Learning from Errors: Systematic Analysis of Complex Writing Errors for Improving Writing Technology

  • First Online: 12 November 2014

Cite this chapter

research on error analysis in writing

  • Cerstin Mahlow 5  

Part of the book series: Text, Speech and Language Technology ((TLTB,volume 48))

1506 Accesses

1 Citations

3 Altmetric

In this paper, we describe ongoing research on writing errors with the ultimate goal to develop error-preventing editing functions in word-processors. Drawing from the state-of-the-art research in errors carried out in various fields, we propose the application of a general concept for action slips as introduced by Norman ( 1981 ). We demonstrate the feasibility of this approach by using a large corpus of writing errors in published texts. The concept of slips considers both the process and the product: some failure in a procedure results in an error in the product, i.e., is visible in the written text. In order to develop preventing functions, we need to determine causes of such visible errors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

https://www.linguistik.hu-berlin.de/institut/professuren/korpuslinguistik/forschung/falko .

Emphasis in the original.

http://www.inputlog.net .

http://kairos.technorhetoric.net/ .

As an example, in the project Integrated language tools for writing and document handling at KTH Stockholm ( http://www.nada.kth.se/iplab/langtools/ ), the development of linguistic search and editing functions was planned but had to be postponed first and was skipped in the end, because the required NLP resources would have to be developed in the first place.

http://lingured.info .

Candillon, W. (2013). XQuery development in the Cloud(9). XQuery code anywhere, anytime. In: XML Prague 2013, pp. 221–236. Prague: University of Economics, Czech Republic.

Google Scholar  

Clément, L., Gerdes, K., & Marlet, R. (2011). A grammar correction algorithm: Deep Parsing and minimal corrections for a grammar checker. In P. Groote, M. Egg & L. Kallmeyer (Eds.), Formal Grammar. 14th International Conference, FG 2009, Bordeaux, France, July 25-26, 2009, Revised Selected Papers , Lecture Notes in Computer Science (Vol. 5591, chap. 4, pp. 47–63). Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-20169-1_4 .

Connors, R. J., & Lunsford, A. A. (1988). Frequency of formal errors in current college writing, or Ma and Pa Kettle do research. College Composition and Communication , 39 (4), 395–409.

Cook, P., & Welsh, J. (2001). Incremental parsing in language-based editors: User needs and how to meet them. Software: Practice and Experience 31 (15), 1461–1486. doi: 10.1002/spe.422 .

Corder, S. P. (1967). The significance of learner’s errors. Produktinformation International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 5 (4), 161–170. doi: 10.1515/iral.1967.5.1-4.161 .

Costa, F., Frasconi, P., Lombardo, V., & Soda, G. (2003). Towards incremental parsing of natural language using recursive neural networks. Applied Intelligence, 19 (1–2), 9–25. doi: 10.1023/a:1023860521975 .

Article   MATH   Google Scholar  

Dale, R. (1990). Automating editorial assistance. In IEEE Colloquium on Hypertext , pp. 3/1–3/3.

Daz-Negrillo, A., & Fernández-Dominguez, J. (2006). Error tagging systems for learner corpora. Revista Española de Lingüstica Aplicada, 19 , 83–102.

Ehrensberger-Dow, M., & Perrin, D. (2009). Capturing translation processes to access metalinguistic awareness. Across Languages and Cultures, 10 (2), 275–288. doi: 10.1556/acr.10.2009.2.6 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition. Oxford Applied Linguistics . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Engelbart, D. C. (1962). Augmenting human intellect: A conceptual framework. Menlo Park: Tech. Rep., Stanford Research Institute.

Flinn, J. Z. (1987). Case studies of revision aided by keystroke recording and replaying software. Computers and Composition, 5 (1), 31–44. doi: 10.1016/s8755-4615(87)80013-0 .

Foster, J. (2010). “cba to check the spelling”: Investigating parser performance on discussion forum posts. In Human Language Technologies: The 2010 Annual Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 381–384). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics.

Foster, J., & Vogel, C. (2004). Parsing Ill-formed text using an error grammar. Artificial Intelligence Review, 21 (3–4), 269–291. doi: 10.1023/b:aire.0000036259.68818.1e .

Foth, K., Menzel, W., Pop, H.F., & Schröder, I. (2000). An experiment on incremental analysis using robust parsing techniques. In Proceedings of the 18th conference on Computational Linguistics ( COLING ‘00 ) (pp. 1026–1030). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics. doi: 10.3115/992730.992798 .

Foth, K., Menzel, W., & Schröder, I. (2005). Robust parsing with weighted constraints. Natural Language Engineering, 11 (1), 1–25. doi: 10.1017/s1351324903003267 .

Gamon, M. (2010). Using mostly native data to correct errors in learners’ writing. In Human Language Technologies: The 2010 Annual Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 163–171). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics.

Gries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.). (2007). Corpora in cognitive linguistics: corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis . Berlin, New York: De Gruyter Mouton.

Gubelmann, R. (2010). Systematischer Überblick über die Fundstücke im Fehlerforum zu “Making word processors process words”. Master’s thesis. Zürich, Schweiz: Universität Zürich, Insitut für Computerlinguistik.

Hausser, R. (2001). Foundations of computational linguistics: Human-computer communication in natural language, 2nd rev. and ext. edn. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer, New York (2001).

Heidorn, G. E. (2000). Intelligent writing assistance: Techniques and applications for the processing of language as text. In R. Dale, H. Moisl, & H. Somers (Eds.), Handbook of natural language processing (pp. 181–207). New York, NY, USA: Marcel Dekker.

Huang, L., & Sagae, K. (2010). Dynamic programming for linear-time incremental parsing. In Proceedings of the 48th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, ACL ‘10 (pp. 1077–1086). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics.

Jensen, K., Heidorn, G. E., Miller, L. A., & Ravin, Y. (1983). Parse fitting and prose fixing: getting a hold on ill-formedness. Computational Linguistics, 9 (3–4), 147–160.

Ko, A. J., Aung, H. H., & Myers, B. A. (2005). Design requirements for more flexible structured editors from a study of programmers’ text editing. In CHI ‘05: CHI ‘05 extended abstracts on human factors in computing systems, pp. 1557–1560. New York, NY, USA: ACM. doi: 10.1145/1056808.1056965 .

Kollberg, P. (1998). S-notation: A Computer based method for studying and representing text composition. Master’s thesis. Kungliga Tekniska Högskolan Stockholm.

Kramer, A. (2004). Rechtschreibkorrektursysteme im Vergleich. DITECT versus Microsoft Word. URL http://www.mediensprache.net/de/networx/docs/networx-35.aspx .

Leacock, C., Chodorow, M., Gamon, M., & Tetreault, J. (2010). Automated grammatical error detection for language learners. Synthesis Lectures on Human Language Technologies (Vol. 9). San Rafael, CA, USA: Morgan & Claypool. doi: 10.2200/s00275ed1v01y201006hlt009 .

Leijten, M., Janssen, D., & Van Waes, L. (2010). Error correction strategies of professional speech recognition users: Three profiles. Computers in Human Behavior, 26 (5), 964–975. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.02.010 .

Leijten, M., Macken, L., Hoste, V., Van Horenbeeck, E., & Van Waes, L. (2012). From character to word level: Enabling the linguistic analyses of inputlog process data. In M. Piotrowski, C. Mahlow & R. Dale (Eds.). Proceedings of the Second Workshop on Computational Linguistics and Writing (CL&W 2012): Linguistic and Cognitive Aspects of Document Creation and Document Engineering (pp. 1–8). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics.

Leijten, M., & Van Waes, L. (2006). Inputlog: New perspectives on the logging of on-line writing. In: K. P. H. Sullivan, E. Lindgren (Eds.), Computer keystroke logging and writing: Methods and applications , studies in writing (Vol. 18, chap. 9, pp. 73–94). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Lüdeling, A., Walter, M., Kroymann, E., & Adolphs, P. (2005). Multi-level error annotation in learner corpora. In Proceedings of Corpus Linguistics 2005 .

Lunsford, A. A., & Lunsford, K. J. (2008). Mistakes Are a fact of life: A national comparative study. College Composition and Communication, 59 (4), 781–806 (2008).

Macdonald, N. H., Frase, L. T., Gingrich, P. S., & Keenan, S. A. (1982). The writer’s workbench: Computer aids for text analysis. IEEE Transactions on Communication, 30 (1), 105–110.

Mahlow, C. (2011). Linguistisch unterstütztes redigieren: Konzept und exemplarische Umsetzung basierend auf interaktiven computerlinguistischen Ressourcen. Ph.D. thesis, University of Zurich.

Meurers, D. (2013). Natural language processing and language learning. In C. A. Chapelle (Ed.) Encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 1–13). Oxford: Blackwell.

Meurers, D., Ziai, R., Amaral, L., Boyd, A., Dimitrov, A., Metcalf, V., & Ott, N. (2010). Enhancing authentic web pages for language learners. In Proceedings of the NAACL HLT 2010 Fifth Workshop on Innovative Use of NLP for Building Educational Applications (pp. 10–18). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics.

Napolitano, D. M., & Stent, A. (2009). TechWriter: An evolving system for writing assistance for advanced learners of English. CALICO Journal, 26 (3), 611–625.

Nivre, J. (2008). Algorithms for deterministic incremental dependency parsing. Computational Linguistics, 34 (4), 513–553. doi: 10.1162/coli.07-056-r1-07-027 .

Article   MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88 , 1–15.

Norman, D. A. (1983). Design rules based on analyses of human error. Communications of the ACM, 26 (4), 254–258. doi: 10.1145/2163.358092 .

Omar, C., Yoon, Y., LaToza, T. D., & Myers, B. A. (2012). Active code completion. In Proceedings of the 2012 International Conference on Software Engineering, ICSE 2012 (pp. 859–869). Piscataway, NJ, USA: IEEE Press.

Perrin, D. (2002). Progression analysis (PA): Investigating writing strategies in the workplace. In T. Olive & C. M. Levy (Eds.) Contemporary Tools and techniques for studying writing, studies in writing (Vol. 10, pp. 105–117). Boston, Dordrecht, London: Kluwer.

Perrin, D. (2006a). Progression analysis: An ethnographic computer-based multi-method approach for investigate natural writing processes. In L. Van Waes, M. Leijten & C. M. Neuwirth (Eds.), Writing and digital media, studies in writing (Vol. 17, pp. 173–179). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Perrin, D. (2006b). Schreibforschung im Kursalltag: Was die Progressionsanalyse praktisch nützt. In O. Kruse, K. Berger & M. Ulmi (Eds.), Prozessorientierte Schreibdidaktik: Schreibtraining für Schule, Studium und Beruf (pp. 279–294). Bern, Stuttgart, Wie: Haupt.

Perrin, D. (2011). Language policy, tacit knowledge, and institutional learning: The case of the Swiss public service broadcaster SRG SSR. Current Issues in Language Planning, 12 (3), 331–348. doi: 10.1080/14664208.2011.604953 .

Perrin, D., & Wildi, M. (2009). Statistical modeling of writing processes. In C. Bazerman, R. Krut, K. Lunsford, S. McLeod, S. Null, P. Rogers, & A. Stansell (Eds.), Traditions of writing research (pp. 378–393). New York: Routledge.

Reznicek, M., Lüdeling, A., & Hirschmann, H. (2013). Competing target hypotheses in the Falko Corpus. A flexible multi-layer corpus architecture. In: A. Daz-Negrillo (Ed.) Automatic treatment and analysis of learner corpus data . Amsterdam: John Benjamins (forthcoming).

Rimrott, A., & Heift, T. (2008). Evaluating automatic detection of misspellings in German. Language Learning & Technology, 12 (3), 73–92.

Roark, B. (2001). Probabilistic top-down parsing and language modeling. Computational Linguistics, 27 (2), 249–276. doi: 10.1162/089120101750300526 .

Schulze, M. (2004). Ein sprachunabhängiger Ansatz zur Entwicklung deklarativer, robuster LA-Grammatiken mit einer exemplarischen Anwendung auf das Deutsche und das Englische. Ph.D. thesis, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg.

Severinson Eklundh, K. (1992). Problems in achieving a global perspective of the text in computer-based writing. In M. Sharples (Ed.) Computers and writing: issues and implementation (pp. 73–84). Boston, Dordrecht, London: Kluwer.

Severinson Eklundh, K. (1994). Linear and nonlinear strategies in computer-based writing. Computers and Composition 11 (3), 203–216. doi: 10.1016/8755-4615(94)90013-2 .

Sloan, G. (1990). Frequency of errors in essays by college freshmen and by professional writers. College Composition and Communication , 41 (3), 199–308.

Smithson, I. (1986). The writing workshop. Computers and Composition, 4 (1), 78–94. doi: 10.1016/s8755-4615(86)80009-3 .

Sullivan, K. P. H., & Lindgren, E. (2006). Computer key-stroke logging and writing: Methods and applications. K. P. H. & E. Lindgren (Eds.) Studies in writing (Vol. 18). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Van De Vanter, M. L. (1995). Practical language-based editing for software engineers. In Software Engineering and Human-Computer Interaction , Lecture Notes in Computer Science (Vol. 896, pp. 251–267). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/bfb0035821 .

Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., & Neuwirth, C. M. (Eds.). (2006). Writing and digital media, studies in writing (Vol. 17). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., & Van Weijen, D. (2009). Keystroke logging in writing research: Observing writing processes with Inputlog. GFL—German as a Foreign Language, 2 (3), 41–64.

Vernon, A. (2000). Computerized grammar checkers 2000: Capabilities, limitations, and pedagogical possibilities. Computers and Composition, 17 (3), 329–349. doi: 10.1016/s8755-4615(00)00038-4 .

Williams, N. (1989). Computer assisted writing software: RUSKIN. In: N. Williams & P. O. Holt (Eds.) Computers and writing: Models and tools (chap. 1, pp. 1–16). Oxford: Intellect.

Wirén, M. (1989). Interactive incremental chart parsing. In Proceedings of the Fourth Conference of the European Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 241–248). Morristown, NJ, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics. doi: 10.3115/976815.976848 .

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Stuttgart Germany, Stuttgart, Germany

Cerstin Mahlow

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cerstin Mahlow .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

CNRS-LIF, UMR 7279, Aix-Marseille University, City, France

CNRS-LIF, UMR 7279, Aix-Marseille University and University of Mainz, Marseille, France

Reinhard Rapp

CNRS-LIF, UMR 7279, Aix-Marseille University, Marseille, France

Gemma Bel-Enguix

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Mahlow, C. (2015). Learning from Errors: Systematic Analysis of Complex Writing Errors for Improving Writing Technology. In: Gala, N., Rapp, R., Bel-Enguix, G. (eds) Language Production, Cognition, and the Lexicon. Text, Speech and Language Technology, vol 48. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08043-7_24

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08043-7_24

Published : 12 November 2014

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-319-08042-0

Online ISBN : 978-3-319-08043-7

eBook Packages : Computer Science Computer Science (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Second Language Writing and Research: The Writing Process and Error Analysis in Student Texts

Academic writing requires conscious effort and much practice in composing, developing, and analyzing ideas. Students writing in a second language are also faced with social and cognitive challenges related to second language acquisition. L1 models of writing instruction and research on composing processes have been the theoretical basis for using the process approach in L2 writing pedagogy. However, language proficiency and competence underlies the ability to write in the L2 in a fundamental way. Therefore, L2 writing instructors should take into account both strategy development and language skill development when working with students. This paper explores error in writing in relation to particular aspects of second language acquisition and theories of the writing process in L1 and L2. It can be argued that a focus on the writing process as a pedagogical tool is only appropriate for second language learners if attention is given to linguistic development, and if learners are able to get sufficient and effective feedback with regard to their errors in writing.

Introduction

The ability to write well is not a naturally acquired skill; it is usually learned or culturally transmitted as a set of practices in formal instructional settings or other environments. Writing skills must be practiced and learned through experience. Writing also involves composing, which implies the ability either to tell or retell pieces of information in the form of narratives or description, or to transform information into new texts, as in expository or argumentative writing. Perhaps it is best viewed as a continuum of activities that range from the more mechanical or formal aspects of "writing down" on the one end, to the more complex act of composing on the other end (Omaggio Hadley, 1993). It is undoubtedly the act of composing, though, which can create problems for students, especially for those writing in a second language (L2) in academic contexts. Formulating new ideas can be difficult because it involves transforming or reworking information, which is much more complex than writing as telling. By putting together concepts and solving problems, the writer engages in "a two-way interaction between continuously developing knowledge and continuously developing text" (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987, p. 12). Indeed, academic writing requires conscious effort and practice in composing, developing, and analyzing ideas. Compared to students writing in their native language (L1), however, students writing in their L2 have to also acquire proficiency in the use of the language as well as writing strategies, techniques and skills. They might also have to deal with instructors and later, faculty members, who may or may not get beyond their language problems when evaluating their work. Although a certain amount of consciousness-raising on the part of the readers may be warranted, students want to write close to error-free texts and they enter language courses with the expectations of becoming more proficient writers in the L2. [-1-]

This paper explores error in writing in relation to particular aspects of second language acquisition and theories of the writing process in L1 and L2. I argue that the process approach to instruction, with its emphasis on the writing process, meaning making, invention and multiple drafts (Raimes, 1991), is only appropriate for second language learners if they are both able to get sufficient feedback with regard to their errors in writing, and are proficient enough in the language to implement revision strategies.

A brief survey of the nature of L2 writing and L1 models of the writing process illustrates why it is difficult to apply L1 research to a model for second language writing. Further, certain social and cognitive factors related to second language acquisition show that strategies involved in the language learning process also affect L2 writing. With a discussion of these factors, fundamental questions about error in writing and L2 proficiency are raised. It should then become apparent that the process approach to writing instruction can only be effective if these two components are taken into consideration.

Models of L1 and L2 Writing

Most ESL students studying in post-secondary institutions have writing skills. However, their purposes for writing are sometimes not the kind valued by Western academic communities. The nature of academic literacy often confuses and disorients students, "particularly those who bring with them a set of conventions that are at odds with those of the academic world they are entering" (Kutz, Groden & Zamel, 1993, p. 30). In addition, the culture-specific nature of schemata--abstract mental structures representing our knowledge of things, events, and situations--can lead to difficulties when students write texts in L2. Knowing how to write a "summary" or "analysis" in Mandarin or Spanish does not necessarily mean that students will be able to do these things in English (Kern, 2000). As a result, any appropriate instruction must take into consideration the influence from various educational, social, and cultural experiences that students have in their native language. These include textual issues, such as rhetorical and cultural preferences for organizing information and structuring arguments, commonly referred to as contrastive rhetoric (Cai, 1999; Connor, 1997; Kaplan, 1987; Kobayashi & Rinnert, 1996; Leki, 1993; 1997; Matalene, 1985), knowledge of appropriate genres (Johns, 1995; Swales, 1990), familiarity with writing topics (Shen, 1989), and distinct cultural and instructional socialization (Coleman, 1996; Holliday, 1997; Valdes, 1995). In addition to instructional and cultural factors, L2 writers have varying commands of the target language, which affect the way structural errors are treated from both social and cognitive points of view.

Much of the research on L2 writing has been closely dependent on L1 research. Although L2 writing is strategically, rhetorically, and linguistically different in many ways from L1 writing (Silva, 1993), L1 models have had a significant influence on L2 writing instruction and the development of a theory of L2 writing. However, a look at two popular L1 models will give us some insight into the problem of developing a distinct construct of L2 writing. [-2-]

The Flower and Hayes (1980, 1981) model focuses on what writers do when they compose. It examines the rhetorical problem in order to determine the potential difficulties a writer could experience during the composing process. The "problem-solving activity" is divided into two major components: the rhetorical situation (audience, topic, assignment), and the writer's own goals (involving the reader, the writer's persona, the construction of meaning, and the production of the formal text). By comparing skilled and less-skilled writers, the emphasis here is placed on "students' strategic knowledge and the ability of students to transform information . . . to meet rhetorically constrained purposes" (Grabe & Kaplan, 1996, p. 116). However, the social dimension is important too. Indeed, writing "should not be viewed solely as an individually-oriented, inner-directed cognitive process, but as much as an acquired response to the discourse conventions . . . within particular communities" (Swales, 1990, p. 4).

In more recent studies that examine the goals students set for themselves, the strategies they use to develop their organizing of ideas and the metacognitive awareness they bring to both these acts, Flower and her colleagues (1990) analyze the academic task of reading-to-write to establish the interaction of context and cognition in performing a particular writing task.

One of the problems they note is the transition students are required to make when entering the academic discourse community (a peculiar, socially constructed convention in itself), where students need to learn how to operate successfully in an academic conversation that implies knowledge of the textual conventions, expectations, and formulaic expressions particular to the discourse. According to the researchers, "conceptualizing this transition as a social/cognitive act of entering a discourse emphasizes both the problem-solving effort of a student learning to negotiate a new situation and the role the situation will play in what is learned" (p. 222). The view that writing is typically a socially situated, communicative act is later incorporated into Flower's (1994) socio-cognitive theory of writing. In the social cognitive curriculum students are taught as apprentices in negotiating an academic community, and in the process develop strategic knowledge. Writing skills are acquired and used through negotiated interaction with real audience expectations, such as in peer group responses. Instruction should, then, afford students the opportunity to participate in transactions with their own texts and the texts of others (Grabe & Kaplan, 1996). By guiding students toward a conscious awareness of how an audience will interpret their work, learners then learn to write with a "readerly" sensitivity (Kern, 2000).

Bereiter and Scardamalia (1987) also propose a model that suggests reasons for differences in writing ability between skilled and less-skilled writers. The basic difference is revealed in their two models of writing: the knowledge-telling model, whose basic structure depends on the processes of retrieving content from memory with regard to topical and genre cues, and the knowledge-transforming model, which involves more reflective problem-solving analysis and goal-setting. The latter model is important because it opens up the idea of multiple processing, which is revealed through writing tasks that vary in processing complexity. The authors discuss the notion of mental representation as a writing strategy. From their research with graduate students, they observe that the students "generated goals for their compositions and engaged in problem solving involving structure and gist as well as verbatim representations" (p. 354). The knowledge-transforming or intentional writing model is different from knowledge telling in that it involves setting of goals that are to be achieved through the composing process, and the purposeful achievement of those goals. The composing process does not depend on memories and emotions and on external (teacher) assistance for its direction. In fact, Bereiter and Scardamalia criticize formal schooling that encourages the more passive kind of cognition by "continually telling students what to do," rather than encouraging them "to follow their spontaneous interests and impulses . . . and assume responsibility for what becomes of their minds" (p. 361). They also argue that the ability to wrestle with and resolve both content and rhetorical problems calls upon a dialectical process for reflection. If students rarely practice the kinds of writing tasks that develop knowledge-transforming skills, they are not likely to be able to perform those skills easily. [-3-]

Both the Flower and Hayes, and the Bereiter and Scardamalia writing process models have served as the theoretical basis for using the process approach in both L1 and L2 writing instruction. By incorporating pre-writing activities such as collaborative brainstorming, choice of personally meaningful topics, strategy instruction in the stages of composing, drafting, revising, and editing, multiple drafts and peer-group editing, the instruction takes into consideration what writers do as they write. Attention to the writing process stresses more of a workshop approach to instruction, which fosters classroom interaction, and engages students in analyzing and commenting on a variety of texts. The L1 theories also seem to support less teacher intervention and less attention to form.

Despite their implications for classroom instruction, not all the components of these models are appropriate in an L2 context. The Flower model, in particular, does not recognize cross-cultural differences and issues related to sociocultural variation in the functions of the written language (Kern, 2000). Additionally, with native speakers, "writing ability is more closely linked to fluency in and familiarity with the conventions of expository discourse" (Kogen 1986, p. 25). L2 writers, however, are in the process of acquiring these conventions and so they often need more instruction about the language itself. Limited knowledge of vocabulary, language structure, and content can inhibit a L2 writer's performance. In addition, the models do not account for growing language proficiency, which is a vital element of L2 writing development.

Similarly, composing, especially in the revision stage, challenges L2 writers. In his research on how L2 writers revise their work, Silva (1993) observes that learners revise at a superficial level. They re-read and reflect less on their written text, revise less, and when they do, the revision is primarily focused on grammatical correction. On the other hand, L1 writing ability may also transfer to L2. As a result, students who are skilled writers in their native languages and have surpassed a certain L2 proficiency level can adequately transfer those skills. Of course, those who have difficulty writing in their native language may not have a repertoire of strategies to help them in their L2 writing development (Sasaki & Hirose, 1996). These observations warrant consideration for L2 instruction and course design, especially for those courses in English for Academic Purposes (EAP) writing that include less-skilled writers or those who have never had the opportunity to engage in more knowledge-transforming tasks in their native languages.

In sum, social-cognitive theories of writing show us how social contexts for writing operate together with the cognitive efforts of the writer, just as they do when a person is acquiring a new language. However, the problem with applying L1 theories and subsequent models of instruction (such as the process approach) to L2 instruction is that L2 writing also involves the cognitively demanding task of generating meaningful text in a second language. As a result, L2 students generally want more teacher involvement and guidance, especially at the revision stage. Consequently, in order to provide effective pedagogy, L2 writing instructors need to understand the social and cognitive factors involved in the process of second language acquisition and error in writing because these factors have a salient effect on L2 writing development. [-4-]

The Sources of Error in L2 Writing: Social and Cognitive Factors

Social factors.

Both social and cognitive factors affect language learning. Exploration of social factors gives us some idea of why learners differ in rate of L2 learning, in proficiency type (for instance, conversational ability versus writing ability), and in ultimate proficiency (Ellis, 1994). Research based on direct (self-report questionnaires) and indirect measures generally shows that learners with positive attitudes, motivation, and concrete goals will have these attitudes reinforced if they experience success. Likewise, learners' negative attitudes may be strengthened by lack of success or by failure (McGroarty, 1996). Needless to say, although ESL learners may have negative attitudes toward writing for academic purposes, many of them are financially and professionally committed to graduating from English-speaking universities, and as a result, have strong reasons for learning and improving their skills.

There is a direct relationship between learner attitudes and learner motivation. Gardner's (1985) socio-educational model is designed to account for the role of social factors in language acquisition. It interrelates four aspects of L2 learning: the social and cultural milieu (which determines beliefs about language and culture), individual learner differences (related to motivation and language aptitude), the setting (formal and/or informal learning contexts), and learning outcomes. Integrative motivation involves a desire to learn an L2 because individuals need to learn the target language to integrate into the community. In addition to this interest, the people or the culture represented by the other language group may also inspire them. On the other hand, instrumental motivation acknowledges the role that external influences and incentives play in strengthening the learners' desire to achieve. Learners who are instrumentally motivated are interested in learning the language for a particular purpose, such as writing a dissertation or getting a job. According to the theory, if second language learning takes place in isolation from a community of target language speakers, then it benefits more from integrative motivation, whereas if it takes place among a community of speakers, then instrumental orientation becomes the more effective motivational factor. Despite problems in Gardner's research design, it can be concluded that motivational factors "probably do not make much difference on their own, but they can create a more positive context in which language learning is likely to flourish" (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994, p. 140). (See Lambert, 1975; Schumann, 1978; Giles, Robinson & Smith, 1980; Giles & Byrne, 1982; and Hamers & Blanc, 1982 for examples of other models that focus on the social circumstances of learning in relation to second language acquisition).

Learners' attitudes, motivations, and goals can explain why some L2 writers perform better than others. For example, at the beginning of each of my ESL writing classes, I often ask students to fill out a personal information form to determine their needs and interests when planning my course. The answers to questions such as, "Do you enjoy writing in English?" and "What are your strengths and weaknesses in writing?" are revealing. Most students will answer that they hate writing in English (and in their native language, for that matter), and are only taking the course for educational and/or career purposes. In fact, it seems that many of the students would prefer to be practicing conversation. Students may enjoy writing e-mail messages to friends around the world, but challenges, such as difficulties getting started, finding the right words, and developing topics, abound. However, if students show an overall interest in the target language (integrative motivation), perceive that there is parental and social support, and have a desire to achieve their professional goals (instrumental motivation), they can become more proficient in their ability to write in English, despite the initial lack of self-motivation. [-5-]

Writing teachers should be aware of how the instrumental motivation of their L2 students will influence the effectiveness of their lessons. Common purposes for learners writing in an EAP context include writing a research paper for publication in an English-speaking journal or writing a business report for a multinational company. These learners may be less motivated to write stories or poetry, because they perceive that these tasks are not related to their needs. Even writing a standard research essay may seem like a waste of time for those who will need to write project reports and memos. If learners perceive writing tasks to be useless, they may approach them in a careless manner. Consequently, it is likely that they will be inattentive to errors, monitoring, and rhetorical concerns (Carson, 2001). However, if students are highly motivated, then any sort of writing task, expressive or otherwise, are welcomed.

Social factors also influence the quality of contact that learners will experience. Indeed, we cannot assume that "more contact" with the target language will result in more acquisition of the L2. Certainly, instructors recommend that students studying English for academic purposes should read academic texts, attend academic lectures, and even work with students who are native speakers in order to become more acquainted with the discourse. However, if they do not engage in the texts, understand the talks, or actively contribute to the study sessions, these activities will have little effect on student progress. Interaction is key. A common complaint among ESL students at university is that they have difficulty meeting native speakers and getting to know them. Students are often disappointed that they do not have as much interaction with native speakers as they had expected. In addition, they often associate with other students from their L1 and speak their native language. Unfortunately, this pattern can slow down L2 development in all skill areas. The instructor is often responsible for providing incentives or opportunities for interactions with native speakers. Generally speaking, if L2 learners are motivated to integrate into the L2, they will develop a higher level of proficiency and positive attitudes, which can have a positive effect on their writing.

In short, learners may continue to exhibit errors in their writing for the following social reasons:

  • negative attitudes toward the target language
  • continued lack of progress in the L2
  • a wide social and psychological distance between them and the target culture, and,
  • a lack of integrative and instrumental motivation for learning.

Cognitive Factors

Academic writing is believed to be cognitively complex. Acquisition of academic vocabulary and discourse style is particularly difficult. According to cognitive theory, communicating orally or in writing is an active process of skill development and gradual elimination of errors as the learner internalizes the language. Indeed, acquisition is a product of the complex interaction of the linguistic environment and the learner's internal mechanisms. With practice, there is continual restructuring as learners shift these internal representations in order to achieve increasing degrees of mastery in L2 (McLaughlin, 1988). [-6-]

One model that applies to both speaking and writing in a second language is Anderson's (1985) model of language production, which can be divided into three stages: construction, in which the writer plans what he/she is going to write by brainstorming, using a mind-map or outline; transformation, in which language rules are applied to transform intended meanings into the form of the message when the writer is composing or revising; and execution, which corresponds to the physical process of producing the text. The first two stages have been described as "setting goals and searching memory for information, then using production systems to generate language in phrases or constituents" (O'Malley & Chamot, 1990, p. 42). Writers vascillate between these processes as they actively develop the meaning they wish to express in writing. Anderson's learning theory supports teaching approaches that combine the development of language and content knowledge, practice in using this knowledge, and strategy training to encourage independent learning (Snow, 2001).

In structuring information, the writer uses various types of knowledge, including discourse knowledge, understanding of audience, and sociolinguistic rules (O'Malley & Chamot, 1990). Organization at both the sentence and the text level is also important for effective communication of meaning, and ultimately, for the quality of the written product (Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1987). For instance, coherence problems may be due to not knowing how to organize text or how to store the relevant information. The transformation stage involves converting information into meaningful sentences. At this point, the writer translates or changes his/her plans into a mental representation of the goals, ideas, and organization developed in the construction stage. Revision is also part of this stage. As previously mentioned, revision is a cognitively demanding task for L2 learners because it not only involves task definition, evaluation, strategy selection, and modification of text in the writing plan (Grabe & Kaplan, 1996), but also the ability of students to analyze and evaluate the feedback they receive on their writing.

Due to the complex process of writing in a second language, learners often find it difficult to develop all aspects of the stages simultaneously. As a result, they selectively use only those aspects that are automatic or have already been proceduralized (O'Malley & Chamot, 1990). In order to enhance or facilitate language production, students can develop particular learning strategies that isolate component mental processes. O'Malley and Chamot have differentiated strategies into three categories: metacognitive, such as planning the organization of written discourse or monitoring (that is, being aware of what one is doing and responding appropriately to the demands of a task); cognitive, such as transferring or using known linguistic information to facilitate a new learning task or using imagery for recalling and using new vocabulary, and social/affective strategies, which involve cooperating with peers, for example, in peer revision classes.

Learner strategies can be effective, but they need to be internalized so that they can be utilized in adverse learning situations. For example, if an environment is perceived to be stressful or threatening, for example, writing as part of a job interview process, or performing under timed test conditions, learners' affective states can influence cognition. Emotional influences along with cognitive factors can account for achievement and performance in L2, to a certain extent. Schumann (1998) argues that affect may influence cognition through its role in framing a problem and in adopting processing strategies. He states that we very often use feelings as information: "When faced with a situation about which we have to make a judgment we often ask ourselves how we feel about it . . . we may also employ feelings when time constraints and competing tasks limit our cognitive capacities" (p. 247). This outcome may affect the way second language students perform when they are under stress. [-7-]

Language transfer is another important cognitive factor related to writing error. Transfer is defined as the influence resulting from similarities and differences between the target language and any other language that has been previously acquired (Odlin, 1989). The study of transfer involves the study of errors (negative transfer), facilitation (positive transfer), avoidance of target language forms, and their over-use (Ellis, 1994). Behaviorist accounts claim that transfer is the cause of errors, whereas from a cognitive perspective, transfer is seen as a resource that the learner actively draws upon in interlanguage development (Selinker, 1972). In other words, "the L1 can have a direct effect on interlanguage development by influencing the hypotheses that learners construct" (Ellis, 1994, p. 342). According to McLaughlin, transfer errors can occur because:

[L]earners lack the necessary information in the second language or the attentional capacity to activate the appropriate second-language routine. But such an account says little about why certain linguistic forms transfer and others do not. (1988, p. 50)

Despite the fact that L1 transfer is no longer viewed as the only predictor or cause of error at the structural level (since it is difficult to distinguish empirically between instances of communication and language transfer in research studies), a writer's first language plays a complex and significant role in L2 acquisition. For example, when learners write under pressure, they may call upon systematic resources from their native language for the achievement and synthesis of meaning (Widdowson, 1990). Research has also shown that language learners sometimes use their native language when generating ideas and attending to details (Friedlander, 1990). In addition, contrastive studies, which have focused on characteristics of L1 languages and cultures, have helped us predict rhetorical error in writing. These studies have been valuable in our understanding of L2 writing development. However, many feel that these studies have also led to reductive, essentializing generalizations about ways of writing and cultural stereotypes about students from certain linguistic backgrounds (Fox, 1994; Leki, 1997; Spack, 1997). As a result, erroneous predictions about students' learning based on their L1 language and culture have occurred regardless of social factors, such as "the contexts, and purpose of their learning to write, or their age, race, class, gender, education, and prior experience" (Raimes, 1998, p. 143). In addition, learners are influenced by many global phenomena and are themselves continually changing with new experiences. In spite of these criticisms, though, an understanding of "difference among epistemological rhetorical, and pedagogical traditions" (Kern, 2000, p. 176) and the impact of language transfer can be illuminating for an understanding of why learners make certain structural and organizational errors. [-8-]

Input and interaction also play important roles in the writing process, especially in classroom settings. Some studies have indicated that input, along with L1 transfer and communicative need may work together to shape interlanguage (Ellis, 1994; Selinker, 1972). Research has focused on four broad areas: input frequency, the nature of comprehensible input, learner output in interaction, and the processes of collaborative discourse construction. Writers need to receive adequate L2 input in order to form new hypotheses about syntactic and rhetorical forms in the target language. If students are not exposed to native-like models of written texts, their errors in writing are more likely to persist. Errors abound in peer review classes or in computer-mediated exchanges where learners read and respond to each other's compositions. Indeed, in many of my own classes, interlanguage talk or discourse is often the primary source of input for many learners. However, if the interaction, oral or written, allows for adequate negotiation of meaning, peer responses can be very useful. (See Pellettieri (2000) for what happens when learners respond to each other on the computer and read texts containing spelling and grammar errors).

We can see that writing in a second language is a complex process involving the ability to communicate in L2 (learner output) and the ability to construct a text in order to express one's ideas effectively in writing. Social and cognitive factors and learner strategies help us in assessing the underlying reasons why L2 learners exhibit particular writing errors. For instance, the writing problems experienced by Spanish speakers living in the United States may be due to a multiplicity of factors, including the effects of transfer and interference from the Spanish language, and cultural norms (Plata, 1995). Spanish-speaking writers must undergo the task of cognitively exchanging the style of the Spanish language for that of English. For this transformation to happen, some students find that creating another persona, such as replacing their birth name with an English one, can help them to become more immersed in the target language and culture. In short, because learners are less familiar and less confident with structural elements of a new language, rhetorical and cultural conventions and even new uses of writing, writing in an L2 can have errors and be less effective than writing in L1 (Kern, 2000).

The Sources of Error in L2 Writing

There are several ways to think about error in writing in light of what we know about second language acquisition and what we know about how texts, context and the writing process interact with one another. As mentioned, students writing in a second language generally produce texts that contain varying degrees of grammatical and rhetorical errors. In fact, depending on proficiency level, the more content-rich and creative the text, the greater the possibility there is for errors at the morphosyntactic level. These kinds of errors are especially common among L2 writers who have a lot of ideas, but not enough language to express what they want to say in a comprehensible way. What we classify as an error, which is associated with learner competence, may actually be a mistake, or more specifically in an EAP context, a "derailment" related to learner performance (Shaughnessy, 1977). These "derailments" occur when students attempt to use the academic voice and make their sentences more intricate, especially when the task requires more complex ideas.

From behaviorist and mentalist perspectives of error, which have emphasized the product (the error itself) to more constructivist views, which focus on underlying process (why the error is made), researchers have attempted to understand the errors in writers' texts by hypothesizing their possible sources (Bartholomae, 1980; Hull, 1985). Although reading an error-filled text can be tiring and disconcerting, errors can help us identify the cognitive strategies that the learner is using to process information. According to Ellis (1985), it is through analyzing learner errors that we elevate "the status of errors from undesirability to that of a guide to the inner working of the language learning process" (p. 53). [-9-]

Whether an error, mistake, or "derailment," awkward discourse can occur for a variety of reasons, some of which have already been mentioned. First of all, learners may translate from L1, or they may try out what they assume is a legitimate structure of the target language, although hindered by insufficient knowledge of correct usage. In the learning process, they often experience native language interference from developmental stages of interlanguage or from nonstandard elements in spoken dialects (a common occurrence in students writing in their native language as well). They also tend to over-generalize the rules for stylistic features when acquiring new discourse structures. In addition, learners are often unsure of what they want to express,which would cause them to make mistakes in any language. Finally, writers in L2 might lack familiarity with new rhetorical structures and the organization of ideas (Carson, 2001; Connor & Kaplan, 1987; Kutz, Groden, & Zamel, 1993; Raimes, 1987). L2 writing relates closely to native-language literacy and particular instructional contexts. Students may not be acquainted with English rhetoric, which can lead to writing that appears off topic or incoherent to many native English speakers. Rhetoric and writing are direct outcomes of sociocultural and political contexts; in other words, they are schematic representations of the writer's unique experiences within a particular social milieu. For example, Chinese or Indonesian students may write in accordance with a set of rhetorical norms (such as the "eight-legged" essay) that differ from those of English (Cai, 1999; Matalene, 1985; Williams, 1989).

Repeating a previous mistake, or backsliding, is a common occurrence in L2 writing. More important, though, is the issue of fossilization--when "learner interlanguage competence diverges in more or less permanent ways from the target language grammar" (Odlin, 1994, p. 13). Fossilized errors can be problematic in writing because the errors become ingrained, like bad habits, in a learner's repertoire, and they reappear despite remediation and correction. They can be common among immigrants who have learned much of the L2 "on the street," where the emphasis is on fluency and not linguistic correctness. Errors in writing, fossilized or otherwise, can be glaring, especially to the reader who has had little experience interacting with L2 speakers and texts.

Implications for Teaching: Proficiency, Instruction and Response to Error

Although instructors may think of errors as part of a language learning process related to linguistic, situational, and psycholinguistic contexts (Carson, 2001), and writing as a skill developed over time, most L2 learners' writing is judged according to criteria that are static and product-based. That teachers draw conclusions about intellectual ability on the basis of structural and grammatical problems has also been well documented (Sternglass, 1997; Zamel, 1998). Variability in writing, which is typical of a learner's interlanguage, is a concern when addressing proficiency issues. The definition of proficiency has consequences for L2 students; it affects their ability to complete writing tasks across the disciplines, cope with the demands of academic English, and receive recognition as well-informed, critical thinkers. [-10-]

One problem in assessing language performance is that it must address the many factors related to the contexts in which language is used. According to Bialystok (1998), any definition of language proficiency is deeply entangled in theoretical attitude. On the one hand, there is the formalist approach, which attempts to explain language as code. According to this perspective, "language proficiency is an ultimately unknowable abstraction that reflects the universal competence of native speakers" (p. 502). On the other hand, there is the functionalist approach, which explains proficiency in its relationship to communication in specific contexts. In this respect, it is "the outcome of social interaction with a linguistic environment" (p. 502). In conversation, often both parties assume some common knowledge and take advantage of verbal and nonverbal communication; however, in written discourse, common knowledge cannot be assumed; therefore, the writer may need to provide more background information in order to communicate clearly.

Language requires a combination of formal structure, that is, a clear set of standards, and communicative application, which includes recognition of variations from the rules. Consequently, a proper definition of language proficiency would "present identifiable standards against which to describe language skills of users in different contexts" (Bialystok, 1998, p. 504). A more complete conceptualization of language performance, then, acknowledges personal characteristics, topical or real-world knowledge, and affective schemata, among other factors related to the social and cultural context (Brown, 2000).

Alongside the cultural and curricular aspects of standardization, there is variability in the process of L2 learning. Learners vary in the ultimate level of proficiency they achieve, with many failing to reach target-language competence. This variation is often the result of individual learner differences in motivation and aptitude, in addition to the use of an assortment of strategies, such as inferencing and self-monitoring for obtaining input and for learning from it (Ellis, 1994; Krashen, 1982). However, instead of setting the standard as a well-defined, functionally balanced system, and proficiency as the degree of deviation from this norm, with errors "marked, counted and statistically analyzed," Klein (1998) advocates acknowledging learner varieties. According to Klein, these are "systems in their own right, error-free by definition and characterized by particular lexical repertoire and particular interaction of organizational principles" (p. 538). In fact, it may be more useful to think about proficiency as a process, one in which learners alternate in their use of linguistic forms according to the linguistic and situational contexts (Ellis, 1994). From a functionalist perspective, communicative competence in writing should also take into consideration learner variability and error within particular contexts. Nevertheless, for L2 writers, the greater the language proficiency (however defined), the better the writing quality. In fact, both language proficiency and composing abilities can, or perhaps should be, accounted for in evaluating L2 writing performance and instruction (Grabe & Kaplan, 1996).

Valuable insights from research in second language acquisition and writing development can assist in developing instructional techniques linking the two processes--acquiring a second language and developing writing skills, especially for academic purposes. Both Flower (1994) and Bereiter and Scardamalia (1987) have stressed the benefits of process approaches to writing instruction and the need for more knowledge-transforming tasks. Taking the concept of "knowledge transformation" further, Wells (2000) argues that writing approached in this way is also an opportunity for knowledge building, "as the writer both tries to anticipate the likely response of the envisaged audience and carries on a dialogue with the text being composed" (p. 77). However, if students have not developed learning strategies to monitor their writing errors, and if they do not receive enough conceptual feedback at the discourse level, then the positive effects of the instruction may backfire. Instructional approaches that can be used effectively with L2 writers show us what is at stake for L2 instructors and students alike. [-11-]

First of all, students may be able to communicate more effectively if they are exposed to models of not only standard paragraphs and essays, but also a variety of genres of writing, including flyers, magazine articles, letters, and so forth. By examining a variety of written texts, students' awareness can be raised with regard to the way words, structures, and genre contribute to purposeful writing. They can also be made aware of different types of textual organization, which can in turn affect L2 students' composing processes (Swales, 1990; Raimes, 1991, 1998). Models can also be used for text analysis, which can help L2 writers see how particular grammatical features are used in authentic discourse contexts. Depending on the learners' levels of proficiency and writing abilities, models can seem fairly formulaic, as in the knowledge-telling model of the five-paragraph essay. However, as the students progress, they need to be aware of a variety of forms that "serve the writer's purpose instead of the other way around" (Atkinson & Ramanathan, 1995, p. 548). Cazden (1992) advocates the practice of scripting and performing texts in order to sensitize students to the many voices in a reading and how they interact. In this way, models of the target language are reinforced.

In addition to the use of written models, Cumming (1995) also points out the benefits of cognitive modeling in writing instruction, which involves explicit demonstration of the strategies experienced writers use when planning, making decisions, and revising texts. He also advocates that ESL instructors make explicit use of thinking or procedural-facilitation prompts and student self-evaluation as the optimal mode of assessment. Both these approaches promote knowledge-transforming models of composing. Self-evaluation can be encouraged in student portfolios, self-review checklists, and teacher and peer responses. In addition, verbalizing the writing process step-by-step can be effective, as it affords both students and teachers the opportunity to consider writing dialogically. However, convincing students to evaluate their own work requires additional instructional tools, and it may not be effective for all learners. Granted, Cummings refers to self-assessment as a component of one-to-one tutoring sessions, which in contrast to the classroom context, are ideally "more conducive environments for the textual, cognitive, and social dimensions of error identification to be integrated with individual students' composing processes and their immediate concerns about language, ideas, and texts" (p. 393). Unfortunately, many teachers have large classes; nonetheless, the use of specific prompts for cognitive modeling in different aspects of composing, including prompts for error identification, has proved to be valuable.

Apprenticeship models of instruction, which developed out of Vygotsky's sociocultural theories of language and literacy, are also becoming more common. Proficient students who are also fairly skilled writers can benefit from this approach. They start with what they already know and can do, but their learning is extended into what Vygotsky termed the "zone of proximal development" through strategic instruction, collaborative construction of opportunities and active participation (Lantolf, 2000; Schinke-Llano, 1995). Apprenticeship models enable learners to utilize the new language as a tool in the process of becoming self-regulatory. Similar to Cumming's suggestions for fostering writing expertise, "students are supported by a scaffold of prompts and explanations, by extensive modeling, by in-process support, and by reflection that connects strategic effort to outcomes" (Flower, 1994, pp. 142-143). Drawing on and revising student knowledge of genres, reflecting on strategies for approaching a variety of literary tasks, and cultivating a metalanguage for discussing texts are important components of socio-literate methods (Johns, 1999). [-12-]

Students come to class both to improve their language proficiency and become more confident in their writing abilities. Writing practice can also present diagnostic feedback that helps learners improve their linguistic accuracy at every level of proficiency. Instruction should provide students with ample amounts of language input and instruction, as well as writing experience (preferably through the interweaving of writing and reading, referred to as "intertextuality" (Blanton, 1999), and feedback to fulfill their goals. Overt classroom instruction through modeling, for instance, is only one part of the teaching process; providing students with feedback on their writing is the other. Essentially, we need to consider factors related to language proficiency, second language acquisition, and writing skill development when giving feedback. Specifically, the effectiveness of feedback may depend on the level of students' motivation, their current language level, their cognitive style, the clarity of the feedback given, the way the feedback is used, and the attitudes of students toward their teacher and the class (Ferris, 1997; Goldstein, 2001; Omaggio Hadley, 1993). Classroom settings, course goals, and grading procedures and standards are also important (Leki, 1990). Systematically encouraging learners to reflect on what they want to write and then helping them to make an appropriate choice of language forms has pedagogic value.

We must be aware of the complexities involved in the revision process and respond to writing so that students can make modifications with confidence and competence. Ideally, learners should be encouraged to analyze and evaluate feedback themselves in order for it to be truly effective. Teacher commentary, student reactions to commentary, and student revisions interact with each other in a formidable way. How teachers intervene in writing instruction, and how L2 writers react to the feedback influences the composing process. Should teachers stress early mastery of the mechanical aspects of writing, or should they urge their students to pay little attention to correctness, at least until after a first draft has been written? Again, process models of writing instruction allow students time to reflect and seek input as they reshape their plans, ideas, and language. In classroom practice, the focus is on idea development, clarity, and coherence before identification and grammar correction. Ideally, instruction and response serve to motivate revisions, encourage learning, induce problem-solving and critical thinking, in addition to further writing practice (Cumming, 1989; White, 1994; Zamel, 1987). Indeed, the process approach may be effective, but if writers' linguistic ability sets limits to what they can do conceptually or affects the writing process itself, then we need a combination of process instruction and attention to language development.

Focused error correction can be highly desirable, but problematic;. In addition, there are many contradictory findings. The initial impulse for many teachers when reading L2 student writing is to edit the work, that is, focus on the structural aspects so that the writing closer resembles target language discourse. Teachers can correct errors; code errors; locate errors, and indicate the number of errors. To its benefit, attention to errors "provides the negative evidence students often need to reject or modify their hypotheses about how the target language is formed or functions" (Larsen-Freeman, 1991, p. 293). [-13-] However, if this focus on error becomes the totality of the response, then language, discourse, and text are equated with structure. It is then assumed that the instructor has the authority to change the student's text and correct it (Rodby, 1992). In addition, some feel it may not be worth the instructor's time and effort to provide detailed feedback on sentence level grammar and syntax, since improvement can be gained by writing practice alone (Robb, Ross, & Shortreed, 1986). Practice alone may improve fluency, but if errors are not pointed out and corrected, they can become ingrained or fossilized in student writing, as mentioned earlier. L1 research may advocate for focusing on conception and organization, and not on mechanical errors, except for a "note reminding the student that the final copy needs to be edited" (White, 1994, p. 109). However, survey reports in L2 have indicated that students both attend to and appreciate their teachers' pointing out of grammar problems (Brice, 1995; Cohen, 1987; Ferris, 1995, 1997; Leki, 1991; Radecki & Swales, 1988). In support of this claim, Fathman and Whalley (1990), from their research on feedback and revision in an ESL context, concluded that grammar and content feedback, whether given separately or together, positively affect rewriting. However, grammatical feedback had more effect on error correction than content feedback had on the improvement of content. Grammatical and rhetorical feedback should be attentive to the writers' level of proficiency and degree of readiness (Ferris, 1995, Hedgcock & Lefkowitz, 1996; Lee, 1997; Leki, 1991). Overly detailed responses may overwhelm L2 writers and discourage substantive revision, whereas minimal feedback may result in only surface modifications to the text. Furthermore, learners may be uncertain about what to do with various suggestions and how to incorporate them into their own revision processes. More research on the effectiveness of responses on revision should be examined. (See Sengupta (2000) for research on the effects of explicit teaching of revision strategies on L2 learners' writing proficiency and perceptions about writing).

Summary and Conclusion

For English L2 writers, the process of writing in an academic environment is challenging. I used to tell my students that the only way to improve their writing is to keep writing--thinking that with enough practice in writing and revision (involving problem solving and reflection), they would eventually acquire the fundamentals, or at least the standard, required of academic discourse. Although the process approach to instruction, characterized by practice, collaboration, and the opportunity for revision, may be suitable for most English L1 writers, it is apparent that many L2 writers do not have the necessary linguistic ability to reap the benefits of the approach. As Yau (1991) points out:

[A]lthough we should not cripple our students' interest in writing through undue stress or grammatical correctness, the influence of second language factors on writing performance is something we have to reckon with and not pretend that concentrating on the process would automatically resolve the difficulty caused by these factors. (p. 268)

Kern (2000) also mentions that process-oriented teaching does not acknowledge the influence of sociocultural context on individual processes. He has characterized it as inattentive to "learners' understanding of links between form and communicative conventions that will allow them to construct meanings in ways that are appropriate within the immediate academic context as well as the larger societal context" (p. 182). [-14-]

Feedback is of utmost importance to the writing process. Without individual attention and sufficient feedback on errors, improvement will not take place. We must accept the fact that L2 writing contains errors; it is our responsibility to help learners to develop strategies for self-correction and regulation. Indeed, L2 writers require and expect specific overt feedback from teachers not only on content, but also on the form and structure of writing. If this feedback is not part of the instructional process, then students will be disadvantaged in improving both writing and language skills.

In order to learn more about L2 writers' use of language in the process of writing, we need to apply to L2 writing the research methods utilized in exploring the composing process in L1 writing, such as think-aloud protocols. We also need to understand how students compose in both their native languages and in English to understand more about their learning strategies (especially in monitoring errors), the role of translation, and transfer of skills. Certainly, ethnographic research in L2 writing that examines the writing process, along with the acquisition of communicative competence, will help to create a more comprehensive theory of L2 writing.

About the Author

Johanne Myles has been teaching ESL, EAP, and TESL for over 20 years in Canada and abroad. She is presently working on a Ph.D in Education with a focus on cultural and curriculum studies at Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. Her research interests include intercultural communication, second language acquisition and second language writing. She intends to conduct ethnographic research on the communicative competence of engineering students who are non-native speakers of English in the workplace environment when on their internships.

Anderson, J. (1985). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: W.H. Freeman.

Atkinson, D. & Ramanathan, V. (1995). Cultures of writing: An ethnographic comparison of L1 and L2 university writing/language programs. TESOL Quarterly, 29, 539-568.

Bartholomae, D. (1980). Study of error. College Composition and Communication, 31, 253-269.

Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bialystok, E. (1998). Coming of age in applied linguistics. Language Learning, 48, 497-518.

Blanton, L. (1999). Classroom instruction and language minority students: On teaching to"Smarter" readers and writers. In L. Harklau, K. Losey & M. Siegal (Eds.), Generation 1.5 meets college composition (pp. 119-142). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Brice, C. (1995). ESL writers' reactions to teacher commentary: A case study. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 394 312).

Brown, H.D. (2000). Principles of language learning and teaching (4th ed.). White Plains, NY: Longman.

Cai, G. (1999). Texts in contexts: Understanding Chinese students' English compositions. In C. Cooper & L. Odell (Eds.), Evaluating writing: The role of teachers' knowledge about text, learning and culture (pp. 279-297).Urbana, Ill: National Council of Teachers of English.

Carson, J. (2001). Second language writing and second language acquisition. In T. Silva and P.Matsuda (Eds.), On second language writing (pp. 191-200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [-15-]

Cazden, C. (1992). Performing expository texts in the foreign language classroom. In C. Kramsch & S. McConnell-Ginet (Eds.), Text and context: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on language study (pp. 67-78). Lexington, MA: D.C.Heath and Company.

Cohen, A. (1987). Student processing of feedback on their compositions. In A. Wendon and J. Rubin (Eds.), Learner strategies in language learning (pp. 57-69). UK: Prentice Hall International.

Coleman, H. (Ed.), (1996). Society and the language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Connor, U. (1997). Contrastive rhetoric: Implications for teachers of writing in multicultural classrooms. In C. Severino, J. Guerra and J. Butler (Eds.), Writing in multicultural settings (pp. 198-208). New York: Modern Language Association of America.

Connor, U. & Kaplan, R. (Eds.), (1987). Writing across languages: Analysis of L2 text. USA: Addison-Wesley.

Cumming, A. (1989). Writing expertise and second language proficiency. Language Learning, 39, 81-141.

Cumming, A. (1995). Fostering writing expertise in ESL composition instruction: Modeling and evaluation. In D. Belcher & G. Braine (Eds.), Academic writing in a second language (pp. 375-397). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Co.

Ellis, R. (1985). Understanding second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Institute of English.

Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Fathman, A. & Whalley, E. (1990). Teacher response to student writing: Focus on form versus content. In B. Kroll (Ed), Second language writing: Research insights for the classroom (pp. 178-190). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Ferris, D. (1995). Student reactions to teacher response in multiple-draft composition classrooms. TESOL Quarterly, 29, 33-53.

Ferris, D. (1997). The influence of teacher commentary on student revision. TESOL Quarterly, 31, 315-339.

Flower, L. (1994). The construction of negotiated meaning: A social cognitive theory of writing. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

Flower, L. & Hayes, J. (1980). The dynamics of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In L. Gregg & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 31-50). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Flower, L. & Hayes, J. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. College Composition and Communication, 32, 365-387.

Flower, L., Stein, V., Ackerman, J., Kantz, M., McCormick, K., & Peck, W., (1990). Reading-to-write: Exploring a cognitive and social process. New York: Oxford University Press.

Fox, H. (1994). Listening to the world: Cultural issues in academic writing. Urbana Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English.

Friedlander, A. (1990). Composing in English: Effects of a first language on writing in English as a second language. In B. Kroll (Ed.), Second language writing: Research insights for the classroom. (pp. 109-125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gardner, R. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning: The role of attitude and motivation. London: Edward Arnold.

Giles, H., Robinson, P. & Smith, P. (Eds.) (1980). Language: Social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Pergamon.

Giles, H. & Byrne, J. (1982). An intergroup approach to second language acquisition. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 3, 17-40. [-16-]

Goldstein, L. (2001). For Kyla: What does the research say about responding to ESL writers. In T. Silva and P. Matsuda (Eds.), On second language writing (pp. 73-90). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Grabe, W. (2001). Notes toward a theory of second language writing. In T. Silva and P. Matsuda (Eds.), On second language writing (pp. 39-58). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Grabe, W. & Kaplan, R. (1996). Theory and practice of writing: An applied linguistic perspective. New York: Longman.

Hamers, J. & Blanc, M. (1982). Towards a social-psychological model of bilingual development. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 1, 29-50.

Hedgcock, J. & Lefkowitz, N. (1996). Some input on input: Two analyses of student response to expert feedback in L2 writing. Modern Language Journal, 80, 287-308.

Holliday, A. (1997). Appropriate methodology and social context Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Hull, G. (1985). Research on error and correction. In B. McClelland & T. Donovan (Eds.), Perspectives on research and scholarship in composition (pp. 162-184). New York: The Modern Language Association of America.

Johns, A. (1999). Opening our doors: Applying socioliterate approaches (SA) to language minority classrooms. In L. Harklau, K. Losey & M. Siegal (Eds), Generation 1.5 meets college composition (pp. 159-171). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Johns, A. (1995). Teaching classroom and authentic genres: Initiating students into academic cultures and discourses. In D. Belcher & G. Braine (Eds.), Academic writing in a second language (pp. 277-291). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Co.

Kaplan, R. (1987). Cultural thought patterns revisited. In U. Connor & R. Kaplan (Eds.), Writing across languages: Analysis of L2 text (pp. 9-21). Reading, Mass: Addison Wesley.

Kern, R. (2000). Literacy and language teaching. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.

Klein, W. (1998). The contribution of second language acquisition research. Language Learning, 48, 527-549.

Kobayashi, H. & Rinnert, C. (1996). Factors affecting composition evaluation in an EFL context: Cultural rhetorical pattern and reader's background. Language Learning, 46, 397-437.

Kogen, M. (1986). The conventions of expository writing. Journal of Basic Writing, 5, 24-37.

Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Kutz, E., Groden, S., & Zamel, V. (1993). The discovery of competence: Teaching and learning with diverse student writers. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook Publishers.

Lambert, W. (1975). Culture and language as factors in learning and education. In A. Wolfgang (Ed.), Education of immigrant students (pp. 55-83). Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.

Lantolf, J. (Ed.) (2000). Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Larsen-Freeman, D. (1991). Teaching grammar. In M. Celce-Murcia (Ed.), Teaching English as a second or foreign language (2nd ed.) Boston: Heinle & Heinle Publishers.

Lee, I. (1997). ESL Learners' performance in error correction in writing: Some implications for teaching. System, 15, 465-477.

Leki, I. (1990). Coaching from the margins: Issues in written response. In B. Kroll (Ed.), Second language writing: Research insights for the classroom (pp. 57-67). New York: Cambridge University Press. [-17-]

Leki, I. (1991). The preferences of ESL students for error correction in college-level writing classes. Foreign Language Annals, 24, 203-217.

Leki, I. (1993). Twenty-five years of contrastive rhetoric: Text analysis and writing pedagogies. In S. Silberstein (Ed.), State of the art TESOL essays (pp. 350-370). Virginia: Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages.

Leki, I. (1997). Cross-talk: ESL issues and contrastive rhetoric. In C. Severino, J. Guerra, and J. Butler (Eds.), Writing in multicultural settings (pp. 234-244).New York: Modern Language Association of America

Matalene, C. (1985). Contrastive rhetoric: An American writing teacher in China. College English, 47, 789-808.

McGroarty, M. (1996). Language attitudes, motivation, and standards. In S. McKay & N. Hornberger (Eds.), Sociolinguistics and language teaching (pp. 3-46). New York: Cambridge University Press. [-18-]

McLaughlin, B. (1988). Theories of second-language learning. Baltimore: Edward Arnold.

Odlin, T. (1989 ). Language transfer. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Odlin, T. (1994). Introduction. In T. Odlin (Ed.), Perspectives on pedagogical grammar (pp. 1-22). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Omaggio Hadley, A. (1993). Teaching language in context. Boston: Heinle & Heinle

O'Malley, J. & Chamot, A. (1990). Learning strategies in second language acquisition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Pellettieri, J. (2000). Negotiation in cyberspace: The role of chatting in the development of grammatical competence. In M. Warschauer & R. Kern (Eds.), Network-based language teaching: Concepts and practice (pp. 59-86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Plata, M. (1995). Success of Hispanic college students on a writing examination. Journal of Educational Issue of Language Minority Students [on-line serial], 15. Available: http://www.ncbe.gwu.edu/miscpubs/jeilms/vol15/success.htm

Radecki, P. & Swales, J. (1988). ESL student reaction to written comments on their written work. System, 16, 355-365.

Raimes, A. (1985). What unskilled ESL students do as they write: A classroom study of composing. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 229-257.

Raimes, A. (1987). Language proficiency, writing ability and composing strategies: A study of ESL college student writers. Language Learning, 37, 439-468.

Raimes, A. (1991). Out of the woods: Traditions in the teaching of writing. TESOL Quarterly, 25, 407-430.

Raimes, A. (1998). Teaching writing. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 18, 142-167.

Robb, T., Ross, S. & Shortreed, I. (1986). Salience of feedback on error and its effect on EFL writing quality. TESOL Quarterly, 20, 83-93.

Rodby, J. (1992). Appropriating literacy: Writing and reading in English as a second language. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook Publishers.

Schinke-Llano, L. (1995). Reenvisioning the second language classroom: A Vygotskian approach. In F. Eckman, D. Highland, P. Lee, J. Milcham & R. Weber (Eds.), Second language acquisition theory and pedagogy (pp. 21-28), Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [-19-]

Schumann, J. (1978). The pidginization process: A model for second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

Schumann, J. (1998). The neurobiology of affect in language. Language Learning, 48, Supplement 1, 527-549.

Sengupta, S. (2000). An investigation into the effects of revision strategy instruction on L2 secondary school learners. System, 28, 97-113.

Shaughnessy, M. (1977). Errors and expectations. New York: Oxford University Press.

Shen, F. (1998). The classroom and the wider culture: Identity as a key to learning composition in English. In V. Zamel & R. Spack (Eds.), Negotiating academic literacies: Teaching and learning across languages and cultures (pp. 123-134). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 10, 209-231.

Silva, T. (1993). Toward an understanding of the distinct nature of L2 writing: The ESL research and its implications. TESOL Quarterly, 27, 657-677.

Snow, M. A. (2001). Content-based and immersion models for second and foreign language teaching. In M. Celce-Murcia (Ed.), Teaching English as a second or foreign language (3rd ed.) (pp. 303-318). Boston: Heinle & Heinle.

Spack, R. (1997). The rhetorical construction of multilingual students. TESOL Quarterly, 31, 765-74.

Sternglass, M. (1997). Time to know them: A longitudinal study of writing and learning at the college level. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

Swales, J. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Valdes, J. (1995) (Ed.) Culture bound. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Wells, G. (2000). Dialogic inquiry in education: Building on the legacy of Vygotsky. In C. Lee & P. Smagorinsky (Eds.), Vygotskian perspectives on literary research (pp. 51-85). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Widdowson, H. (1990). Aspects of language teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

White, E. (1994). Teaching and assessing writing. (2nd ed.) San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers

Williams, J. (1989). Preparing to teach writing. California: Wadsworth Publishing Co.

Yau, M. (1991). The role of language factors in second language writing. In L. Malave & G. Duquette (Eds), Language, culture and cognition: A collection of studies in first and second language acquisition (pp. 266-283). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

Zamel, V. (1983). The composing processes of advanced ESL students: Six case studies. TESOL Quarterly, 17, 165-187.

Zamel, V. (1987). Recent research on writing pedagogy. TESOL Quarterly, 21, 697-715.

Zamel, V. (1998). Strangers in academia: The experiences of faculty and ESL students across the curriculum. In V. Zamel & R. Spack (Eds.), Negotiating academic literacies: Teaching and learning across languages and cultures (pp. 249-264). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts

Latest science news, discoveries and analysis

research on error analysis in writing

Chinese virologist who was first to share COVID genome sleeps on street after lab shuts

research on error analysis in writing

Scientists tried to give people COVID — and failed

research on error analysis in writing

The science of 3 Body Problem: what’s fact and what’s fiction?

research on error analysis in writing

Do cutting-edge CAR-T-cell therapies cause cancer? What the data say

‘chatgpt for crispr’ creates new gene-editing tools, this social sciences hub galvanized india’s dynamic growth. can it survive, first fetus-to-fetus transplant demonstrated in rats, how reliable is this research tool flags papers discussed on pubpeer, why doing science is difficult in india today yamini aiyar.

research on error analysis in writing

85 million cells — and counting — at your fingertips

research on error analysis in writing

How I’m supporting other researchers who have moved to Lithuania

research on error analysis in writing

Should the Maldives be creating new land?

research on error analysis in writing

‘Shut up and calculate’: how Einstein lost the battle to explain quantum reality

What china’s mission to collect rocks from the moon's far side could reveal, epic blazes threaten arctic permafrost. can fire-fighters save it, superconductivity hunt gets boost from china's $220 million physics 'playground', bird flu virus has been spreading among us cows for months, rna reveals.

research on error analysis in writing

Retractions are part of science, but misconduct isn’t — lessons from a superconductivity lab

research on error analysis in writing

Any plan to make smoking obsolete is the right step

research on error analysis in writing

Citizenship privilege harms science

How to meet africa’s grand challenges with african know-how alfred r. bizoza, ecologists: don’t lose touch with the joy of fieldwork chris mantegna, current issue.

Issue Cover

The Maldives is racing to create new land. Why are so many people concerned?

Surprise hybrid origins of a butterfly species, stripped-envelope supernova light curves argue for central engine activity, optical clocks at sea, research analysis.

research on error analysis in writing

Elephant-nose fish ‘see’ farther by electric sensing when in groups

research on error analysis in writing

Ancient DNA traces family lines and political shifts in the Avar empire

research on error analysis in writing

A chemical method for selective labelling of the key amino acid tryptophan

research on error analysis in writing

Robust optical clocks promise stable timing in a portable package

How to stop students cramming for exams send them to sea, targeting rna opens therapeutic avenues for timothy syndrome, bioengineered ‘mini-colons’ shed light on cancer progression, galaxy found napping in the primordial universe.

research on error analysis in writing

I strive to make the Great Barrier Reef more resilient to heat stress

research on error analysis in writing

Scientists urged to collect royalties from the ‘magic money tree’

research on error analysis in writing

Breaking ice, and helicopter drops: winning photos of working scientists

I fell out of love with the lab, and in love with business, how bioinformatics led one scientist home to lithuania, books & culture.

research on error analysis in writing

How volcanoes shaped our planet — and why we need to be ready for the next big eruption

research on error analysis in writing

Dogwhistles, drilling and the roots of Western civilization: Books in brief

research on error analysis in writing

Cosmic rentals

Las borinqueñas remembers the forgotten puerto rican women who tested the first pill, dad always mows on summer saturday mornings, nature podcast.

Nature Podcast

Latest videos

Nature briefing.

An essential round-up of science news, opinion and analysis, delivered to your inbox every weekday.

research on error analysis in writing

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Department of History

Gallito awarded a summer 2024 pura.

Gallito awarded a Summer 2024 PURA

Congratulations to History major Hilary Gallito on being awarded a Summer 2024 PURA (Provost’s Undergraduate Research Award) for her project proposal titled, “War, Women, and a Revolution: An Analysis of Classical Themes in Early American Women’s Writing!”

Summer PURA operates as the summer cycle for the PURA program. This program was created to assist and encourage Hopkins undergraduate students to continue or begin independent research, scholarly and creative projects with our amazing university mentors over the summer and without curricular obligations inhibiting full-time effort.

Office of Undergraduate Research

How do ssris interact with maternal physiology and offspring neurodevelopment.

Project and Position Details We are looking for an undergraduate or post-graduate volunteer laboratory intern to help complete projects related to the prenatal and gestational impacts of SSRI use. Students will be exposed to a variety of wet lab and in silico techniques, including but not limited to evaluating maternal care and depression-like behaviors in mice, brain and placenta dissections and microdissections, animal necropsy, working with clinical samples (human placenta, maternal plasma, cord blood), and molecular techniques (qPCR, ELISA, immunohistochemistry, metabolomics). Students will have opportunities for data analysis, manuscript writing, and presenting. The Santillan Lab is a welcoming, positive environment that encourages creativity, teamwork, and scientific development! Past lab members have gone on to graduate school, medical school, nursing school, dental school, and related training programs.

Qualifications Some undergraduate education or more

Time Commitment 10 hrs/week minimum

Compensation Volunteer Academic Credit

Start Date   Immediate, over the summer

Project Duration  Ongoing or potential to be a continuous position

How to Apply Email [email protected].

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Here’s how you know

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Take action

  • Report an antitrust violation
  • File adjudicative documents
  • Find banned debt collectors
  • View competition guidance
  • Competition Matters Blog

New HSR thresholds and filing fees for 2024

View all Competition Matters Blog posts

We work to advance government policies that protect consumers and promote competition.

View Policy

Search or browse the Legal Library

Find legal resources and guidance to understand your business responsibilities and comply with the law.

Browse legal resources

  • Find policy statements
  • Submit a public comment

research on error analysis in writing

Vision and Priorities

Memo from Chair Lina M. Khan to commission staff and commissioners regarding the vision and priorities for the FTC.

Technology Blog

Consumer facing applications: a quote book from the tech summit on ai.

View all Technology Blog posts

Advice and Guidance

Learn more about your rights as a consumer and how to spot and avoid scams. Find the resources you need to understand how consumer protection law impacts your business.

  • Report fraud
  • Report identity theft
  • Register for Do Not Call
  • Sign up for consumer alerts
  • Get Business Blog updates
  • Get your free credit report
  • Find refund cases
  • Order bulk publications
  • Consumer Advice
  • Shopping and Donating
  • Credit, Loans, and Debt
  • Jobs and Making Money
  • Unwanted Calls, Emails, and Texts
  • Identity Theft and Online Security
  • Business Guidance
  • Advertising and Marketing
  • Credit and Finance
  • Privacy and Security
  • By Industry
  • For Small Businesses
  • Browse Business Guidance Resources
  • Business Blog

Servicemembers: Your tool for financial readiness

Visit militaryconsumer.gov

Get consumer protection basics, plain and simple

Visit consumer.gov

Learn how the FTC protects free enterprise and consumers

Visit Competition Counts

Looking for competition guidance?

  • Competition Guidance

News and Events

Latest news, ftc expands patent listing challenges, targeting more than 300 junk listings for diabetes, weight loss, asthma and copd drugs.

View News and Events

Upcoming Event

Older adults and fraud: what you need to know.

View more Events

Sign up for the latest news

Follow us on social media

-->   -->   -->   -->   -->  

gaming controller illustration

Playing it Safe: Explore the FTC's Top Video Game Cases

Learn about the FTC's notable video game cases and what our agency is doing to keep the public safe.

Latest Data Visualization

Visualization of FTC Refunds to Consumers

FTC Refunds to Consumers

Explore refund statistics including where refunds were sent and the dollar amounts refunded with this visualization.

About the FTC

Our mission is protecting the public from deceptive or unfair business practices and from unfair methods of competition through law enforcement, advocacy, research, and education.

Learn more about the FTC

Lina M. Khan

Meet the Chair

Lina M. Khan was sworn in as Chair of the Federal Trade Commission on June 15, 2021.

Chair Lina M. Khan

Looking for legal documents or records? Search the Legal Library instead.

  • Cases and Proceedings
  • Premerger Notification Program
  • Merger Review
  • Anticompetitive Practices
  • Competition and Consumer Protection Guidance Documents
  • Warning Letters
  • Consumer Sentinel Network
  • Criminal Liaison Unit
  • FTC Refund Programs
  • Notices of Penalty Offenses
  • Advocacy and Research
  • Advisory Opinions
  • Cooperation Agreements
  • Federal Register Notices
  • Public Comments
  • Policy Statements
  • International
  • Office of Technology Blog
  • Military Consumer
  • Consumer.gov
  • Bulk Publications
  • Data and Visualizations
  • Stay Connected
  • Commissioners and Staff
  • Bureaus and Offices
  • Budget and Strategy
  • Office of Inspector General
  • Careers at the FTC

Fact Sheet on FTC’s Proposed Final Noncompete Rule

Facebook

  • Competition
  • Office of Policy Planning
  • Bureau of Competition

The following outline provides a high-level overview of the FTC’s proposed final rule :

  • Specifically, the final rule provides that it is an unfair method of competition—and therefore a violation of Section 5 of the FTC Act—for employers to enter into noncompetes with workers after the effective date.
  • Fewer than 1% of workers are estimated to be senior executives under the final rule.
  • Specifically, the final rule defines the term “senior executive” to refer to workers earning more than $151,164 annually who are in a “policy-making position.”
  • Reduced health care costs: $74-$194 billion in reduced spending on physician services over the next decade.
  • New business formation: 2.7% increase in the rate of new firm formation, resulting in over 8,500 additional new businesses created each year.
  • This reflects an estimated increase of about 3,000 to 5,000 new patents in the first year noncompetes are banned, rising to about 30,000-53,000 in the tenth year.
  • This represents an estimated increase of 11-19% annually over a ten-year period.
  • The average worker’s earnings will rise an estimated extra $524 per year. 

The Federal Trade Commission develops policy initiatives on issues that affect competition, consumers, and the U.S. economy. The FTC will never demand money, make threats, tell you to transfer money, or promise you a prize. Follow the  FTC on social media , read  consumer alerts  and the  business blog , and  sign up to get the latest FTC news and alerts .

Press Release Reference

Contact information, media contact.

Victoria Graham Office of Public Affairs 415-848-5121

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) AN ERROR ANALYSIS IN WRITING THESIS ABSTRACT OF ENGLISH DEPARTMENT

    research on error analysis in writing

  2. PPT

    research on error analysis in writing

  3. [PDF] ERROR ANALYSIS IN WRITING AN ENGLISH NARRATIVE COMPOSITION

    research on error analysis in writing

  4. (PDF) Error Analysis in a Written Composition

    research on error analysis in writing

  5. An Introduction to Error Analysis

    research on error analysis in writing

  6. (PDF) STUDENTS’ ERRORS ON WRITING (An Analysis Studies At Junior High

    research on error analysis in writing

VIDEO

  1. Error Analysis 3rd Class

  2. Error Analysis 3rd Class

  3. Error Analysis 1st Class

  4. Error Analysis 2nd Class

  5. Error Analysis

  6. Experimental methods and Error analysis

COMMENTS

  1. Error Analysis in Students' Writing Composition

    Further analysis of this research is to find out the types of errors and the most dominant type s of errors especially in the aspect of grammar. According to Quirk et al., as cited in Kast (20 08 ...

  2. PDF Error Analysis of Written Essays: Do Private School Students Show ...

    International Journal of Research in Education and Science (IJRES) 609 (2002), the ability to produce an error-free piece of writing in L2 is a 'major achievement'.

  3. An error-analysis study from an EFL writing context: Human and

    Evaluating written texts is believed to be a time-consuming process that can lack consistency and objectivity. Automated essay scoring (AES) can provide solutions to some of the limitations of human scoring. This research aimed to evaluate the performance of one AES system, Grammarly, in comparison to human raters. Both approaches' performances were analyzed quantitatively using Corder's ...

  4. Error Analysis, Theories of Language, and the Teaching of Writing

    Children's writing: Research directions and hypotheses based upon an examination of the writing process of seven-year-old children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo.

  5. PDF Linguistic Error Analysis on Students' Thesis Proposals

    Kikula and Qorro's (2007) research objective was to identify the common mistakes and problems in research proposal writing. The data consisted of 240 proposals. Their results revealed that the top three most problematic issues were: writing the research problem, articulating the importance of the research problem, and proposing an appropriate ...

  6. Error Analysis: Past, Present, and Future

    Patterns of strengths and weaknesses (PSW) on the WISC-V, DAS-II, and KABC-II and their relationship to students' errors in oral language, reading, writing, spelling, and math. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment , 35(1-2), 168-185.

  7. PDF An error-analysis study from an EFL writing context: Human and

    An error-analysis study from an EFL writing context: Human and Automated Essay Scoring Approaches Norah Almusharraf1 · Hind Alotaibi2 Technology, Knowledge and Learning (2023) 28:1015-1031 ... research is needed to evaluate these systems' performance in various settings and different contexts. Thus, this study sought to answer the ...

  8. Learning from Errors: Systematic Analysis of Complex Writing Errors for

    3.1 Research on Writing Processes. When writers use electronic writing tools, writing process data can be collected using keystroke-logging tools. Van Waes et al. and Sullivan and Lindgren provide a good overview of keystroke-logging technology.Originally, logging tools were developed to allow for better analysis of revising operations carried out by writers.

  9. PDF Analytical Assessment of the Common Writing Errors among Saudi

    This research explored the most common writing errors of Science and Arts students of the Foundation year program at a Saudi University that hinder them from achieving their course goals, learning outcomes and ... Thus, the study and analysis of student's errors in writing has been an interesting and productive field for a large

  10. Error Analysis

    Generally, errors committed by EFL students in learning English at any level of study. This research focuses on university students who are taking Academic Writing course in the third semester.

  11. Second Language Writing and Research: The Writing Process and Error

    Abstract. Academic writing requires conscious effort and much practice in composing, developing, and analyzing ideas. Students writing in a second language are also faced with social and cognitive challenges related to second language acquisition. L1 models of writing instruction and research on composing processes have been the theoretical ...

  12. PDF An Error Analysis on Students' Descriptive Writing

    students' writing to know the type of errors the students do most in their writing grammatically to know the causes of errors. After that, the writer describes the number of errors that the students made. The writer describes the errors in each sentence and gives an analysis. For this research, the writer analyses each

  13. PDF A Student's Guide to Data and Error Analysis

    HERMAN J. C. BERENDSEN is Emeritus Professor of Physical Chemistry the University of Groningen, the Netherlands. His research started in magnetic resonance, but focused later on molecular dynamics simulations systems of biological interest. He is one of the pioneers in this field and, over 37 000 citations, is one of the most quoted authors in ...

  14. PDF ERROR ANALYSIS ON STUDENTS' WRITING

    ERROR ANALYSIS ON STUDENTS' WRITING Marites Quibol-Catabay* ... International Journal of Advanced Research in ISSN: 2278-6236 Management and Social Sciences Impact Factor: 5.313 ... despite of offering these subjects, students still made errors in their writing. Some errors are related to sentence problems such as wrong forms of verbs ...

  15. PDF Error Analysis in the Descriptive Text Writing of Vocational High

    errors and this seemed to become their habit in writing. There are many ways to make good English writing. Errors in writing text in English are also done by some students at the previous research site conducted by Hendri et al (2018), who found that firstly, the types of morphological errors occur in the omission, addition, and misformation which

  16. PDF ERROR ANALYSIS (UNCERTAINTY ANALYSIS)

    5 USES OF UNCERTAINTY ANALYSIS (II) • Provide the only known basis for deciding whether: - Data agrees with theory - Tests from different facilities (jet engine performance) agree - Hypothesis has been appropriately assessed (resolved) - Phenomena measured are real • Provide basis for defining whether a closure check has been achieved - Is continuity satisfied (does the same ...

  17. Latest science news, discoveries and analysis

    Find breaking science news and analysis from the world's leading research journal.

  18. Gallito awarded a Summer 2024 PURA

    Congratulations to History major Hilary Gallito on being awarded a Summer 2024 PURA (Provost's Undergraduate Research Award) for her project proposal titled, "War, Women, and a Revolution: An Analysis of Classical Themes in Early American Women's Writing!" Summer PURA operates as the summer cycle for the PURA program. This program was created to assist and...

  19. PDF An Analysis of Errors in Written English Sentences: A Case Study of

    An Analysis of Errors in Written English Sentences: A Case Study of Thai EFL Students Kanyakorn ... The purposes of the present study were to examine the language errors in a writing of English major students in a ... A number of research studies, as a result, have been conducted both to find out causes of writing problems and to ...

  20. How do SSRIs interact with maternal physiology and offspring

    We are looking for an undergraduate or post-graduate volunteer laboratory intern to help complete projects related to the prenatal and gestational impacts of SSRI use. Students will have opportunities for data analysis, manuscript writing, and presenting. The Gumusoglu Lab is a welcoming, positive

  21. PDF An Error Analysis of Research Project Abstracts Written by Thai

    Errors in writing are unavoidable while students are trying to develop their writing skills. There . have been several studies on identifying writing problems or errors in students' writing. It is believed that identifying students' written tasks is an effective tool to explore the difficulties involved in learning language.

  22. Fact Sheet on FTC's Proposed Final Noncompete Rule

    The following outline provides a high-level overview of the FTC's proposed final rule:. The final rule bans new noncompetes with all workers, including senior executives after the effective date.