact German terms deployed in the two key laws, the Kontrollratsgesetz no. 25 from 29.04.1946 and the Militärregierungsgesetz, no. 23 from 12.09.1949 were “grundlegende wissenschaftliche Forschung” and “angewandte wissenschaftliche Forschung”. The regulations defined military relevance on the basis of several classified research fields. For more details of how the allies controlled research, see Heinemann ( 2001 ).

19 In the German university tradition, Humboldt represents the link between education and research. For the current state of research on Humboldt’s role in university discourse, see Eichler ( 2012 ), Jarausch ( 1999 ).

20 The semantic differences between the two versions of the speech held by philosopher Karl Jaspers in 1923 and 1946 are significant when defining the role of universities in the wake of changes to the respective political systems: while Jaspers emphasized the objectivity of scientific knowledge in 1923, he replaced the term “objectivity” with “truth” in 1946 (Jaspers 1923 ; 1946 ).

21 The definition of frontier research relies again on the basic-applied nomenclature. In contrast to recent trends of promoting more applied forms of research, it shifts the balance further towards the basic-research pole of the continuum. See also the mission statement on the ERC website: http://erc.europa.eu/mission .

This article is part of a larger research project on changing notions of science in modern history funded by the VolkswagenStiftung. I would like to thank Isabelle Huber and Johannes Wittlinger for their competent research assistance. I am also grateful for the valuable comments and suggestions of Oliver Treib, Claudia Stein, Ulrich Wengenroth, Peter Weingart, Florian Schmaltz, Anne Sudrow and the two anonymous referees. Finally, I am much obliged to Gabrielle Robilliard for her careful language editing.

  • Abelson Philip H. Pressure on basic research. Science. 1966; 153 :11. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Alexander Jerome. Pure science. Science. 1945; 101 :37–38. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arthur JC. Development of vegetable physiology. Science. 1895; 2 :360–373. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ash Mitchell G. Wissenschaft und Politik als Ressourcen für einander. In: vom Bruch Rüdiger, Kaderas Brigitte., editors. Wissenschaften und Wissenschaftspolitik. Bestandsaufnahmen zu Formationen, Brüchen und Kontinuitäten im Deutschland des 20. Jahrhunderts. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2002. pp. 31–51. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ash Mitchell G. Wissenschaftswandlungen und politische Umbrüche im 20. Jahrhundert - was hatten sie miteinander zu tun? In: v. Bruch Rüdiger, Gerhardt Uta, Pawliczek Aleksandra., editors. Kontinuitäten und Diskontinuitäten in der Wissenschaftsgeschichte des 20. Jahrhunderts. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2006. pp. 19–37. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Astin Allen V. Basic research in government laboratories. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 143–157. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Auchter EC. Agricultural science and the people’s welfare. Science. 1942; 96 :283–289. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bachér Franz. Rede anlässlich der Kundgebung des NSD-Dozentenbundes Gau Groß-Berlin. In: NSD-Dozentenbund Gau Groß-Berlin, editor. Wissenschaft und Vierjahresplan. Berlin: NSD-Dozentenbund; 1937. pp. 17–31. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baker John R. The threat to pure science. Science. 1945; 101 :300–301. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baker WO. The paradox of choice. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 41–72. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barton Henry A, Burnham George H. The American Institute of Physics. A report to physicists. Science. 1943; 97 :172–176. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bauermeister Hermann. Die Bedeutung der Forschung für die Wehrtechnik. Marine-Rundschau. 1938; 43 :476–479. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becher Erich. Geisteswissenschaften und Naturwissenschaften. Untersuchungen zur Theorie und Einteilung der Realwissenschaften. München/Leipzig: Duncker & Humblot; 1921. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becher Helmut. Über die Zweckmäßigkeitsforschung in der Embryologie und seine finale Betrachtung einiger Wachstumsvorgänge und Einrichtungen in der Placenta. Anatomischer Anzeiger. 1923; 56 :272–290. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker Karl. Rede des Generals Prof. Dr. Becker. In: Pressestelle des Reichserziehungsministeriums, editor. Ein Ehrentag der deutschen Wissenschaft. Die Eröffnung des Reichsforschungsrats am 25. Mai 1937. Berlin: Weidmann; 1937. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bender Thomas. Politics, intellect, and the American University, 1945–1995. Daedalus. Journal of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences. 1997; 126 :1–38. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bennett RD. Reply to Professor Reed (letters to the editor) Science. 1946; 103 :85. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berkner Lloyd V. Secrecy and scientific progress. Science. 1956; 123 :783–786. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bird Kai, Sherwin JM. American Prometheus. The triumph and tragedy of J. Robert Oppenheimer. New York: Random House; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blakeslee Albert F. Ideals of science. Science. 1940; 92 :589–592. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blakeslee Albert F. Origin and ideals of the national science fund. Science. 1941; 94 :356–358. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blumenberg Hans. Paradigms for a metaphorology. Translated from the German with an afterword by Robert Savage. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Library; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braun-Thürmann Holger, Knie Andreas, Simon Dagmar. Unternehmen Wissenschaft. Ausgründungen als Grenzüberschreitungen akademischer Forschung. Bielefeld: Transcript Verlag; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brüche Ernst. Grundlagenforschung im Krieg. Physikalische Blätter. 1944; 1 :112–115. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bud Robert. “Applied science”: A phrase in search of a meaning. ISIS. 2012; 103 :537–545. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bud Robert. Focus: Applied science—Introduction. ISIS. 2012; 103 :515–517. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bud Robert, Roberts Gerylynn K. Science versus practice. Chemistry in Victorian Britain. Manchester: Manchester University Press; 1984. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bush Vannevar. Science. The endless frontier. A report to the president. Washington, DC: United States Government Printing Office; 1945. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bush Vannevar. Die Forschung von Heute und die Welt von Morgen. Essen-Bredeney: Stifterverb. für d. dt. Wissenschaft; 1954. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butenandt Alfred. Der Chemiker und die Geheimnisse des Lebens. Illustrierte Zeitung - Leipzig. 1940; 195 :122–123. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Calvert Jane. What’s special about basic research? Science, Technology and Human Values. 2006; 31 :199–220. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cassidy David. Controlling German science, II: Bizonal occupation and the struggle over West German science policy, 1946–1949. Historical Studies in Physical and Biological Sciences. 1996; 26 :197–239. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clarke Sabine. Pure science with a practical aim. The meanings of fundamental research in Britain, circa 1916–1950. ISIS. 2010; 101 :285–311. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Committee on Loyalty in Relation to Government Support of Unclassified Research Loyalty and Research. Report. Science. 1956; 123 :660–662. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Compton Arthur H. The human meaning of science. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press; 1940. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Compton Arthur H. Science and our nation’s future. Science. 1945; 101 :207–209. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Compton, Karl T. 1934. Science still holds a great promise. An answer to those who contend that ills of today can be blamed on technology. The New York Times Magazine 16, December 6–8.
  • Compton Karl T. Scientists face the world. In: Compton Karl T, Trullinger Robert W, Bush Vannevar., editors. Scientists face the world of 1942. New Brunswick: Rutgers; 1942. pp. 3–45. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conant James B. The role of science in our unique society. Science. 1948; 107 :77–83. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conant James B. Science and common sense. London: Oxford University Press; 1951. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conant James B. Gemeinsame Entwicklung der Industrie und der Naturwissenschaft. Essen: Stifterverb. für d. dt. Wissenschaft; 1953. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conant James B. Modern science and modern man. New York: Columbia University Press; 1961. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coulter John M. Botany as a national asset. Science. 1917; 45 :225–231. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coulter John M. The botanical opportunity. Science. 1919; 49 :363–367. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daniels George H. The pure-science ideal and democratic culture. Science. 1967; 156 :1699–1705. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daston Lorraine. Die Kultur der wissenschaftlichen Objektivität. In: Otto Gerhard Oexle, editor. Naturwissenschaft, Geisteswissenschaft, Kulturwissenschaft: Einheit - Gegensatz - Komplementarität? Göttingen: Wallstein; 2000. pp. 9–39. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dear Peter. What is the history of science the history of? ISIS. 2005; 96 :390–406. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dennis Micheal Aaron. “Our first line of defense”: two university laboratories in the postwar American State. ISIS. 1994; 85 :427–455. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dennis Micheal Aaron. Reconstructing sociotechnical order. Vannevar Bush and US science policy. In: Jasanoff Sheila., editor. States of knowledge. The co-production of science and social order. London: Routledge; 2004. pp. 225–253. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. 1956. Angewandte Forschung in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Lage und Ausbaunotwendigkeiten. Denkschrift . Bad Godesberg: Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft.
  • Dingler Hugo. Die Grundlagen der angewandten Geometrie. Eine Untersuchung über den Zusammenhang zwischen Theorie und Erfahrung in den exakten Wissenschaften. Leipzig: Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft; 1911. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drescher-Kaden, Friedrich Karl. 1941. Die Tätigkeit der Akademie der Wissenschaften im Jahre 1940/1941. Jahrbuch der Akademie der Wissenschaften in Göttingen 1940/41: 7–17.
  • Dryden Hugh L. The scientist in contemporary life. Science. 1954; 120 :1052–1055. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dubos René. Scientist and public. Why is the scientist, once a “natural philosopher”, now considered a barbarian by many educated laymen? Science. 1961; 133 :1207–1211. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DuBridge Lee A. Basic research and the private university. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 107–120. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dunn LC. Organization and support of science in the United States. Science. 1945; 102 :548–554. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Edgerton David. “The linear model” did not exist. Reflections on the history and historiography of science and research in industry in the twentieth century. In: Grandin Karl, Wormbs Nina, Widmalm Sven., editors. The science-industry nexus. History, policy, implications. Sagamore Beach: Science History Publications; 2004. pp. 31–57. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eichler Martin. Die Wahrheit des Mythos Humboldt. Historische Zeitschrift. 2012; 294 :59–78. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenhower Dwight D. Science: Handmaiden of freedom. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 133–142. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenhower Dwight D. Farewell address, January 17, 1961 (broadcast by radio and television to the nation) In: Whitney Gleaves., editor. American presidents: Farewell messages to the nation, 1796–2001. Lanham: Lexington Books; 2003. pp. 413–416. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elvehjem CA. Basic research and the state university. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 87–105. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Endell Kurd. Was bringt die moderne Tonforschung dem Keramiker. Berichte der Deutschen Keramischen Gesellschaft E.V. 1942; 23 :110–113. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Epple Moritz. Rechnen, Messen, Führen. Kriegsforschung am Kaiser-Wilhelm-Institut für Strömung1937–1945. In: Maier Helmut., editor. Rüstungsforschung im Nationalsozialismus. Organisation, Mobilisierung und Entgrenzung der Technikwissenschaften. Göttingen: Wallstein; 2002. pp. 305–356. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Epple Moritz. Kommentar zur Sektion Natur- und Technikwissenschaften. In: Orth Karin, Oberkrome Willi., editors. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1920–1970. Forschungsförderung im Spannungsfeld von Wissenschaft und Politik. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2010. pp. 211–215. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erbe Walter. Autonomie und Weltoffenheit der Universität. Universitas. 1954; 9 :1171–1180. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erker Paul. “Vierjahresplan-Chemie” und “Polymer Science”. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft und die Chemische Forschung von den 1920er bis in die 1960er Jahre. In: Orth Karin, Oberkrome Willi., editors. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1920–1970. Forschungsförderung im Spannungsfeld von Wissenschaft und Politik. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2010. pp. 183–202. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Etzkowitz Henry, Leydesdorff Loet., editors. Universities and the global knowledge economy. A triple helix of university–industry–government relations. London: Printer; 1997. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feibleman James. The threat to pure science. Science. 1944; 100 :521. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feynman Richard P. The value of science. Engineering and Science. 1955; 19 :13–15. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fisk James B. Basic research in industrial laboratories. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 159–167. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flachowsky Sören. Von der Notgemeinschaft zum Reichsforschungsrat. Wissenschaftspolitik im Kontext von Autarkie, Aufrüstung und Krieg. Stuttgart: Steiner; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flachowsky Sören. “Werkzeug der deutschen Kriegsführung”. Die Forschungspolitik der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft und des Reichsforschungsrates zwischen 1920 und 1945. In: Orth Karin, Oberkrome Willi., editors. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1920–1970. Forschungsförderung im Spannungsfeld von Wissenschaft und Politik. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2010. pp. 53–69. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forman Paul. Behind quantum electronics: National security as basis for physical research in the United States, 1940–1960. Historical Studies in the Physical and Biological Sciences. 1987; 18 :149–229. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forman Paul. (Re)cognizing postmodernity: Helps for historians - of science especially. Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte. 2010; 33 :157–175. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fosdick Raymond B. The work of the Rockefeller Foundation—The year in brief. Science. 1934; 97 :379–381. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Frowein F. Forschungskontrolle in der Bizone. Chemie Ingenieur Technik. 1949; 21 :160–161. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Frowein F. Forschungskontrolle in Westdeutschland. Chemie Ingenieur Technik. 1950; 22 :47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Galison Peter. Ten problems in history and philosophy of science. The History of Science Society. 2008; 99 :111–124. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gates David M. Basic research in Europe. Different countries favor different systems for the support and organization of scientific work. Science. 1958; 128 :227–235. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Geiger Roger L. To advance knowledge. The growth of American research universities, 1900–1940. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1986. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gibb Alexander. Research in engineering. Science. 1937; 86 :232–236. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gibbons Michael, Limoges Camille, Nowotny Helga, Schwartzman Simon, Scott Peter, Trow Martin. The new production of knowledge. The dynamics of science and research in contemporary societies. London: Sage; 1994. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gieryn Thomas F. Cultural boundaries of science. Credibility on the line. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Godin, Benoît. 2005a. The linear model of innovation. The historical construction of an analytical framework . Project on the History and Sociology of S&T Statistics Working Paper No. 30. Montreal: Canadian Science and Innovation Indicators Consortium. http://www.csiic.ca/PDF/Godin_30.pdf .
  • Godin Benoît. Measurement and statistics on science and technology. London: Routledge; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gooday Graeme. “Vague and artificial”: The historically elusive distinction between pure and applied science. ISIS. 2012; 103 :546–554. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goudsmit Samuel. Alsos. The failure in German science. London: Sigma Books; 1947. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenberg Daniel S. Science for sale. The perils, rewards, and delusions of campus capitalism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2007. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenewalt Crawford H. Basic research: A technological savings account. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 127–132. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guston David H. Between politics and science. Assuring the integrity and productivity of research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hahn Otto. Ist Grundlagenforschung Luxus? Umwälzende Erfindungen sind die Frucht subtiler Kleinarbeit. Göttinger Universitäts-Zeitung. 1949; 4 :5–6. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hahn Otto. Die Bedeutung der Grundlagenforschung für die Wirtschaft. Essen: Stifterverb. für d. dt. Wissenschaft; 1954. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamblin Jacob Darwin. The Navy’s “sophisticated” pursuit of science. Undersea warfare, the limits of internationalism, and the utility of basic research 1945–1956. ISIS. 2002; 93 :1–27. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hartmann Hans. Was heißt Deutsche Forschung? Illustrierte Zeitung - Leipzig. 1940; 195 :108. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heinemann Manfred. Überwachung und “Inventur” der deutschen Forschung. Das Kontrollratsgesetz Nr. 25 und die alliierte Forschungskontrolle im Bereich der Kaiser-Wilhelm-/Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. In: Mertens Lothar., editor. Politischer Systembruch als irreversibler Faktor von Modernisierung in der Wissenschaft? Berlin: Duncker & Humblot; 2001. pp. 167–199. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heiss R. Bemerkenswerte ausländische Entwicklungen in der lebensmittelindustriellen Verfahrenstechnik. Chemie Ingenieur Technik. 1950; 22 :121–127. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Henkel Heinrich. Der Begriff der Wissenschaft in Forschung und Lehre. Die deutsche Hochschule Schriftenreihe zur Erneuerung und künftigen Gestaltung des gesamten deutschen Hochschulwesens. 1933; 1 :8–29. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hershberg James B. James B. Conant. Harvard to Hiroshima and the making of the nuclear age. New York: Alfred A. Knopf; 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hiedemann Egon. Grundlagen und Ergebnisse der Ultraforschung. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter; 1939. [ Google Scholar ]
  • High-Level Expert Group. 2005. Frontier research: The European Challenge. High-Level Expert Group report. Brussels: European Commission. http://ec.europa.eu/research/future/pdf/hleg_fullreport_frontier_research_april2005.pdf .
  • Hilbert, David. 1901. Mathematische Probleme. Archiv für Mathematik und Physik 1: 44–63, 213–237.
  • Hoffmann Johannes, Suhr Otto. Stand der Verpackungstechnik. Zeitschrift des Vereines Deutscher Ingenieure. 1944; 88 :543–550. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jarausch Konrad H. Das Humboldt-Syndrom: Die westdeutschen Universitäten 1945–1989—Ein akademischer Sonderweg. In: Ash Mitchell G., editor. Mythos Humboldt: Vergangenheit und Zukunft der deutschen Universitäten. Köln: Böhlau; 1999. pp. 58–79. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jasanoff Sheila. Designs on nature. Science and democracy in Europe and the United States. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jaspers Karl. Die Idee der Universität. Berlin: Springer; 1923. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jaspers Karl. Die Idee der Universität. Berlin: Springer; 1946. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jewett Frank B. The promise of technology. Science. 1944; 99 :1–6. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Joerges Bernward, Shinn Terry. A fresh look at instrumentation. An introduction. In: Joerges Bernward, Shinn Terry., editors. Instrumentation between science, state and industry. Dordrecht: Kluwer; 2001. pp. 1–13. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaiser Wilhelm-Gesellschaft zur Förderung der Wissenschaften Tätigkeitsbericht der Kaiser Wilhelm-Gesellschaft zur Förderung der Wissenschaften (April 1939 bis März 1939) Die Naturwissenschaften. 1939; 27 :321–333. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karns Alexander Jennifer. Thinking again about science in technology. ISIS. 2012; 103 :518–526. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kevles Daniel J. The National Science Foundation and the debate over postwar research policy, 1942–1945: A political interpretation of science—The endless frontier. ISIS. 1977; 68 :4–26. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kilgore Harley M. Science and the government. Science. 1945; 102 :630–638. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Killian James R. Capsule conclusions. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 121–126. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Killian James R. Science and public policy. Recent Actions by the Federal Government in helping science and technology help the nation are surveyed. Science. 1959; 129 :129–136. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kistiakowsky George B. Allocating support for basic research—And the importance of practical applications. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists. 1966; 21 :12–18. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klein Ursula. Ein Bergrat, zwei Minister und sechs Lehrende. Versuche der Gründung einer Bergakademie in Berlin um 1770. N.T.M. 2010; 18 :437–468. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kline Ronald. Construing “technology” as “applied science”. Public rhetoric of scientists and engineers in the United States, 1880–1945. ISIS. 1995; 86 :194–221. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kolloid-Gesellschaft Bericht über die XI. Hauptversammlung der Kolloid-Gesellschaft am 21. und 22. September 1936 in Dresden. Kolloid-Zeitschrift. 1936; 77 :153–161. [ Google Scholar ]
  • König Wolfgang. 100 Jahre “Dr.-Ing.”. Ein “Ritterschlag der Wissenschaft”. Das Promotionsrecht der Technischen Hochschulen und der VDI Verein Deutscher Ingenieure. Düsseldorf: VDI-Verlag; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koselleck Reinhart. Begriffsgeschichten. Studien zur Semantik und Pragmatik der politischen und sozialen Sprache. Frankfurt a.M: Suhrkamp; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krauch Carl. Forschung tut not! Der Vierjahresplan. 1939; 3 :814–816. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krauch Carl. Forschung in Hochschule und Werk. Illustrierte Zeitung - Leipzig. 1940; 195 :121–122. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krauch Carl. Über den Einsatz der Forschung im Rahmen des Vierjahresplans. Der Deutsche Chemiker. 1941; 7 :1–2. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krieck Ernst. Der Neubau der Universität. Die deutsche Hochschule. Schriftenreihe zur Erneuerung und künftigen Gestaltung des gesamten deutschen Hochschulwesens. 1933; 1 :1–7. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krige John. American hegemony and the postwar reconstruction of science in Europe. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krige John. Building the arsenal of knowledge. Centaurus. 2010; 52 :280–296. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latour Bruno. Science in action. Cambridge/MA: Cambridge University Press; 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latour Bruno. We have never been modern. Cambridge/MA: Harvard University Press; 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latour Bruno. Reassembling the social. An introduction to actor-network-theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leob Leonard B. Scientific research and national welfare. Science. 1946; 103 :540–541. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leslie Stuart W. The Cold War and American science. The military–industrial–academic complex at M.I.T. and Stanford. New York: Columbia University Press; 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lewin Kurt. Der Begriff der Genese in Physik, Biologie und Entwicklungsgeschichte. Eine Untersuchung zur vergleichenden Wissenschaftslehre. Berlin: Springer; 1922. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liebig Justus Freiherr von. Die moderne Landwirthschaft als Beispiel der Gemeinnützigkeit der Wissenschaften. Rede in der öffentlichen Sitzung der k. Akademie der Wissenschaften zu München am 28. November 1861. Braunschweig: Friedrich Vieweg und Sohn; 1862. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Link Jürgen. Interdiskurs, System der Kollektivsymbole, Literatur. Thesen zu einer generativen Diskurs- und Literaturtheorie. In: Eschbach Achim., editor. Perspektiven des Verstehens. Bochum: Studienverlag Dr. Nobert Brockmeyer; 1986. pp. 128–146. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Löhr Hanns. Wesen und Sinn der nationalsozialistischen wissenschaftlichen Akademie des NSD-Dozentenbundes der Christian-Albrechts-Universität. In: Reichsdozentenführung, editor. Grundfragen der deutschen Universität und Wissenschaft. Neumünster: Karl Wachholz; 1938. pp. 13–25. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lucier Paul. The origins of pure and applied science in gilded age America. ISIS. 2012; 103 :527–536. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luxbacher Günther. Werkstoff, Sparstoffe, Heimstoff. Metallforschung in Deutschland 1920–1970. In: Orth Karin, Oberkrome Willi., editors. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1920–1970. Forschungsförderung im Spannungsfeld von Wissenschaft und Politik. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2010. pp. 163–181. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marcus Alan I. Agricultural science and the quest for legitimacy. Farmers, agricultural colleges and experiment stations, 1870–1890. Ames: Iowa State University Press; 1985. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mehrtens Herbert. Moderne, Sprache, Mathematik. Eine Geschichte des Streits um die Grundlagen der Disziplin und des Subjekts formaler Systeme. Frankfurt a.M: Suhrkamp; 1990. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mehrtens Herbert. Irresponsible purity: The political and moral structure of mathematical sciences in the National Socialist state. In: Renneberg Monika, Walker Mark., editors. Science, technology and National Socialism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994. pp. 324–338. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meinel Christoph. Reine und angewandte Chemie. Die Entstehung einer neuen Wissenschaftskonzeption in der Chemie der Aufklärung. Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte. 1985; 8 :25–45. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Merton Robert King. A note on science and democracy. Journal of Legal and Political Sociology. 1942; 1 :115–126. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mittelstraß Jürgen. Leonardo-Welt. Über Wissenschaft, Forschung und Verantwortung. Frankfurt a.M: Suhrkamp; 1994. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moore Jared S. The threat to pure science. Science. 1945; 101 :62. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morison Robert S. Support of basic research from private philanthropy. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 221–248. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mowery David C, Rosenberg Nathan. The U.S. national innovation system. In: Nelson Richard R., editor. National innovation systems. A comparative analysis. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 29–75. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nagel Anne Chr. “Er ist der Schrecken überhaupt der Hochschule“ - Der Nationalsozialistische Deutsche Dozentenbund in der Wissenschaftspolitik des Dritten Reichs. In: Scholtyseck Joachim, Studt Christoph., editors. Universitäten und Studenten im Dritten Reich. Bejahung, Anpassung, Widerstand. XIX Königswinterer Tagung vom 17.–19. Februar 2006. Berlin: LIT Verlag; 2008. pp. 115–132. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Niemeier Georg. Grundlagenforschung und Zweckforschung. Physikalische Blätter. 1944; 1 :106–109. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nutting PG. Organized knowledge and national welfare. Science. 1917; 46 :247–252. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • OECD . The measurement of scientific and technical activities. Proposed standard practice for surveys of research and experimental development. Frascati manual. Paris: Director of Information, OECD; 1976. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oppenheimer Robert J. The need for new knowledge. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 1–15. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orth Karin. Autonomie und Planung der Forschung. Förderpolitische Strategien der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft 1949–1968. Stuttgart: Steiner; 2011. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ostwald Wilhelm. Theorie und Praxis. Zeitschrift des Österreichischen Ingenieur- und Architekten-Vereins. 1905; 57 :3–9. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ostwald Wilhelm. Erfinder und Entdecker. Leipzig: Rütten & Loening; 1908. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ostwald Wilhelm. Die Wissenschaft. Vortrag, gehalten auf dem Ersten Monisten-Kongresse zu Hamburg am 10. September 1911. Leipzig: Alfred Kröner; 1911. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ostwald Wilhelm. Was ist Wissenschaft? Archiv für Philosophie und Soziologie. 1929; 33 :13–30. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ostwald Wolfgang. Kolloidchemische Probleme der Zellwolle-Forschung. Kolloid-Zeitschrift. 1942; 98 :129–131. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Owens Larry. The counterproductive management of science in the Second World War: Vannevar Bush and the Office of Scientific Research and Development. Business History Review. 1994; 68 :515–576. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pearson John M. The opportunity of pure science. Science. 1944; 100 :471–472. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Phillips Melba. Dangers confronting American science. Science. 1952; 116 :439–443. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pielke Roger., Jr ‘‘Basic research’’ as a political symbol. Minerva. 2012; 50 (3):339–361. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Polanyi Michael. The republic of science. Its political and economic theory. Minerva. 1962; 1 :54–73. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Popp Berman Elizabeth. Creating the market university. How academic science became a economic engine. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Potter AA. Research and invention in engineering colleges. Science. 1940; 91 :1–7. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Price Don K. The scientific establishment. The American system gives scientists in government a freedom and influence unmatched in other countries. Science. 1962; 136 :1099–1106. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rapoport Anatol. Scientific approach to ethics. Science. 1957; 125 :796–799. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reagan Michael D. Basic and applied research: A meaningful distinction? Science. 1967; 155 :1383–1386. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reichenbächer Ernst. Gründzüge zu einer Theorie der Materie. Zeitschrift für Physik. 1937; 107 :285–309. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rein FH. Die gegenwärtige Situation der deutschen Universität. Universitas. 1946; 1 :897–902. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reingold Nathan. Vannevar Bush’s new deal for research: Or the triumph of the old order. Historical Studies in the Physical and Biological Sciences. 1987; 17 :299–344. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reppe Walter. Lenkungs- und Nachwuchsprobleme der Forschung. Chemie Ingenieur Technik. 1950; 22 :1–5. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Richter Rud. “The Anwendbarkeit” - als Brandmal der deutschen Wissenschaft. Natur und Volk. Bericht der Senckenbergischen Naturforschenden Gesellschaft. 1943; 73 :205–207. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Riedler, Alois. 1900. Ueber die geschichtliche und zukünftige Bedeutung der Technik. Rede zum Geburtstagsfeste seiner Majestät des Kaisers und Königs Wilhelm II. in der Halle der Königlichen Technischen Hochschule zu Berlin am 26. Januar 1900 . [Halle].
  • Robbins William J. The support of fundamental science. Science. 1941; 94 :8. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robin Eugene VD. The threat to pure science. Science. 1944; 100 :519–521. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ross Sydney. Scientist. The story of a word. Annals of Science. 1962; 18 :65–85. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rössler Constantin. System der Staatslehre. Leipzig: Falcke & Roessler; 1857. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rulf Friedrich. Über die Grundlagenforschung in der Geometrie. Monatshefte für Mathematik und Physik. 1913; 24 :142–158. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Runkel Roland. Wesen, Ziele und Aufgaben der Zellwandforschung: Zugleich III: Mitteilung “Zur Kenntnis der Zellwände tropischer Laubhölzer“ European Journal of Wood and Wood Products. 1942; 5 :305–309. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rust Bernhard. Nationalsozialismus und Wissenschaft. Illustrierte Zeitung - Leipzig. 1940; 195 (118–119):141. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sachse Carola. Bedingte Umbrüche: Überlegungen zum Verhältnis von Wissenschaft und Politik in der Westdeutschen Nachkriegsgesellschaft. In: Orth Karin, Oberkrome Willi., editors. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1920–1970. Forschungsförderung im Spannungsfeld von Wissenschaft und Politik. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner; 2010. pp. 471–481. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sayre Wallace. Scientists and American science policy. Who speaks for science? The future offers more dilemmas than unequivocal answers in science policy. Science. 1961; 133 :864–895. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schatzberg Eric. From art to applied science. ISIS. 2012; 103 :555–563. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schauz Désirée. Diskursiver Wandel am Beispiel der Disziplinarmacht. Geschichtstheoretische Implikationen der Dispositivanalyse. In: Landwehr Achim., editor. Diskursiver Wandel. Wiesbaden: VS Verlag; 2010. pp. 89–111. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schultze Joachim H. Grundlagenforschung und Zweckforschung in der Modernen Geographie. Petermanns Geographische Mitteilungen. 1943; 89 :193–203. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schultze Walter. Grundfragen der deutschen Universität und Wissenschaft. Rede des Reichsdozentenführers Reichsamtsleiter SS-Brigadeführer Professor Dr. Walter Schultze gehalten zu der Einweihung der ersten Akademie des NSD-Dozentenbundes in Kiel. In: Reichsdozentenführung, editor. Grundfragen der deutschen Universität und Wissenschaft. Neumünster: Karl Wachholz; 1938. pp. 1–11. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Science Adjustments in the educational program at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Science. 1942; 95 :90–91. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shapin Steven. The scientific life. A moral history of a late modern vocation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shapin Steven. Never pure. Historical studies of science as if it was produced by people with bodies, situated in time, space, culture, and society, and struggling for credibility and authority. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shepard Ward. Science for democracy. Science. 1946; 103 :65–68. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shils Edward A. The torment of secrecy. The background and consequences of American security policies. Glencoe (Ill): Free Press; 1956. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Simons JH. Research in wartime. Science. 1943; 97 :390–392. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sinnott Edmund W. Ten million scientists. Science. 1950; 111 :123–129. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sörensen E. Organisation der Forschung. Zeitschrift des Vereines Deutscher Ingenieure. 1952; 94 :158–160. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spaght Monroe E. Basic research in industry. Science. 1955; 121 :784–789. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stadlinger Hermann. Rationalisierung auch im chemischen Laboratorium! Chemiker-Zeitung. 1944; 68 :224–229. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steelman, John R. 1947a. Science and public policy . Vol. 3: Administration for Research. Washington, DC: U. S. Govt. Print. Off.
  • Steelman, John R. 1947b. Science and public policy. A report to the president . Vol. 1: A program for the nation. New York: Arno Press.
  • Steinsiek Peter-Michael. Forst- und Holzforschung im “Dritten Reich”. Remagen: Verlag Kessel; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stern Alexander W. The threat to pure science. Science. 1944; 100 :356. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stern Alexander W. Pure science. Science. 1945; 101 :38. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stock Alfred. Rede auf der Hauptversammlung am 7. Mai 1938. Berichte der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft. 1938; 71 :147–153. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stokes Donald E. Pasteur’s quadrant. Basic science and technological innovation. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press; 1997. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Szent-Györgyi Albert. Science, ethics, and politics. Science. 1957; 125 :225–226. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Szöllösi-Janze Margit. Science and social space: Transformations in the institutions of Wissenschaft from the Wilhelmine Empire to the Weimar Republic. Minerva. 2005; 43 :339–360. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor Hugh S. The organization, direction and support of research in the physical sciences. Science. 1944; 99 :249–256. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor William Palmer. Dangers for science? Science. 1953; 118 :449–450. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thelin John R. A history of American higher education. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tuve Merle A. Basic research in private research institutes. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: The American Association for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 169–184. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Venter Craig J. A life decoded. My genome, my life. New York: Viking; 2007. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Verein Deutscher Chemiker Versammlungsbericht: Institut für chemische Technologie synthetischer Fasern an der T.H. Breslau. Die Chemie N.F. 1943; 56 :61–62. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vettel Eric J. Biotech. The countercultural origins of an industry. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vieweg Richard. Über einige Fragen der Hochschulreform. Zeitschrift des Vereines Deutscher Ingenieure. 1950; 92 :729–733. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Virchow, Rudolf. 1877. Die Freiheit der Wissenschaft im modernen Staat. Rede gehalten in der dritten allgemeinen Sitzung der fünfzigsten Versammlung deutscher Naturforscher und Ärzte zu München am 22. September 1877 . Berlin: Wiegandt, Hempel & Parey.
  • Wagner Patrick. “Reservat der Ordinarien”. Zur Geschichte der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft zwischen 1920 und 1970. In: Orth Karin, Oberkrome Willi., editors. Die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 1920–1970. Forschungsförderung im Spannungsfeld von Wissenschaft und Politik. Stuttgart: Steiner; 2010. pp. 23–38. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walden Paul. Persönlichkeit und Leistung in der Naturwissenschaft. Universitas. 1946; 1 :713–730. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang Jessica. Liberals, the progressive left, and the political economy of postwar American science. The National Science Foundation debate. Historical Studies in the Physical and Biological Sciences. 1995; 26 :139–166. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang Jessica. American science in an age of anxiety. Scientists, anticommunism, and the Cold War. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang Jessica. Merton’s shadow: Perspectives on science and democracy since 1940. Historical Studies in the Physical and Biological Sciences. 1999; 30 :279–306. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waterman Alan T. Present role of the National Science Foundation. The Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists. 1951; 7 :165–167. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waterman Alan T. Basic research in the United States. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. 17–40. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waterman Alan T. The role of the National Science Foundation. The Annals of The American Academy. 1960; 327 :123–131. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weaver Warren. Preface. In: Wolfle Dael., editor. Symposium on basic research. Washington, DC: American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science; 1959. pp. XI–XVII. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weaver Warren. The moral un-neutrality of science. Science. 1961; 133 :255–261. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weidlein Edward R. Various results of being researchful. Science. 1935; 82 :553–556. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weingart Peter. Ökonomisierung der Wissenschaft. N.T.M. 2008; 16 :477–484. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Westwick Peter J. Secret science: A classified community in the national laboratories. Minerva. 2000; 38 :363–391. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wever Franz. Aufgaben und Wege der Eisenforschung. Stahl und Eisen. 1952; 72 :1053–1055. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Willing Willi. Rede anlässlich der Kundgebung des NSD-Dozentenbundes Gau Groß-Berlin. In: NSD-Dozentenbundes Gau Groß-Berlin, editor. Wissenschaft und Vierjahresplan. Berlin: NSD-Dozentenbund; 1937. pp. 3–16. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Witzell Karl. Die Bedeutung von Wissenschaft und Forschung für die Entwicklung der Kampfmittel der Kriegsmarine. Nauticus. Jahrbuch für Deutschlands Seeinteressen. 1944; 27 :205–223. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zachary GP. Endless frontier. Vannevar Bush, engineer of the American century. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1997. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zenneck J. Die Bedeutung der Forschung. Physikalische Blätter. 1944; 1 :6–12. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ziegelmayer Wilhelm. Rohstoff-Fragen der deutschen Volksernährung. Eine Darstellung der ernährungswirtschaftlichen und ernährungswissenschaftlichen Aufgaben unserer Zeit. Dresden: Theodor Steinkopff; 1936. [ Google Scholar ]
  • skip to Cookie Notice
  • skip to Main Navigation
  • skip to Main Content
  • skip to Footer
  • Find a Doctor
  • Find a Location
  • Appointments & Referrals
  • Patient Gateway
  • Español
  • Leadership Team
  • Quality & Safety
  • Equity & Inclusion
  • Community Health
  • Education & Training
  • Centers & Departments
  • Browse Treatments
  • Browse Conditions A-Z
  • View All Centers & Departments
  • Clinical Trials
  • Cancer Clinical Trials
  • Cancer Center
  • Digestive Healthcare Center
  • Heart Center
  • Mass General for Children
  • Neuroscience
  • Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Information for Visitors
  • Maps & Directions
  • Parking & Shuttles
  • Services & Amenities
  • Accessibility
  • Visiting Boston
  • International Patients
  • Medical Records
  • Billing, Insurance & Financial Assistance
  • Privacy & Security
  • Patient Experience
  • Explore Our Laboratories
  • Industry Collaborations
  • Research & Innovation News
  • About the Research Institute
  • Innovation Programs
  • Education & Community Outreach
  • Support Our Research
  • Find a Researcher
  • News & Events
  • Ways to Give
  • Patient Rights & Advocacy
  • Website Terms of Use
  • Apollo (Intranet)
  • Like us on Facebook
  • Follow us on Twitter
  • See us on LinkedIn
  • Print this page

Press Release May | 20 | 2024

Yoga and Meditation-Induced Altered States of Consciousness are Common in the General Population

Key takeaways.

  • Yoga, mindfulness, meditation, breathwork, and other practices can foster altered states of consciousness
  • In a study of adults in the general population, a team including researchers from Massachusetts General Hospital found that a surprisingly high percentage of participants (45%) reported experiencing altered states of consciousness
  • Strikingly, almost one-fifth of the respondents reported experiences of suffering following altered states of consciousness

BOSTON—Yoga, mindfulness, meditation, breathwork, and other practices are gaining in popularity due to their potential to improve health and well-being. The effects of these practices are mostly positive and occasionally transformational, yet they are known to sometimes be associated with challenging altered states of consciousness.

New research by a team including investigators from Massachusetts General Hospital, a founding member of the Mass General Brigham healthcare system, reveals that altered states of consciousness associated with meditation practice are far more common than expected.

Although many people reported positive outcomes, that were sometimes even considered transformational, from these experiences, for a substantial minority the experiences were negative. The results are published in the journal Mindfulness .

“With more people engaging in mindfulness, meditation, and other contemplative and mind-body practices, we thought that altered states and their effects might be common among the general population. We conducted a series of international surveys to investigate and indeed found that such experiences were widespread,” said senior author Matthew D. Sacchet, PhD, the director of the Meditation Research Program at Massachusetts General Hospital and an associate professor of Psychiatry at Harvard Medical School.

“Altered states were most often followed by positive, and sometimes even transformational effects on wellbeing,” Sacchet adds.” With that said negative effects on well-being were also reported in some cases, with a small subset of individuals reporting substantial suffering.”

For the study, a panel of experts in psychiatry, neuroscience, meditation, and survey design developed a questionnaire on the experience of altered states of consciousness.

Among 3,135 adults in the US and the UK who completed the online questionnaire, 45% reported experiencing non-pharmacologically induced altered states of consciousness at least once in their lives.

This is far more than expected from the 5% (US) to 15% (UK) of these population estimated to have undertaken mindfulness practice.

The experiences included derealization (the feeling of being detached from your environment), unitive experiences (a sense of unity or “oneness”), ecstatic thrills, vivid perceptions, changes in perceived size, bodily heat or electricity, out-of-body experiences, and perception of non-physical lights.

Respondents reported a mix of positive and negative well-being following altered states, with 13% claiming moderate or greater suffering and 1.1% claiming life-threatening suffering. Of those who experienced suffering, 63% did not seek help.

“Rather than being extremely unusual and rare, our study found that altered states of consciousness are a common variant of normal human experience,” said Sacchet. “However, we’ve found that those who experience negative outcomes related to these altered states often do not seek help, and that clinicians are poorly prepared to recognize or support these kinds of experiences. This has contributed to what might be considered a public health issue as a certain proportion of people have difficulty integrating their experiences of altered states into their existing conceptions of self and reality.”

Sacchet noted that additional studies are needed to identify individual characteristics associated with experiencing altered states of consciousness, and with potential suffering associated with these states. He also stressed the importance of applying this research to patient care. 

“We should not dismiss meditation and other practices as inherently dangerous but rather we need to better understand and support meditators to fully realize the potential of these practices,” he said. “Similar to psychotherapy, pharmacology, and other therapeutic tools it’s important that we learn to best implement and support people when engaging with these powerful practices.”

He added that “ancient meditation manuals from the wisdom traditions may be useful for classifying and understanding altered states of consciousness. They may provide guidance into how to better manage altered states when they may be difficult. We clearly need more research to further study and understand this possibility.”

“Clinical curriculum on altered states of consciousness should be developed to better support clinicians caring for patients experiencing suffering linked to these kinds of experiences,” Sacchet added.

“Also, those who teach meditation practices should ensure that participants are aware of potential risk,” he said. “Together, these kinds of safeguards will help to ensure that these very promising and powerful practices are taught and experienced safely.”

Authorship: M. J. Wright, J. Galante, J. S. Corneille, A. Grabovac, D. M. Ingram, and M. D. Sacchet.

Disclosures: Disclosure forms provided by the authors are available with the full text of this article at https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12671-024-02356-z Funding: Massey University and Emergence Benefactors

Paper cited:  Wright, M.J., Galante, J., Corneille, J.S. et al. Altered States of Consciousness are Prevalent and Insufficiently Supported Clinically: A Population Survey. Mindfulness (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-024-02356-z

  • Press Release

Centers and Departments

  • Research Institute
  • Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
  • Mental Health
  • Mind/Body Medicine

Check out the Mass General Research Institute blog

Bench Press highlights the groundbreaking research and boundary-pushing scientists working to improve human health and fight disease.

Support Research at Mass General

Your gift helps fund groundbreaking research aimed at understanding, treating and preventing human disease.

About Massachusetts General Hospital

Massachusetts General Hospital, founded in 1811, is the original and largest teaching hospital of Harvard Medical School. The Mass General Research Institute conducts the largest hospital-based research program in the nation, with annual research operations of more than $1 billion and comprises more than 9,500 researchers working across more than 30 institutes, centers and departments. MGH is a founding member of the Mass General Brigham healthcare system.

  • Open access
  • Published: 23 May 2024

Combining plasma Aβ and p-tau217 improves detection of brain amyloid in non-demented elderly

  • Yoshiki Niimi 1   na1 ,
  • Shorena Janelidze 2   na1 ,
  • Kenichiro Sato 1 , 3 ,
  • Naoki Tomita 4 , 5 ,
  • Tadashi Tsukamoto 6 ,
  • Takashi Kato 7 ,
  • Kenji Yoshiyama 8 ,
  • Hisatomo Kowa 9 ,
  • Atsushi Iwata 10 ,
  • Ryoko Ihara 10 ,
  • Kazushi Suzuki 11 ,
  • Kensaku Kasuga 12 ,
  • Takeshi Ikeuchi 12 ,
  • Kenji Ishii 13 ,
  • Kengo Ito 7 ,
  • Akinori Nakamura 7 ,
  • Michio Senda 14 ,
  • Theresa A. Day 15 ,
  • Samantha C. Burnham 15 ,
  • Leonardo Iaccarino 15 ,
  • Michael J. Pontecorvo 15 ,
  • Oskar Hansson 1 , 16 &
  • Takeshi Iwatsubo 1 , 3  

Alzheimer's Research & Therapy volume  16 , Article number:  115 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

1289 Accesses

31 Altmetric

Metrics details

Maximizing the efficiency to screen amyloid-positive individuals in asymptomatic and non-demented aged population using blood-based biomarkers is essential for future success of clinical trials in the early stage of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). In this study, we elucidate the utility of combination of plasma amyloid-β (Aβ)-related biomarkers and tau phosphorylated at threonine 217 (p-tau217) to predict abnormal Aβ-positron emission tomography (PET) in the preclinical and prodromal AD.

We designed the cross-sectional study including two ethnically distinct cohorts, the Japanese trial-ready cohort for preclinica and prodromal AD (J-TRC) and the Swedish BioFINDER study. J-TRC included 474 non-demented individuals (CDR 0: 331, CDR 0.5: 143). Participants underwent plasma Aβ and p-tau217 assessments, and Aβ-PET imaging. Findings in J-TRC were replicated in the BioFINDER cohort including 177 participants (cognitively unimpaired: 114, mild cognitive impairment: 63). In both cohorts, plasma Aβ(1-42) (Aβ42) and Aβ(1-40) (Aβ40) were measured using immunoprecipitation-MALDI TOF mass spectrometry (Shimadzu), and p-tau217 was measured with an immunoassay on the Meso Scale Discovery platform (Eli Lilly).

Aβ-PET was abnormal in 81 participants from J-TRC and 71 participants from BioFINDER. Plasma Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio and p-tau217 individually showed moderate to high accuracies when detecting abnormal Aβ-PET scans, which were improved by combining plasma biomarkers and by including age, sex and APOE genotype in the models. In J-TRC, the highest AUCs were observed for the models combining p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio, APOE , age, sex in the whole cohort (AUC = 0.936), combining p-tau217, Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio, APOE , age, sex in the CDR 0 group (AUC = 0.948), and combining p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio, APOE , age, sex in the CDR 0.5 group (AUC = 0.955), respectively. Each subgroup results were replicated in BioFINDER, where the highest AUCs were seen for models combining p-tau217, Aβ42/40 ratio, APOE , age, sex in cognitively unimpaired (AUC = 0.938), and p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio, APOE , age, sex in mild cognitive impairment (AUC = 0.914).

Conclusions

Combination of plasma Aβ-related biomarkers and p-tau217 exhibits high performance when predicting Aβ-PET positivity. Adding basic clinical information (i.e., age, sex, APOE  ε genotype) improved the prediction in preclinical AD, but not in prodromal AD. Combination of Aβ-related biomarkers and p-tau217 could be highly useful for pre-screening of participants in clinical trials of preclinical and prodromal AD.

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common neurodegenerative disorder and the leading cause of dementia worldwide, threatening aging societies with a vastly increasing number of patients with dementia, and its economic and social burden. Two disease-modifying therapies (DMTs) targeting amyloid-β (Aβ) pathology, aducanumab and lecanemab, have recently been approved by the FDA for use in the early symptomatic stage of AD [ 1 , 2 , 3 , 4 ]. Another anti-Aβ drug, donanemab, has met the primary and secondary cognitive endpoints in its phase 3 clinical trial [ 5 ]. While the slowing of cognitive decline in response to these therapies was modest, results from the donanemab and lecanemab trials [ 6 , 7 ] suggest that Aβ-targeting DMTs may be more effective in the earliest stages of AD[ 3 , 4 , 6 , 7 ]. This will most likely lead to a shift in the target population of future clinical trials of DMTs to preclinical and prodromal AD. However, recruitment of participants in the earliest stages of AD is challenging due to the low prevalence of preclinical AD in cognitively normal individuals and those with subjective cognitive decline [ 8 ] and the invasiveness and high cost of the current gold standard markers, i.e., amyloid positron emission tomography (PET) scans and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers. The use of emerging blood-based biomarkers in the screening of potential trial participants has been highlighted as an efficient approach to overcome these limitations [ 9 , 10 ]. Although promising results on plasma biomarkers, e.g., Aβ(1–42) (Aβ42) reductions [ 11 , 12 , 13 ] or increases in phosphorylated tau [ 14 ] have recently been reported, the accuracy of the combination of plasma biomarkers in predicting amyloid status in individuals at the preclinical or prodromal stages of AD has not been fully investigated. In addition, the effect of ethnic differences on the predictive power of plasma biomarkers has not been well characterized [ 15 , 16 ]. In this study, we demonstrated the very high performance of the combination of plasma Aβ and p-tau217 to detect brain Aβ-PET positivity in people with early-stage AD in the Japanese J-TRC cohort, which was replicated in the second Caucasian BioFINDER cohort.

Participants were recruited from the J-TRC in-person cohort (J-TRC onsite study), which consists of web-based registry participants (J-TRC webstudy), existing local cohort participants (ORANGE registry)[ 17 ], and outpatients from J-TRC organizing institutions (the University of Tokyo Hospital, National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tohoku University Hospital, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Osaka University Hospital, Kobe University Hospital). Webstudy participants were invited to a face to face study according to the previously reported algorithm [ 18 ]. Individuals with a diagnosis of dementia at enrollment were excluded. Participants were assessed for cognitive and clinical impairment, including the Cognitive Function Instrument (CFI) [ 19 ], the Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite (PACC) [ 20 ], and the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scales. PACC includes MMSE (Mini-Mental State Examination), WMS (Wechsler Memory Scale) Delayed Recall, WAIS-R (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised) Digit Symbol, and FCSRT (Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test). Participants also underwent blood biomarker testing for Aβ(1–40) (Aβ40) and Aβ(1–42) (Aβ42) (Shimadzu), plasma p-tau217 (Eli Lilly), APOE genotyping, and amyloid PET with either [ 18 F]-florbetapir (FBP) or [ 18 F]-flutemetamol (FMM). Participants diagnosed with preclinical or prodromal AD are followed annually until they are referred to appropriate clinical trials. As of December 2023, ~ 14,000 subjects have consented to participate in the webstudy, and 630 have been invited for in-person assessment. In this study, we evaluated 474 subjects enrolled in the J-TRC onsite study from July 2020 to November 2022. From the BioFINDER cohort [ 21 ], we examined 177 participants, all of whom underwent evaluation of Aβ40 and Aβ42 (Shimadzu) and p-tau217 (Eli Lilly) in plasma and Aβ PET imaging with FMM.

Sample collection and plasma biomarker measurements

On the first day of the J-TRC in-person study, 14 mL of blood was collected from each participant and was placed in two 7 mL vacuum tubes containing 10.5 mg of EDTA.2Na and centrifuged (2000 × g, 5 min, 4 ℃) to obtain plasma samples. Aliquots of 3 ml plasma were immediately frozen at -80 °C and transferred to and stored at the Brain Research Institute, Niigata University. Plasma levels of Aβ42 and Aβ40 were measured by Shimadzu Techno-Research Inc (Kyoto, Japan) using an Immunoprecipitation-Mass Spectrometry (IP-MS)-based method as previously described [ 11 , 22 ]. Analysis of plasma p-tau217 was performed using the Meso Scale Discovery (MSD) platform at Eli Lilly and Company [ 21 , 23 ]. In the BioFINDER, plasma levels of Aβ42, Aβ40 and p-tau217 were measured using the same assays as in the J-TRC at Shimadzu Techno-Research (Aβ42 and Aβ40) and Lund University (p-tau217). Details of plasma sampling and biomarker analysis in the BioFINDER are described in previous reports [ 22 , 24 ].

Aβ PET imaging

PET scans using 370 ± 74 MBq of FBP or 185 ± 37 MBq of FMM were performed at baseline in all the J-TRC onsite study participants. Acquisition times were 20 min for FBP and 30 min for FMM, starting 50 min (FBP) or 90 min (FMM) after injection of each tracer, followed by image reconstruction using the parameters determined for each PET camera [ 25 ]. Aβ PET scan results were interpreted visually by two independent nuclear medicine specialists qualified to read amyloid PET scans in accordance with Japanese guidelines and manufacturers’ instructions, and then by a third rater (adjudicator) if the two raters disagreed. We calculated the centiloid scale using the CapAIBL software package for reference only [ 26 ]. In the BioFINDER cohort, scans were acquired 90–110 min after injection of ~ 185 MBq FMM and global standard uptake value ratio (SUVR) values were calculated using the entire cerebellum as the reference region. Aβ PET status was determined by applying a Gaussian mixture model-based threshold of 1.138 to neocortical SUVR values determined in a sample of all BioFINDER 1 participants (N = 445) who underwent FMM PET.

R (version 4.1.0), an open-source software environment, was used for statistical analyses. The chi-square test was used to compare sex, CDR, and the presence of APOE ε4 allele status between groups. The Shapiro–Wilk test was used to test the distribution of numerical variables. The Wilcoxon rank-sum test was used to compare variables with non-normal distributions, and Student’s t-test was used to compare variables with normal and equal distributions between groups. Receiver operator characteristic (ROC) analysis was used to assess the ability of each biomarker to predict Aβ-PET positivity. Cutoff values were determined using a Youden index. The DeLong test was used to compare area under the curve (AUC) metrics from two ROC evaluations. To investigate the improvement in accuracy for predicting Aβ-PET positivity by combining plasma biomarkers with age, sex, and APOE , we applied a logistic regression model. The Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) was calculated to assess model fit. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. The Benjamini–Hochberg correction was applied for multiple comparisons.

The J-TRC cohort

Participants.

The characteristics of the participants from the J-TRC cohort, including the comparison between the Aβ-PET positive and negative groups, are shown in Table  1 . Of the 474 participants in the J-TRC cohort, the visual interpretation by the two raters agreed in 94% of the cases, and 81 were classified as Aβ-PET positive, with global CDR scores of 0 in 331 participants and 0.5 in 143 participants. The rate of APOE ε4 allele carrier (one or two alleles) in the entire cohort was 20.3%. Compared with the Aβ negative group, the Aβ-PET positive group had a significantly higher mean age, prevalence of APOE ε4 allele positivity, and proportion of participants with a CDR 0.5, while no significant differences were found for sex or education. The Aβ-PET positive group also had worse average cognitive scores as assessed by MMSE, WMS Delayed Recall, WAIS-R Digit Symbol, FCSRT, and CFI. As expected, the Aβ-PET positive group had a significantly lower plasma Aβ42/Aβ40 (Aβ42/40) ratio and higher p-tau217 levels (Table  1 a). The CDR 0.5 group had lower educational attainment and more severe cognitive impairment, whereas no differences were found in the rate of APOE ε4 allele carrier compared with the CDR 0 group (Table  1 b).

Prediction of Aβ PET status using plasma biomarkers

We first evaluated the performance of plasma biomarkers, e.g., Aβ42/40 ratio, p-tau217, p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio, and the combination of plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio, to identify individuals with abnormal Aβ-PET scans (Fig.  1 ). In the entire J-TRC cohort, all tested plasma biomarker models showed moderate to high accuracy (AUCs ranging from 0.856 (95%CI: 0.808–0.904) for the Aβ42/40 ratio to 0.920 (95%CI: 0.884–0.957) for the combination of plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio). The AUC values including the blood biomarkers showed higher AUCs compared to those combining the cognitive screening test with clinical information, e.g., MMSE with age, sex, and APOE status (AUC 0.721 (95%CI: 0.660–0.782)). The combination of plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio had a significantly higher AUC than the Aβ42/40 ratio alone (p < 0.001, Table  2 ). We next examined the discriminative accuracy of the plasma biomarkers for Aβ PET status separately in the CDR 0 and CDR 0.5 groups. In the CDR 0 group, all tested plasma biomarker models showed moderate to high accuracy AUCs ranging from 0.876 (95%CI: 0.831–0.922) for Aβ42/40 ratio to 0.938 (95%CI: 0.902–0.975) for the combination of plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio. The combination of plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio showed a significantly higher AUC compared to the other AUCs ( p  = 0.013 for Aβ42/40 ratio, p  = 0.012 for p-tau217, p  = 0.049 for p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio, Table  2 ). In the CDR 0.5 group, plasma biomarkers also showed moderate to high accuracy (AUC ranging from 0.830 (95%CI: 0.740–0.920) for Aβ42/40 ratio to 0.925 (95%CI: 0.881–0.969) for p-tau217). However, in contrast to the results within the entire cohort and the CDR 0 group, the AUCs of the combination of plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio in the CDR 0.5 group were not significantly different from other AUCs (Table  2 ).

figure 1

ROC curve analysis for the detection of amyloid PET positivity. ROC curve analysis in the total participants from the J-TRC cohort (n = 474) ( A ), in the CDR 0 participants from the J-TRC cohort ( n  = 331) ( B ), and in the CDR 0.5 participants from the J-TRC cohort (n = 143) ( C ), in the total participants from the BioFINDER cohort ( n  = 177) ( D ), in the CU participants from the BioFINDER cohort ( n  = 114) ( E ) and in the MCI participants from the BioFINDER cohort ( n  = 63) ( F ). CDR: clinical dementia rating (global score), CU: cognitively unimpaired, MCI: mild cognitive impairment

Prediction of Aβ PET status using plasma biomarkers and age, sex, and APOE

We next tested whether combining age, sex, and APOE genotype with the plasma biomarker improved their ability to detect Aβ-PET positivity, with results shown in Table  2 . The addition of age, sex, and APOE genotype nominally increased the AUCs for all models tested in the J-TRC cohort, regardless of the CDR values (Table  3 ). The DeLong test among all models, e.g., biomarkers alone and a model with clinical information (CI) including age, sex, and APOE , showed that in the CDR 0 group, the combination of p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio with CI was superior to Aβ42/40 or p-tau217 alone and with CI. In contrast, there was no significant difference in AUCs in the CDR 0.5 group (Table  2 ). The ranking of the different models sorted by the AUC is shown in Fig.  2 . The results obtained from the J-TRC cohort showed that the combination of p-tau217 with Aβ42/40 ratio with age, sex, and APOE had the highest AUC for detecting Aβ-PET positivity in the CDR 0 participants, whereas a model combining p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio with age, sex, and APOE was superior to other models in the CDR 0.5 population. Furthermore, in the CDR 0 group, the AIC value of the combination of p-tau217 with Aβ42/40 ratio, or the combination of p-tau217 with Aβ42/40 ratio and CI was lower compared to the models including other plasma biomarkers (Table  3 ). At the same time, the AIC of p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio with CI was the smallest in the CDR 0.5 group. The results of AIC support that these combinations fit better than any other biomarker modeling in each group.

figure 2

Ranking of different biomarker combination models sorted by the AUC. AUC (95% CI) ranking in the total participants from the J-TRC cohort ( n  = 474) ( A ), in the CDR 0 participants from the J-TRC cohort ( n  = 331) ( B ), in the CDR 0.5 participants from the J-TRC cohort ( n  = 143) ( C ), in the total participants from the BioFINDER cohort ( n  = 177) ( D ), in the CU participants from the BioFINDER cohort ( n  = 114) ( E ), in the MCI participants form the BioFINDER cohort ( n  = 63) ( F ). Dotted lines represent the results for biomarkers, while solid lines represent the results for biomarkers with clinical information. AUC values are shown in the right side of each line. CI: clinical information including age, sex, and APOE , CDR: clinical dementia rating (global score), CU: cognitively unimpaired, MCI: mild cognitive impairment

Validation in the BioFINDER cohort

We then sought to validate the results from the J-TRC cohort using data from the Swedish BioFINDER study (Tables 1 , 4 and 5 , Fig.  2 D, E, and F). The relative proportions of the cognitively unimpaired (equal to CDR 0) and MCI (Mild cognitive impairment) (equal to CDR 0.5) were not different compared to J-TRC. The proportions of the Aβ-PET positive participants and APOE ε4-positive participants were higher in the BioFINDER cohort. ROC analysis showed that the four variables had moderate to high accuracy, similar to the J-TRC cohort (Fig.  1 ). In the cognitively unimpaired group, the combination of p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio showed a higher AUC compared to p-tau217 and p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio (Table  4 ). In the MCI group, there were no significant differences in AUCs between the tested biomarker models. These results were similar to those in the J-TRC cohort (Tables  2 and 3 ). The results of the ROC analysis (Fig.  2 D, E, and F) showed that the combination of p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio with CI had the highest AUC in the cognitively unimpaired group, followed by p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio. This order was identical to that observed in the CDR 0 group of the J-TRC cohort. In the MCI group, the p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio with CI exhibited the highest AUC similar to the findings in the J-TRC. In the cognitively unimpaired group, the AIC value of the combination of p-tau217 with Aβ42/40 ratio, or the combination of p-tau217 with Aβ42/40 ratio and CI, was smaller compared to the models including other plasma biomarkers, as in J-TRC (Table  5 ). Also, the AIC of p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio with CI was the smallest in the MCI group. These results showed that each model fit better compared to other models, similar to the J-TRC cohort. However, some differences were observed in the performance of variables between J-TRC and BioFINDER in the MCI group: p-tau217 was the third best in the J-TRC, whereas it was the seventh best in the BioFINDER cohort (AUC: 0.925 and 0.829, respectively).

In this study, we have shown that the plasma Aβ42/40 ratio determined by IP/MS and plasma p-tau217 measured by MSD immunoassay are biomarkers that predict brain Aβ PET positivity in the Japanese population of non-demented individuals, and that a combination of these Aβ42 and p-tau217 markers showed unprecedented high discriminative values with an AUC of ~ 0.93, which was reproduced in the European BioFINDER cohort. Emerging evidence supports the importance of newer blood-based markers in the detection of cerebral Aβ pathology [ 10 , 22 ]. In a head-to-head comparison study of several plasma Aβ assays, MS-based plasma Aβ biomarkers were reported to be generally superior to immunoassays in detecting abnormal brain amyloid status: IP-MS method developed by Washington University showed the highest AUC of 0.852 for CSF Aβ42/40 status in cognitively unimpaired and MCI subjects, which was improved to 0.882 by the addition of APOE genotype [ 22 ]. Here we showed that the plasma Aβ42/40 ratio, as determined by the Shimadzu-developed IP-MS assay, predicted brain Aβ PET positivity in the J-TRC cohort, which enrolled non-demented elderly individuals by consecutive recruitment through web-based participation and local cohort or memory clinic, thus reflecting the characteristics of elderly individuals in the general population. Plasma Aβ42/40 measures have been shown to be highly discriminative of CSF Aβ42/40 and Aβ PET status, as well as predictive of cognitive decline and progression from cognitively unimpaired to MCI and from MCI to AD [ 11 , 12 , 27 ]. Changes in the plasma Aβ42/40 ratio precede elevated amyloid levels detected by PET scans, similar to those in CSF [ 9 , 28 ]. Notably, plasma Aβ42/40 showed moderate accuracy in both the cognitively unimpaired (CDR 0) and the MCI (CDR 0.5) subjects, while the accuracy of the plasma p-tau217 was higher, and in the CDR 0.5 group, p-tau217 showed high accuracy (AUC 0.925). It has been well documented that high levels of plasma p-tau, especially p-tau217, are associated with abnormal Aβ PET and CSF Aβ42/40 in different stages of AD [ 9 , 21 , 28 , 29 ]. Elevated plasma p-tau levels have been shown to be highly specific for brain amyloid deposition, allowing the differentiation of AD from non-AD dementia [ 21 , 23 , 30 , 31 , 32 ]. Furthermore, a recent study suggests that plasma p-tau has a strong surrogacy in preclinical and prodromal AD compared to Aβ42/40 ratio or p-tau231 [ 33 ]. The superiority of p-tau217 over Aβ42/40 ratio, especially in the prodromal population, is confirmed in BioFINDER. Furthermore, our study suggested an interesting difference in the predictive ability between Aβ42/40 ratio and p-tau217. As shown in Table  3 , Aβ42/40 ratio showed relatively higher sensitivity (0.904) and lower specificity (0.733) in the CDR 0 group, while the results were opposite in the CDR 0.5 group (0.743, 0.875, respectively). P-tau217 showed the opposite result (sensitivity 0.761, specificity 0.896 in the CDR 0 group while 0.897 and 0.826, respectively, in the CDR 0.5 group). These results indicate that when blood biomarker is used as a pre-screen for amyloid PET, p-tau217 reduces false positive compared to Aβ42/40 ratio in the CDR 0 group, while Aβ42/40 ratio may be better compared to p-tau217 in the CDR 0.5 group. These results are not replicated in the BioFINDER cohort, where p-tau217 showed higher specificity compared to Aβ42/40 ratio regardless of group. This difference may be due to the difference in disease progression between CDR 0.5 in J-TRC and MCI in BioFINDER, as shown by the plasma p-tau217 level being higher in MCI in BioFINDER (0.28 ± 0.15) compared to CDR 0.5 group in J-TRC (0.24 ± 0.18).

Our results also suggest an intriguing difference in the optimal combination of plasma Aβ42, Aβ42/40, p-tau217 and clinical information in detecting abnormal Aβ PET between the CDR 0 and 0.5 populations. The combination of p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio showed the best performance in the CDR 0 population, while the p-tau217/Aβ42 ratio performed best in the CDR 0.5 group. These results were reproduced in BioFINDER. One interpretation of these findings would be that the plasma Aβ42/40 ratio and p-tau217 gradually change with the progression of brain amyloid accumulation before the threshold of Aβ-PET positivity is reached; the optimal combination of Aβ and p-tau markers may change with disease progression. The benefit of combining plasma Aβ and p-tau biomarkers has not been extensively studied. The superiority of the combination of high performance plasma p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 assays to identify brain Aβ positivity, predict the presence of AD neuropathologic changes [ 34 ], Aβ PET centiloid metric [ 35 ], and future development of AD dementia has only been reported [ 36 ]. Our study demonstrated that the combination of p-tau217 and Aβ42/40 ratio, and these with age, sex, and APOE genotype, is a useful tool for predicting Aβ-PET positivity in the cognitively unimpaired or preclinical population. The contribution of each variable to the modeling is shown by a nomogram in Supplementary Figs.  1 and 2 .

In many of the previous studies, participants recruited from regional or clinical cohorts included individuals at high-risk for AD with a higher prevalence of the APOE ε4 allele (e.g., 45.2% in AHEAD 3–45 study [ 35 ], 47.5% in the BioFINDER study [ 23 ], 53.9% in the ALFA + cohort [ 32 ] and 47.8% in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention cohort [ 31 ]), whereas the APOE ε4 positivity in the J-TRC cohort was 20.2%, which is close to that of the general population in Japan and Asia [ 37 ]. Our study used Aβ-PET positivity as the standard of truth, which was determined by visual interpretation in J-TRC and rated with quantitative measures in BioFINDER. While quantitative methods are popular in Europe, visual assessment is the standard in Japan, as indicated by Japanese guidelines and regulations. As a result, there is variation in amyloid PET interpretation criteria depending on the research protocol. This study examined the reproducibility in two cohorts, each with its own protocol including Aβ-PET rating method. In general, visual and quantitative ratings are known to provide comparable results, allowing for error in borderline cases and taking into account possible differences in case of localized uptake. In this study, the inter-rater variability of the visual reading of the J-TRC was low; the two independent raters agreed in 94% of cases. Also, quantitative measures are not free from variation due to bias, e.g., selection of a software program and a cutoff level. Therefore, the difference in PET rating method between the two cohorts would not substantially affect the conclusions of our study.

Our results may be relevant to the use of plasma biomarkers for prescreening in clinical trials of preclinical AD populations in the real world. Our study also addresses the potential impact of ethnic factors on the usability of plasma biomarkers [ 15 , 38 ]. Our results showed high performance of the combination of plasma Aβ42, Aβ42/40 ratio, and p-tau217 in non-demented elderly individuals in Asian-Japanese as well as Western populations. Thus, these biomarkers could greatly facilitate the prescreening of participants in global preclinical AD trials that require the enrollment of participants from diverse ethnicities. In addition, these blood-based biomarkers may play an important role in the early detection of individuals at high risk of developing AD and for the early and appropriate diagnosis of cognitive decline or dementia due to AD.

This study has several limitations. Other promising plasma biomarkers such as other phosphorylated tau species (e.g., p-tau231), phosphorylated/non-phosphorylated tau ratio, GFAP, NFL should also be investigated. Our study population of CDR 0.5 in J-TRC and MCI in BioFINDER is relatively small. The rate of APOE ε4 allele carriers is not the same in the two cohorts. Thus, the generalizability of our findings, e.g., the difference of optimal combination for cognitively impaired or MCI population in the real-world setting, should be verified in future studies.

This study demonstrated a high accuracy of the combination of plasma Aβ markers and p-tau217 to detect Aβ-PET positivity in preclinical and prodromal AD in the Japanese trial-ready cohort, which was replicated in the Swedish BioFINDER cohort. These results provide us with optimal indices to identify potential participants and minimize the financial and physical burden of clinical trials of DMTs in the very early stages of AD.

Availability of data and materials

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Abbreviations

Alzheimer’s disease

Akaike information criterion

Area under the curve

Clinical dementia rating

Cognitive function instrument

Clinical information

Cerebrospinal fluid

Disease-modifying therapy

[ 18 F]-florbetapir

Free and cued selective reminding test

[ 18 F]-flutemetamol

Immunoprecipitation-mass spectrometry

Japanese trial-ready cohort for preclinical and prodromal AD

Mild cognitive impairment

Mini-mental state examination

Meso scale discovery

Preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite

Positron emission tomography

Receiver operator characteristic

Standard uptake value ratio

Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised

Wechsler memory scale

Cummings J, Aisen P, Apostolova LG, Atri A, Salloway S, Weiner M. Aducanumab: Appropriate Use Recommendations. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2021;8(4):398–410.

CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Cummings J, Apostolova L, Rabinovici GD, Atri A, Aisen P, Greenberg S, et al. Lecanemab: Appropriate Use Recommendations. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2023;10(3):362–77.

Budd Haeberlein S, Aisen PS, Barkhof F, Chalkias S, Chen T, Cohen S, et al. Two Randomized Phase 3 Studies of Aducanumab in Early Alzheimer’s Disease. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2022;9(2):197–210.

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

van Dyck CH, Swanson CJ, Aisen P, Bateman RJ, Chen C, Gee M, et al. Lecanemab in Early Alzheimer’s Disease. N Engl J Med. 2023;388(1):9–21.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Sims JR, Zimmer JA, Evans CD, Lu M, Ardayfio P, Sparks J, et al. Donanemab in Early Symptomatic Alzheimer Disease: The Trailblazer-alz 2 randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2023;330(6):512–27.

Sims JR, Iwatsubo T, Greenberg SM, Mintun M, Atri A, Zimmer JA, et al. S2- Donanemab In early symptomatic alzheimer’s disease: additional insights from TRAILBLAZER-ALZ 2. 16th Clinical Trials on alzheimer’s disease (CTAD) Boston, MA (USA) October 24–27, 2023: Symposia. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2023;10(1):S6–7.

Van Dyck CH, Johnson K, Sperling R, Irizarry M. S4- Lecanemab for early alzheimer’ s disease:long-term outcomes, predictive biomarkers and novel subcutaneous administration. 16th Clinical trials on alzheimer’s disease (CTAD) Boston, MA (USA) October 24–27, 2023: Symposia. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2023;10(1):S9–11.

Parnetti L, Chipi E, Salvadori N, D’Andrea K, Eusebi P. Prevalence and risk of progression of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease stages: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):7.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Leuzy A, Mattsson-Carlgren N, Palmqvist S, Janelidze S, Dage JL, Hansson O. Blood-based biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. EMBO Mol Med. 2022;14(1): e14408.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Teunissen CE, Verberk IMW, Thijssen EH, Vermunt L, Hansson O, Zetterberg H, et al. Blood-based biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease: towards clinical implementation. Lancet Neurol. 2022;21(1):66–77.

Nakamura A, Kaneko N, Villemagne VL, Kato T, Doecke J, Doré V, et al. High performance plasma amyloid-β biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. Nature. 2018;554(7691):249–54.

Schindler SE, Bollinger JG, Ovod V, Mawuenyega KG, Li Y, Gordon BA, et al. High-precision plasma β-amyloid 42/40 predicts current and future brain amyloidosis. Neurology. 2019;93(17):e1647–59.

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Pannee J, Törnqvist U, Westerlund A, Ingelsson M, Lannfelt L, Brinkmalm G, et al. The amyloid-β degradation pattern in plasma—A possible tool for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 2014;573:7–12.

Gonzalez-Ortiz F, Kac PR, Brum WS, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, Karikari TK. Plasma phospho-tau in Alzheimer’s disease: towards diagnostic and therapeutic trial applications. Mol Neurodegener. 2023;18(1):18.

Schindler SE, Karikari TK, Ashton NJ, Henson RL, Yarasheski KE, West T, et al. Effect of Race on Prediction of Brain Amyloidosis by Plasma Aβ42/Aβ40, Phosphorylated Tau, and Neurofilament Light. Neurology. 2022;99(3):e245–57.

Hajjar I, Yang Z, Okafor M, Liu C, Waligorska T, Goldstein FC, et al. Association of Plasma and Cerebrospinal Fluid Alzheimer Disease Biomarkers With Race and the Role of Genetic Ancestry, Vascular Comorbidities, and Neighborhood Factors. JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(10): e2235068.

Saji N, Sakurai T, Suzuki K, Mizusawa H, Toba K. ORANGE’s challenge: developing wide-ranging dementia research in Japan. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15(7):661–2.

Sato K, Ihara R, Suzuki K, Niimi Y, Toda T, Jimenez-Maggiora G, et al. Predicting amyloid risk by machine learning algorithms based on the A4 screen data: Application to the Japanese Trial-Ready Cohort study. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2021;7(1): e12135.

Walsh SP, Raman R, Jones KB, Aisen PS. ADCS Prevention Instrument Project: the Mail-In Cognitive Function Screening Instrument (MCFSI). Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20(4 Suppl 3):S170–8.

Donohue MC, Sperling RA, Salmon DP, Rentz DM, Raman R, Thomas RG, et al. The preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite: measuring amyloid-related decline. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(8):961–70.

Palmqvist S, Janelidze S, Quiroz YT, Zetterberg H, Lopera F, Stomrud E, et al. Discriminative Accuracy of Plasma Phospho-tau217 for Alzheimer Disease vs Other Neurodegenerative Disorders. JAMA. 2020;324(8):772–81.

Janelidze S, Teunissen CE, Zetterberg H, Allué JA, Sarasa L, Eichenlaub U, et al. Head-to-Head Comparison of 8 Plasma Amyloid-β 42/40 Assays in Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(11):1375–82.

Janelidze S, Berron D, Smith R, Strandberg O, Proctor NK, Dage JL, et al. Associations of Plasma Phospho-Tau217 Levels With Tau Positron Emission Tomography in Early Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(2):149–56.

Janelidze S, Palmqvist S, Leuzy A, Stomrud E, Verberk IMW, Zetterberg H, et al. Detecting amyloid positivity in early Alzheimer’s disease using combinations of plasma Aβ42/Aβ40 and p-tau. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;18(2):283–93.

Ikari Y, Akamatsu G, Nishio T, Ishii K, Ito K, Iwatsubo T, et al. Phantom criteria for qualification of brain FDG and amyloid PET across different cameras. EJNMMI Phys. 2016;3(1):23.

Klunk WE, Koeppe RA, Price JC, Benzinger TL, Devous MD Sr, Jagust WJ, et al. The Centiloid Project: Standardizing quantitative amyloid plaque estimation by PET. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(1):1–15.e4.

Hanon O, Vidal JS, Lehmann S, Bombois S, Allinquant B, Baret-Rose C, et al. Plasma amyloid beta predicts conversion to dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: The BALTAZAR study. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;18(12):2537–50.

Angioni D, Delrieu J, Hansson O, Fillit H, Aisen P, Cummings J, et al. Blood Biomarkers from Research Use to Clinical Practice: What Must Be Done? A Report from the EU/US CTAD Task Force. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2022;9(4):569–79.

Thijssen EH, La Joie R, Strom A, Fonseca C, Iaccarino L, Wolf A, et al. Plasma phosphorylated tau 217 and phosphorylated tau 181 as biomarkers in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a retrospective diagnostic performance study. Lancet Neurol. 2021;20(9):739–52.

Janelidze S, Bali D, Ashton NJ, Barthélemy NR, Vanbrabant J, Stoops E, et al. Head-to-head comparison of 10 plasma phospho-tau assays in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2023;146(4):1592–601.

Jonaitis EM, Janelidze S, Cody KA, Langhough R, Du L, Chin NA, et al. Plasma phosphorylated tau 217 in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Commun. 2023;5(2):fcad057.

Milà-Alomà M, Ashton NJ, Shekari M, Salvadó G, Ortiz-Romero P, Montoliu-Gaya L, et al. Plasma p-tau231 and p-tau217 as state markers of amyloid-β pathology in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Med. 2022;28(9):1797–801.

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Ashton NJ, Janelidze S, Mattsson-Carlgren N, Binette AP, Strandberg O, Brum WS, et al. Differential roles of Aβ42/40, p-tau231 and p-tau217 for Alzheimer’s trial selection and disease monitoring. Nat Med. 2022;28(12):2555–62.

Salvadó G, Ossenkoppele R, Ashton NJ, Beach TG, Serrano GE, Reiman EM, et al. Specific associations between plasma biomarkers and postmortem amyloid plaque and tau tangle loads. EMBO Mol Med. 2023;15(5): e17123.

Rissman RA, Langford O, Raman R, Donohue MC, Abdel-Latif S, Meyer MR, et al. Plasma Aβ42/Aβ40 and phospho-tau217 concentration ratios increase the accuracy of amyloid PET classification in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2024;20(2):1214–24.

Palmqvist S, Stomrud E, Cullen N, Janelidze S, Manuilova E, Jethwa A, et al. An accurate fully automated panel of plasma biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2023;19(4):1204–15.

Miyashita A, Kikuchi M, Hara N, Ikeuchi T. Genetics of Alzheimer’s disease: an East Asian perspective. J Hum Genet. 2023;68(3):115–24.

Raman R, Quiroz YT, Langford O, Choi J, Ritchie M, Baumgartner M, et al. Disparities by Race and Ethnicity Among Adults Recruited for a Preclinical Alzheimer Disease Trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(7): e2114364.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Drs. Paul Aisen, Gustavo Jimenez-Miggiora, Robert Rissman, Rema Raman and Reisa Sperling for their kind collaboration through the TRC-PAD study, Kazunari Ishii, Yoshitaka Inui and Etsuko Imabayashi for their contribution in PET diagnosis, Kenji Toba and Katsuhiko Yanagisawa for making the data analyzed in the ORANGE registry available, Hiroyuki Arai, Yasuyuki Taki, Taizen Nakase, Harumasa Takano, Yoshie Omachi, Keisuke Suzuki, Toshihisa Tanaka, Manabu Ikeda, Riki Matsumoto, Tatsuya Maruyama, Takashi Moritoyo for their contribution in the J-TRC onsite study, and Yutaka Matsuyama for supervision in statistical analysis. We thank Shimadzu Techno-Research inc and Dr. Shinichi Iwamoto for their help in plasma biomarker analysis, Micron inc and Mr. Yasuhiko Ikari in support of PET imaging analyses, IQVIA Japan in the management of clinical studies, SRL for their assistance in sample handling, Dr. Rachael Burchfield for assistance with p-tau217 data acquisition, and Mr. Naohisa Hatakeyama and Dr. Jeffrey Dage for their help in p-tau analysis.

This study was supported by the Japanese Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED), Grant Number JP19dk0207048h001, JP20dk0207048h002, JP21dk0207048h0003, JP22dk0207048h004, JP23dk0207048h005, JP23dk0207057, and by a grant from an anonymous Foundation. The BioFINDER study was supported by by the National Institute of Aging (R01AG083740), Alzheimer’s Association (SG-23–1061717), Swedish Research Council (2022–00775), ERA PerMed (ERAPERMED2021-184), the Knut and Alice Wallenberg foundation (2017–0383), the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (AF-980907), the Swedish Brain Foundation (FO2021-0293), The Parkinson foundation of Sweden (1412/22), the Cure Alzheimer’s fund, the Konung Gustaf V:s och Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation (2020-O000028), Regionalt Forskningsstöd (2022–1259) and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement (2022-Projekt0080).

Author information

Yoshiki Niimi and Shorena Janelidze contributed equally in this work.

Authors and Affiliations

Unit for Early and Exploratory Clinical Development, The University of Tokyo Hospital, 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-Ku, Tokyo, 113-0033, Japan

Yoshiki Niimi, Kenichiro Sato, Oskar Hansson & Takeshi Iwatsubo

Clinical Memory Research Unit, Department of Clinical Sciences Malmö, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden

Shorena Janelidze

Department of Neuropathology, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan

Kenichiro Sato & Takeshi Iwatsubo

Department of Aging Research and Geriatric Medicine, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

Naoki Tomita

Department of Geriatric Medicine and Neuroimaging, Tohoku University Hospital, Sendai, Japan

Department of Neurology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan

Tadashi Tsukamoto

Department of Clinical and Experimental Neuroimaging, National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Aichi, Japan

Takashi Kato, Kengo Ito & Akinori Nakamura

Department of Psychiatry, Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan

Kenji Yoshiyama

Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kobe University, Hyogo, Japan

Hisatomo Kowa

Department of Neurology, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Tokyo, Japan

Atsushi Iwata & Ryoko Ihara

Division of Neurology, Internal Medicine, National Defense Medical College, Saitama, Japan

Kazushi Suzuki

Department of Molecular Genetics, Brain Research Institute, Niigata University, Niigata, Japan

Kensaku Kasuga & Takeshi Ikeuchi

Integrated Research Initiative for Living Well With Dementia, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute for Geriatric and Gerontology, Tokyo, Japan

Kenji Ishii

Department of Molecular Imaging Research, Kobe City Medical Center General Hospital, Hyogo, Japan

Michio Senda

Eli Lilly and Company, Indianapolis, USA

Theresa A. Day, Samantha C. Burnham, Leonardo Iaccarino & Michael J. Pontecorvo

Memory Clinic, Skåne University Hospital, Lund, Sweden

Oskar Hansson

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

YN, SJ, OH, and TIw had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: OH, TIw. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: YN, SJ, KSa, NT, TT, TK, KY, HK, AI, RI, KSu, KK, KIs, KIt, AN, MS, TAD, SB, LI, MP, OH, TIw. Drafting of the manuscript: YN, SJ, OH, TIw. Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content: TK, AI, KSu, TIk, KIs, KIt, AN, MS, MP. Statistical analysis: YN, SJ, KSa. Obtained funding: AI, AN, OH, TIw. Administrative, technical, or material support: TAD, SB, LI, MP. Supervision: AI, TIk, KIs, KIt, AN, MS, OH, TIw.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Oskar Hansson or Takeshi Iwatsubo .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The study was conducted in accordance with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and subsequent amendments, and all participants provided written informed consent. The J-TRC study is approved by the ethical review boards of the University of Tokyo and each collaborating institute, and BioFINDER is approved by the Regional Ethics Committee in Lund, Sweden.

Competing interests

YN has received consultancy/speaker fees from Eisai, and Eli Lilly and Company. SJ reported grants from the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation outside the submitted work. TK has received speaker fees from Eisai, PDR Pharmaceutical, and Nihon Medi-Physics. AI has acquired research support from Janssen pharmaceuticals, Fujirebio, Sysmex, Chugai Pharmaceuticals, Kobayashi pharmaceuticals, Dream medical partners, Sound Innovation, and SONY, and received consultancy/speaker fees from Eisai, Daiichi Sankyo, Kowa, HU frontier, Janssen pharmaceuticals, Eli Lilly and Company, MSD, Biogen, IQVIA, owns stock of Eisai. IR has received speaker fees from Eisai. TIk has received research grant for the institution from Fujirebio, Eli Lilly and Eisai. In the past 3 years, TIk has received consultancy/speaker fees from Eisai, Eli Lilly, Fujirebio, Novo Nordisk, Daiichi Sankyo, Chugai, Roche and FDR Pharma. KIs has acquired research support from GE Healthcare and Nihon Medi-Physics, consultancy/speaker fees from Nihon Medi-Physics, Eisai, and Eli Lilly. KIt has received consultancy/speaker fees from Eisai, and Eli Lilly. AN has received speaker fee from Eisai, Towa, PDRadiopharma and Nihon Medi-Physics. MS has acquired research support (for the institution) from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Eli Lilly Japan, GE Healthcare, Cerveau Technologies, Meilleur Technologies, Eisai, Biogen, Janssen Pharma, and Sumitomo Heavy Industries, as well as received consultancy fees (for the institution) from Eli Lilly Japan. TAD, SB, LI and MP are full-time employees and minor stockholders of Eli Lilly and company. OH has acquired research support (for the institution) from ADx, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, Biogen, Eli Lilly and Company, Eisai, Fujirebio, GE Healthcare, Pfizer, and Roche. In the past 2 years, OH has received consultancy/speaker fees from AC Immune, Amylyx, Alzpath, BioArctic, Biogen, Bristol Meyer Squibb, Cerveau, Eisai, Eli Lilly and Company, Fujirebio, Merck, Novartis, Novo Nordisk, Roche, Sanofi and Siemens. TIw has received consultancy/speaker fee from Biogen, Eisai, Eli-Lilly and Company, and Roche/Chugai. No other disclosures were reported.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

13195_2024_1469_moesm1_esm.pptx.

Supplementary Material 1. Supplemental Figure 1 Nomograms for the logistic regression analysis to detect Aβ-PET positivity in the J-TRC cohort. CI: clinical information including age, sex, and APOE , CDR: clinical dementia rating (global score). Supplemental Figure 2 Nomograms for the logistic regression analysis to detect Aβ-PET positivity in the BioFINDER cohort. CI: clinical information including age, sex, and APOE , CU: cognitively unimpaired, MCI: mild cognitive impairment.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Niimi, Y., Janelidze, S., Sato, K. et al. Combining plasma Aβ and p-tau217 improves detection of brain amyloid in non-demented elderly. Alz Res Therapy 16 , 115 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-024-01469-w

Download citation

Received : 12 March 2024

Accepted : 29 April 2024

Published : 23 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-024-01469-w

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Blood-based biomarker
  • Amyloid positron emission tomography

Alzheimer's Research & Therapy

ISSN: 1758-9193

what is basic research study

Vegetables and Fruits

Basket of food including grapes apples asparagus onions lettuce carrots melon bananas corn

  • Vegetables and fruits are an important part of a healthy diet, and variety is as important as quantity.
  • No single fruit or vegetable provides all of the nutrients you need to be healthy. Eat plenty every day.

A diet rich in vegetables and fruits can lower blood pressure, reduce the risk of heart disease and stroke, prevent some types of cancer, lower risk of eye and digestive problems, and have a positive effect upon blood sugar, which can help keep appetite in check. Eating non-starchy vegetables and fruits like apples, pears, and green leafy vegetables may even promote weight loss. [1] Their low glycemic loads prevent blood sugar spikes that can increase hunger.

At least nine different families of fruits and vegetables exist, each with potentially hundreds of different plant compounds that are beneficial to health. Eat a variety of types and colors of produce in order to give your body the mix of nutrients it needs. This not only ensures a greater diversity of beneficial plant chemicals but also creates eye-appealing meals.

what is basic research study

Tips to eat more vegetables and fruits each day

  • Keep fruit where you can see it . Place several ready-to-eat washed whole fruits in a bowl or store chopped colorful fruits in a glass bowl in the refrigerator to tempt a sweet tooth.
  • Explore the produce aisle and choose something new . Variety and color are key to a healthy diet. On most days, try to get at least one serving from each of the following categories: dark green leafy vegetables; yellow or orange fruits and vegetables; red fruits and vegetables; legumes (beans) and peas; and citrus fruits.
  • Skip the potatoes . Choose other vegetables that are packed with different nutrients and more slowly digested  carbohydrates .
  • Make it a meal . Try cooking new  recipes that include more vegetables. Salads, soups, and stir-fries are just a few ideas for increasing the number of tasty vegetables in your meals.

what is basic research study

5 common questions about fruits and vegetables.

Vegetables, fruits, and disease, cardiovascular disease.

There is compelling evidence that a diet rich in fruits and vegetables can lower the risk of heart disease and stroke.

  • A meta-analysis of cohort studies following 469,551 participants found that a higher intake of fruits and vegetables is associated with a reduced risk of death from cardiovascular disease, with an average reduction in risk of 4% for each additional serving per day of fruit and vegetables. [2]
  • The largest and longest study to date, done as part of the Harvard-based Nurses’ Health Study and Health Professionals Follow-up Study, included almost 110,000 men and women whose health and dietary habits were followed for 14 years.
  • The higher the average daily intake of fruits and vegetables, the lower the chances of developing cardiovascular disease. Compared with those in the lowest category of fruit and vegetable intake (less than 1.5 servings a day), those who averaged 8 or more servings a day were 30% less likely to have had a heart attack or stroke. [3]
  • Although all fruits and vegetables likely contributed to this benefit, green leafy vegetables, such as lettuce, spinach, Swiss chard, and mustard greens, were most strongly associated with decreased risk of cardiovascular disease. Cruciferous vegetables such as broccoli, cauliflower, cabbage, Brussels sprouts , bok choy, and kale ; and citrus fruits such as oranges, lemons, limes, and grapefruit (and their juices) also made important contributions. [3]
  • When researchers combined findings from the Harvard studies with several other long-term studies in the U.S. and Europe, and looked at coronary heart disease and stroke separately, they found a similar protective effect: Individuals who ate more than 5 servings of fruits and vegetables per day had roughly a 20% lower risk of coronary heart disease [4] and stroke, [5] compared with individuals who ate less than 3 servings per day.

Blood pressure

  • The  Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) study [6] examined the effect on blood pressure of a diet that was rich in fruits, vegetables, and low-fat dairy products and that restricted the amount of saturated and total fat. The researchers found that people with high blood pressure who followed this diet reduced their systolic blood pressure (the upper number of a blood pressure reading) by about 11 mm Hg and their diastolic blood pressure (the lower number) by almost 6 mm Hg—as much as medications can achieve.
  • A randomized trial known as the Optimal Macronutrient Intake Trial for Heart Health (OmniHeart) showed that this fruit and vegetable-rich diet lowered blood pressure even more when some of the carbohydrate was replaced with healthy unsaturated fat or protein. [7]
  • In 2014 a meta-analysis of clinical trials and observational studies found that consumption of a vegetarian diet was associated with lower blood pressure. [8]

Numerous early studies revealed what appeared to be a strong link between eating fruits and vegetables and protection against cancer . Unlike case-control studies, cohort studies , which follow large groups of initially healthy individuals for years, generally provide more reliable information than case-control studies because they don’t rely on information from the past. And, in general, data from cohort studies have not consistently shown that a diet rich in fruits and vegetables prevents cancer.

  • For example, over a 14-year period in the Nurses’ Health Study and the Health Professionals Follow-up Study, men and women with the highest intake of fruits and vegetables (8+ servings a day) were just as likely to have developed cancer as those who ate the fewest daily servings (under 1.5). [3]
  • A meta-analysis of cohort studies found that a higher fruit and vegetable intake did not decrease the risk of deaths from cancer. [2]

A more likely possibility is that some types of fruits and vegetables may protect against certain cancers.

  • A study by Farvid and colleagues followed a Nurses’ Health Study II cohort of 90,476 premenopausal women for 22 years and found that those who ate the most fruit during adolescence (about 3 servings a day) compared with those who ate the lowest intakes (0.5 servings a day) had a 25% lower risk of developing breast cancer. There was a significant reduction in breast cancer in women who had eaten higher intakes of apples, bananas , grapes, and corn during adolescence, and oranges and kale during early adulthood. No protection was found from drinking fruit juices at younger ages. [9]
  • Farvid and colleagues followed 90, 534 premenopausal women from the Nurses’ Health Study II over 20 years and found that higher fiber intakes during adolescence and early adulthood were associated with a reduced risk of breast cancer later in life. When comparing the highest and lowest fiber intakes from fruits and vegetables, women with the highest fruit fiber intake had a 12% reduced risk of breast cancer; those with the highest vegetable fiber intake had an 11% reduced risk. [10]
  • After following 182,145 women in the Nurses’ Health Study I and II for 30 years, Farvid’s team also found that women who ate more than 5.5 servings of fruits and vegetables each day (especially cruciferous and yellow/orange vegetables) had an 11% lower risk of breast cancer than those who ate 2.5 or fewer servings. Vegetable intake was strongly associated with a 15% lower risk of estrogen-receptor-negative tumors for every two additional servings of vegetables eaten daily. A higher intake of fruits and vegetables was associated with a lower risk of other aggressive tumors including HER2-enriched and basal-like tumors. [11]
  • A report by the World Cancer Research Fund and the American Institute for Cancer Research suggests that non-starchy vegetables—such as lettuce and other leafy greens, broccoli, bok choy, cabbage, as well as garlic, onions, and the like—and fruits “probably” protect against several types of cancers, including those of the mouth, throat, voice box, esophagus, and stomach. Fruit probably also protects against lung cancer. [12]

Specific components of fruits and vegetables may also be protective against cancer. For example:

  • A line of research stemming from a finding from the Health Professionals Follow-up Study suggests that tomatoes may help protect men against prostate cancer, especially aggressive forms of it. [12] One of the pigments that give tomatoes their red hue—lycopene—could be involved in this protective effect. Although several studies other than the Health Professionals Study have also demonstrated a link between tomatoes or lycopene and prostate cancer, others have not or have found only a weak connection. [14]
  • Taken as a whole, however, these studies suggest that increased consumption of tomato-based products (especially cooked tomato products) and other lycopene-containing foods may reduce the occurrence of prostate cancer. [12] Lycopene is one of several carotenoids (compounds that the body can turn into vitamin A) found in brightly colored fruits and vegetables, and research suggests that foods containing carotenoids may protect against lung, mouth, and throat cancer. [12] But more research is needed to understand the exact relationship between fruits and vegetables, carotenoids, and cancer.

Some research looks specifically at whether individual fruits are associated with risk of type 2 diabetes. While there isn’t an abundance of research into this area yet, preliminary results are compelling.

  • A study of over 66,000 women in the Nurses’ Health Study, 85,104 women from the Nurses’ Health Study II, and 36,173 men from the Health Professionals Follow-up Study—who were free of major chronic diseases—found that greater consumption of whole fruits—especially blueberries, grapes, and apples—was associated with a lower risk of type 2 diabetes. Another important finding was that greater consumption of fruit juice was associated with a higher risk of type 2 diabetes. [15]
  • Additionally a study of over 70,000 female nurses aged 38-63 years, who were free of cardiovascular disease, cancer, and diabetes, showed that consumption of green leafy vegetables and fruit was associated with a lower risk of diabetes. While not conclusive, research also indicated that consumption of fruit juices may be associated with an increased risk among women. (16)
  • A study of over 2,300 Finnish men showed that vegetables and fruits, especially berries, may reduce the risk of type 2 diabetes. [17]

Data from the Nurses’ Health Studies and the Health Professional’s Follow-up Study show that women and men who increased their intakes of fruits and vegetables over a 24-year period were more likely to have lost weight than those who ate the same amount or those who decreased their intake. Berries, apples, pears, soy, and cauliflower were associated with weight loss while starchier vegetables like potatoes, corn, and peas were linked with weight gain. [1] However, keep in mind that adding more produce into the diet won’t necessarily help with weight loss unless it replaces another food, such as refined carbohydrates of white bread and crackers.

Gastrointestinal health

Fruits and vegetables contain indigestible fiber, which absorbs water and expands as it passes through the digestive system. This can calm symptoms of an irritable bowel and, by triggering regular bowel movements, can relieve or prevent constipation. [18] The bulking and softening action of insoluble fiber also decreases pressure inside the intestinal tract and may help prevent diverticulosis. [19]

Eating fruits and vegetables can also keep your eyes healthy, and may help prevent two common aging-related eye diseases—cataracts and macular degeneration—which afflict millions of Americans over age 65. [20-23] Lutein and zeaxanthin, in particular, seem to reduce risk of cataracts. [24]

  • Bertoia ML, Mukamal KJ, Cahill LE, Hou T, Ludwig DS, Mozaffarian D, Willett WC, Hu FB, Rimm EB. Changes in intake of fruits and vegetables and weight change in United States men and women followed for up to 24 years: analysis from three prospective cohort studies. PLoS medicine . 2015 Sep 22;12(9):e1001878.
  • Wang X, Ouyang Y, Liu J, Zhu M, Zhao G, Bao W, Hu FB. Fruit and vegetable consumption and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer: systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ . 2014 Jul 29;349:g4490.
  • Hung HC, Joshipura KJ, Jiang R, Hu FB, Hunter D, Smith-Warner SA, Colditz GA, Rosner B, Spiegelman D, Willett WC. Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of major chronic disease. Journal of the National Cancer Institute . 2004 Nov 3;96(21):1577-84.
  • He FJ, Nowson CA, Lucas M, MacGregor GA. Increased consumption of fruit and vegetables is related to a reduced risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of cohort studies. Journal of human hypertension . 2007 Sep;21(9):717.
  • He FJ, Nowson CA, MacGregor GA. Fruit and vegetable consumption and stroke: meta-analysis of cohort studies. The Lancet . 2006 Jan 28;367(9507):320-6.
  • Appel LJ, Moore TJ, Obarzanek E, Vollmer WM, Svetkey LP, Sacks FM, Bray GA, Vogt TM, Cutler JA, Windhauser MM, Lin PH. A clinical trial of the effects of dietary patterns on blood pressure. New England Journal of Medicine . 1997 Apr 17;336(16):1117-24.
  • Appel LJ, Sacks FM, Carey VJ, Obarzanek E, Swain JF, Miller ER, Conlin PR, Erlinger TP, Rosner BA, Laranjo NM, Charleston J. Effects of protein, monounsaturated fat, and carbohydrate intake on blood pressure and serum lipids: results of the OmniHeart randomized trial. JAMA . 2005 Nov 16;294(19):2455-64.
  • Yokoyama Y, Nishimura K, Barnard ND, Takegami M, Watanabe M, Sekikawa A, Okamura T, Miyamoto Y. Vegetarian diets and blood pressure: a meta-analysis. JAMA internal medicine. 2014 Apr 1;174(4):577-87.
  • Farvid MS, Chen WY, Michels KB, Cho E, Willett WC, Eliassen AH. Fruit and vegetable consumption in adolescence and early adulthood and risk of breast cancer: population based cohort study. BMJ . 2016 May 11;353:i2343.
  • Farvid MS, Eliassen AH, Cho E, Liao X, Chen WY, Willett WC. Dietary fiber intake in young adults and breast cancer risk. Pediatrics . 2016 Mar 1;137(3):e20151226.
  • Farvid MS, Chen WY, Rosner BA, Tamimi RM, Willett WC, Eliassen AH. Fruit and vegetable consumption and breast cancer incidence: Repeated measures over 30 years of follow‐up. International journal of cancer . 2018 Jul 6.
  • Wiseman M. The Second World Cancer Research Fund/American Institute for Cancer Research Expert Report. Food, Nutrition, Physical Activity, and the Prevention of Cancer: A Global Perspective: Nutrition Society and BAPEN Medical Symposium on ‘Nutrition support in cancer therapy’. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society . 2008 Aug;67(3):253-6.
  • Giovannucci E, Liu Y, Platz EA, Stampfer MJ, Willett WC. Risk factors for prostate cancer incidence and progression in the health professionals follow‐up study. International journal of cancer . 2007 Oct 1;121(7):1571-8.
  • Kavanaugh CJ, Trumbo PR, Ellwood KC. The US Food and Drug Administration’s evidence-based review for qualified health claims: tomatoes, lycopene, and cancer. Journal of the National Cancer Institute . 2007 Jul 18;99(14):1074-85.
  • Muraki I, Imamura F, Manson JE, Hu FB, Willett WC, van Dam RM, Sun Q. Fruit consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes: results from three prospective longitudinal cohort studies. BMJ . 2013 Aug 29;347:f5001.
  • Bazzano LA, Li TY, Joshipura KJ, Hu FB. Intake of fruit, vegetables, and fruit juices and risk of diabetes in women. Diabetes Care . 2008 Apr 3.
  • Mursu J, Virtanen JK, Tuomainen TP, Nurmi T, Voutilainen S. Intake of fruit, berries, and vegetables and risk of type 2 diabetes in Finnish men: the Kuopio Ischaemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study–. The American journal of clinical nutrition . 2013 Nov 20;99(2):328-33.
  • Lembo A, Camilleri M. Chronic constipation. New England Journal of Medicine . 2003 Oct 2;349(14):1360-8.
  • Aldoori WH, Giovannucci EL, Rockett HR, Sampson L, Rimm EB, Willett AW. A prospective study of dietary fiber types and symptomatic diverticular disease in men. The Journal of nutrition . 1998 Oct 1;128(4):714-9.
  • Brown L, Rimm EB, Seddon JM, Giovannucci EL, Chasan-Taber L, Spiegelman D, Willett WC, Hankinson SE. A prospective study of carotenoid intake and risk of cataract extraction in US men–. The American journal of clinical nutrition . 1999 Oct 1;70(4):517-24.
  • Christen WG, Liu S, Schaumberg DA, Buring JE. Fruit and vegetable intake and the risk of cataract in women–. The American journal of clinical nutrition . 2005 Jun 1;81(6):1417-22.
  • Moeller SM, Taylor A, Tucker KL, McCullough ML, Chylack Jr LT, Hankinson SE, Willett WC, Jacques PF. Overall adherence to the dietary guidelines for Americans is associated with reduced prevalence of early age-related nuclear lens opacities in women. The Journal of nutrition . 2004 Jul 1;134(7):1812-9.
  • Cho E, Seddon JM, Rosner B, Willett WC, Hankinson SE. Prospective study of intake of fruits, vegetables, vitamins, and carotenoidsand risk of age-related maculopathy. Archives of Ophthalmology . 2004 Jun 1;122(6):883-92.
  • Christen WG, Liu S, Glynn RJ, Gaziano JM, Buring JE. Dietary carotenoids, vitamins C and E, and risk of cataract in women: a prospective study. Archives of Ophthalmology . 2008 Jan 1;126(1):102-9.

Terms of Use

The contents of this website are for educational purposes and are not intended to offer personal medical advice. You should seek the advice of your physician or other qualified health provider with any questions you may have regarding a medical condition. Never disregard professional medical advice or delay in seeking it because of something you have read on this website. The Nutrition Source does not recommend or endorse any products.

IMAGES

  1. Five Basic Types of Research Studies

    what is basic research study

  2. Basic Research

    what is basic research study

  3. Difference Between Basic and Applied Research(With Table)

    what is basic research study

  4. What is Basic Research and Why is it Useful?

    what is basic research study

  5. Getting Started with Research

    what is basic research study

  6. Components of Research Process

    what is basic research study

VIDEO

  1. The Differences in Basic Research vs Applied Research

  2. Day 2: Basics of Scientific Research Writing (Batch 18)

  3. Difference between Basic research And Applied research

  4. Basic versus Applied Research

  5. Types of Research || Basic Research and Applied Research

  6. Basic research

COMMENTS

  1. Basic Research

    Interdisciplinary: Basic research is often interdisciplinary, drawing on multiple fields of study to address complex research questions. Basic research can be conducted in fields ranging from physics and chemistry to biology and psychology. Open-ended: Basic research is open-ended, meaning that it does not have a specific end goal in mind ...

  2. Basic Research in Psychology: Definition and Examples

    Basic Research in Psychology. Basic research—also known as fundamental or pure research—refers to study and research meant to increase our scientific knowledge base. This type of research is often purely theoretical, with the intent of increasing our understanding of certain phenomena or behavior. In contrast with applied research, basic ...

  3. What is Basic Research?

    Basic research is essential to developing theories related to human behavior and mental processes. The subfield of cognition is a significant benefactor of basic research as it relies on novel theoretical frameworks relating to memory and learning. With limited established knowledge of the mind, psychology is an ideal field for basic research.

  4. Basic Research: What it is with examples

    Basic Research Examples. There can be many examples of basic research; here are some of them: A study of how stress affects labor productivity. Studying the best factors of pricing strategies. Understand the client's level of satisfaction before certain interactions with the company providing solutions. The understanding of the leadership ...

  5. Basic Research

    Basic research is defined by the National Science Foundation (NSF 2010: 9) in the US as 'systematic study directed toward fuller knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind'.Similarly, 'basic research is experimental or theoretical work undertaken primarily to acquire new ...

  6. Research Types Explained: Basic, Clinical, Translational

    Basic medical research is usually conducted by scientists with a PhD in such fields as biology and chemistry, among many others. They study the core building blocks of life — DNA, cells, proteins, molecules, etc. — to answer fundamental questions about their structures and how they work. For example, oncologists now know that mutations in ...

  7. Basic research

    Basic research, also called pure research, fundamental research, basic science, or pure science, is a type of scientific research with the aim of improving scientific theories for better understanding and prediction of natural or other phenomena. In contrast, applied research uses scientific theories to develop technology or techniques, which can be used to intervene and alter natural or other ...

  8. Types of Research Designs Compared

    Basic vs. applied: Basic research aims to develop knowledge, theories and predictions, while applied research aims to develop techniques, ... you have to make more concrete decisions about your research methods and the details of the study. Read more about creating a research design. Other interesting articles. If you want to know more about ...

  9. Basic Research: Understanding The Way Things Work and Why It Matters

    In basic science, scientists utilize many simple organisms, like fruit flies and worms, because they are easy to study and manipulate, yet share many molecular and biological processes with humans. Results of these basic studies contribute to our understanding in fields such as genetics, biochemistry, stem cell research, and neurobiology.

  10. The Concept of Basic Research

    Animal research is also important in another type of research, called basic research. Basic research experiments are performed to further scientific knowledge without an obvious or immediate benefit. The goal of basic research is to understand the function of newly discovered molecules and cells, strange phenomena, or little-understood processes.

  11. Research Methods

    Research methods are specific procedures for collecting and analyzing data. Developing your research methods is an integral part of your research design. When planning your methods, there are two key decisions you will make. First, decide how you will collect data. Your methods depend on what type of data you need to answer your research question:

  12. Fundamental Research

    Fundamental research, also known as basic research or ... For example, a study entitled "A critical assessment of the role of organizational culture in facilitating management-employee communications" is a fundamental study, but findings of this study may be used to increase the levels of effectiveness of management-employee communications ...

  13. What is Basic Research? Insights from Historical Semantics

    In other studies, categories such as basic and applied research no longer play a major role. Research grounded in approaches such as actor-network theory, that is studies emphasizing the societal context of science, anthropological studies focusing on day-to-day laboratory work, and the new - although still vague - concept of technoscience are united in their critique of discursive ...

  14. Definitions of Basic, Applied, and Fundamental Research

    NSF Definition of Basic Research: Basic research is defined as systematic study directed toward fuller knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind. (In Bill Berry's presentation, Meeting 1, Committee on Department of Defense ...

  15. Basic Research vs. Applied Research: What's the Difference?

    Basic research, or fundamental research, is a type of investigation focused on improving the understanding of a particular phenomenon, study or law of nature. This type of research examines data to find the unknown and fulfill a sense of curiosity.

  16. Basic Research: Definition, Examples

    Basic research is the foundation of scientific and technological advancement. It explores new ideas, principles, and theories that can lead to breakthroughs and innovations. In this article, you will learn the definition, examples, and types of basic research, as well as its benefits and limitations. If you are interested in the nature and purpose of basic research, visit iEduNote.com to read ...

  17. What is basic research?

    "All good basic research ends with new perspectives that can also have practical applications," says Nissen. "All basic research projects lead to further projects. So it has a big effect on people's daily lives." In that sense, basic research is no different to applied research, he says. "It just has a longer term perspective."

  18. "What does basic research mean?"

    Basic research is defined as "a systematic study directed toward greater knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind.". In categorical budget analyses, NIH compares basic research spending to that of applied or translational ...

  19. Understanding Basic Research vs. Applied Research

    Basic research vs. applied research. Where basic research gathers information and data on a subject, applied research uses that data to look for answers to questions. Applied research takes the data obtained in basic research and applies it to answer a question and provide a possible solution. There are three types of applied research:

  20. What is Pure or Basic Research? + [Examples & Method]

    Basic research is a type of research approach that is aimed at gaining a better understanding of a subject, phenomenon or basic law of nature. This type of research is primarily focused on the advancement of knowledge rather than solving a specific problem. Basic research is also referred to as pure research or fundamental research.

  21. The Basic Concepts of Research: the Key to Getting Started in Research

    This was followed by sessions on scientific writing. DAY 1 taught the basic concepts of scientific research, including: (1) how to formulate a topic for research and to describe the what, why, and how of the protocol, (2) biomedical literature search and review, (3) study designs, statistical concepts, and result analyses, and (4) publication ...

  22. What Is a Research Design

    A research design is a strategy for answering your research question using empirical data. Creating a research design means making decisions about: Your overall research objectives and approach. Whether you'll rely on primary research or secondary research. Your sampling methods or criteria for selecting subjects. Your data collection methods.

  23. Wk 2 Summative Assessment Article Analysis and Research ...

    2 Article Analysis and Research Considerations Two types of research should be considered and they include the basic and applied research. Researchers can tell if the research is basic or applied in the process of conducting a research analysis. Different approaches are taken by basic and applied research to build new knowledge and examine what already exists.

  24. Cleveland awards $600,000 to study 'universal basic employment'

    CLEVELAND, Ohio — City Hall is giving $600,000 to United Way of Greater Cleveland to conduct research on how to set up a "universal basic employment" pilot program in Cleveland. A city news ...

  25. A Basic Bibliometric Review of Research on Arab Female ...

    This study aims to provide an evidence-mapping of the knowledge base of Arab female entrepreneurship. It reviews 107 articles on Arab FE published in both Arabic and English language journals between 1990 and 2020. Being a systematic review, this study utilizes the basic bibliometric analysis to uncover the field's structure and dynamics, gain deeper insights, and enrich conversation on the ...

  26. What is Basic Research? Insights from Historical Semantics

    In fact, this study has shown that the term basic research cannot be seen as a simple synonym for the older notion of pure science. As a consequence, the assumption made in social-scientific studies that the ideal of basic research structured modern science continuously up until the postmodern era, when application-oriented research was thought ...

  27. Yoga and Meditation-Induced Altered States of Consciousness are Common

    Yoga, mindfulness, meditation, breathwork, and other practices can foster altered states of consciousness. In a study of adults in the general population, a team including researchers from Massachusetts General Hospital found that a surprisingly high percentage of participants (45%) reported experiencing altered states of consciousness.

  28. Combining plasma Aβ and p-tau217 improves detection of brain amyloid in

    Maximizing the efficiency to screen amyloid-positive individuals in asymptomatic and non-demented aged population using blood-based biomarkers is essential for future success of clinical trials in the early stage of Alzheimer's disease (AD). In this study, we elucidate the utility of combination of plasma amyloid-β (Aβ)-related biomarkers and tau phosphorylated at threonine 217 (p-tau217 ...

  29. Vegetables and Fruits

    Eat plenty every day. A diet rich in vegetables and fruits can lower blood pressure, reduce the risk of heart disease and stroke, prevent some types of cancer, lower risk of eye and digestive problems, and have a positive effect upon blood sugar, which can help keep appetite in check. Eating non-starchy vegetables and fruits like apples, pears ...