Internet Archive Audio

horror game video essay

  • This Just In
  • Grateful Dead
  • Old Time Radio
  • 78 RPMs and Cylinder Recordings
  • Audio Books & Poetry
  • Computers, Technology and Science
  • Music, Arts & Culture
  • News & Public Affairs
  • Spirituality & Religion
  • Radio News Archive

horror game video essay

  • Flickr Commons
  • Occupy Wall Street Flickr
  • NASA Images
  • Solar System Collection
  • Ames Research Center

horror game video essay

  • All Software
  • Old School Emulation
  • MS-DOS Games
  • Historical Software
  • Classic PC Games
  • Software Library
  • Kodi Archive and Support File
  • Vintage Software
  • CD-ROM Software
  • CD-ROM Software Library
  • Software Sites
  • Tucows Software Library
  • Shareware CD-ROMs
  • Software Capsules Compilation
  • CD-ROM Images
  • ZX Spectrum
  • DOOM Level CD

horror game video essay

  • Smithsonian Libraries
  • FEDLINK (US)
  • Lincoln Collection
  • American Libraries
  • Canadian Libraries
  • Universal Library
  • Project Gutenberg
  • Children's Library
  • Biodiversity Heritage Library
  • Books by Language
  • Additional Collections

horror game video essay

  • Prelinger Archives
  • Democracy Now!
  • Occupy Wall Street
  • TV NSA Clip Library
  • Animation & Cartoons
  • Arts & Music
  • Computers & Technology
  • Cultural & Academic Films
  • Ephemeral Films
  • Sports Videos
  • Videogame Videos
  • Youth Media

Search the history of over 866 billion web pages on the Internet.

Mobile Apps

  • Wayback Machine (iOS)
  • Wayback Machine (Android)

Browser Extensions

Archive-it subscription.

  • Explore the Collections
  • Build Collections

Save Page Now

Capture a web page as it appears now for use as a trusted citation in the future.

Please enter a valid web address

  • Donate Donate icon An illustration of a heart shape

Horror video games : essays on the fusion of fear and play

Bookreader item preview, share or embed this item, flag this item for.

  • Graphic Violence
  • Explicit Sexual Content
  • Hate Speech
  • Misinformation/Disinformation
  • Marketing/Phishing/Advertising
  • Misleading/Inaccurate/Missing Metadata

[WorldCat (this item)]

plus-circle Add Review comment Reviews

142 Previews

12 Favorites

Better World Books

DOWNLOAD OPTIONS

No suitable files to display here.

PDF access not available for this item.

IN COLLECTIONS

Uploaded by station09.cebu on March 28, 2023

SIMILAR ITEMS (based on metadata)

  • Tools and Resources
  • Customer Services
  • Communication and Culture
  • Communication and Social Change
  • Communication and Technology
  • Communication Theory
  • Critical/Cultural Studies
  • Gender (Gay, Lesbian, Bisexual and Transgender Studies)
  • Health and Risk Communication
  • Intergroup Communication
  • International/Global Communication
  • Interpersonal Communication
  • Journalism Studies
  • Language and Social Interaction
  • Mass Communication
  • Media and Communication Policy
  • Organizational Communication
  • Political Communication
  • Rhetorical Theory
  • Share This Facebook LinkedIn Twitter

Article contents

Virtual reality horror games and fear in gaming.

  • Tammy Jin-Hsuan Lin Tammy Jin-Hsuan Lin National Chengchi University
  • https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.013.1478
  • Published online: 22 November 2023

Fear is a basic human emotion important for survival and for staying alert to potential danger. In psychology, fear is defined as a discrete emotion to help humans adapt to the environment and serves as a signal for potential danger to help humans avoid or prepare for such threats. While fear is typically experienced through real-world threats as a natural response for survival, modern society also exposes us to fear through mediated content like movies and news. Interactive media, such as video games and virtual reality (VR), have emerged as new ways to experience fear because of their immersive environments. Researchers have discovered that people have similar reactions to both real-life and mediated threats. Previous studies have explored the reasons and methods behind how people experience fear through media. With advancements in technology, researchers have also examined the emotional impact of interactive media, such as video games and VR.

This article examines fear elements, fear reactions, and coping reactions in video games and VR. Results indicate that horror games are the most likely to elicit fear responses in video games, and participants often experience greater cognitive than physical reactions. In VR, research has shown that elements that make players feel realistic inside the games, termed plausibility illusion elements, are most effective in eliciting fear. Players’ reactions toward the VR horror games include active approach strategies, or a constant reminder that the VR events are not real; directly disengaging physically and mentally; and other self-help coping strategies. In addition to immediate fear during VR-horror gameplay, some players showed residual fear on the day after they finished playing the game, indicating that the Tetris effect is strong in VR-horror games. Overall, the empirical evidence in existing gaming literature show that emotional responses are greater in VR than in non-VR video games. The literature also explores the appeal of horror games, and their mechanisms are reviewed. By understanding the fear responses of audiences in video games and VR, researchers and the industry can design effective intervention and training materials.

Media-elicited fear, mediated fright, is reviewed, followed by the appeal of horror games. Game elements from the design perspective to discuss various elements in horror games that may elicit fear are also reviewed. Fear reactions, negative emotions experienced in horror games and other game-related elements such as music and soundtrack, virtual environment, game characters’ appearance, and their facial expressions, all contributed to the fear emotion among players. The discussion of unique affordances and traits of VR and its implications conclude the article.

  • virtual reality
  • horror games
  • virtual reality horror games
  • fear in gaming
  • interactivity
  • self-efficacy

Fear Elicited From Media

Fear is a negative-valenced emotion that arises when we perceive a significant and personally relevant threat in our surroundings ( Easterling & Leventhal, 1989 ; Lang, 1984 ; Witte, 1992 ). Some argue that fear is a psychological concept rather than a biological mechanism that can be scientifically identified ( Adolphs, 2013 ). However, research on the brain suggests that the amygdala, located in the medial temporal lobe, is associated with fear ( Davis, 1992 ). Fear is defined as a complex reaction involving immediate emotional responses and subsequent cognitive reactions to a perceived threatening stimulus in the environment ( Lynch & Martins, 2015 , p. 299). Verbal self-reports have been established as a sensitive measure to communicate the experience of fear, surpassing physiological data such as heart rate and skin conductance ( Rogers, 1983 ) or facial expressions ( Tourangeau & Ellsworth, 1979 ) as methods to detect fear.

Fear is not limited to real-life situations but can also be experienced in mediated contexts such as watching movies ( Cantor, 2002 ) or consuming news ( Cantor & Nathanson, 1996 ). Several factors in media content contribute to inducing suspense and fear among audiences. These factors include the presence of dangerous characters or monsters in films or victims in news stories ( Cantor & Sparks, 1984 ; Sparks & Cantor, 1986 ), the perceived likelihood of threats occurring in the real world versus a fictional context ( Cantor & Nathanson, 1996 ; Cantor & Sparks, 1984 ), the depiction of blood and injuries ( Harrison & Cantor, 1999 ), and abstract threats ( Cantor et al., 1986 ).

Numerous studies have focused on children’s developmental differences in their fear responses to these elements in films and news. Cantor (2002) found that younger children were more afraid of fear-inducing stimuli, such as the Hulk, compared to older children. As children grow older, they tend to be more frightened by realistic portrayals of events, whereas younger children are more frightened by fantasy events. Furthermore, depictions of abstract threats, such as a nuclear war, which young children may not fully understand, elicit stronger fear reactions among older children.

In addition to children, adults also experience fear in response to mediated content. Johnson (1980) discovered that adults considered “depicted brutality” in films as the most terrifying element that induces fear and distressing emotions. Furthermore, filmed depictions of bodily injuries lead to higher levels of physiological arousal and self-reported anxiety among adults ( Averill et al., 1972 ; Boyle, 1984 ).

Interactive media such as video games and VR are different from other forms of media, such as news clips and movies, because they allow audiences to engage with the content in a more active way—known as interactivity. This means that audiences have more control over the narrative and can make decisions continuously while playing these games and in VR environments. As a result, players can experience greater immersion and presence within the game world and VR environments, which may also lead to greater fear in these virtual environments with horror content ( Hemenover & Bowman, 2018 ). In contrast to watching a news story depicting horrific events through the third-person view, horror games allow players to choose what and how to react when facing mediated threats, and horror games in VR further put players as the protagonists because all threats are directed toward them as if they are actually happening to them. The unique affordances of the technology of digital games and VR have drawn academic attention to both players’ fear perpetuated in the game and the various elements in designing these games.

Players’ Appeal to Horror Games

Regarding video games ( Kjeldgaard-Christiansen & Clasen, 2019 ), researchers argue that a combination of evolutionary theory and game studies can explain why players are drawn to horror video games ( Lynch, 2018 ). Horror games allow players to simulate fear scenarios of uncertainty and danger, challenging them to assess and negotiate potential threats. While horror games instill negative emotions, they also offer stimulating challenges that can expand players’ emotional and behavioral repertoire, leading to a sense of mastery and explaining the genre’s paradoxical appeal. The authors illustrate their evolutionary approach through an analysis of Playdead’s puzzle-horror game Limbo.

In addition to the evolutionary mechanism, the excitation transfer theory and model of suspense were employed to examine why players enjoyed horror games. Researchers ( Lin et al., 2018 ) investigated the underlying mechanism of a paradoxical issue: VR-horror game players experienced fear in the game but provided enjoyable appraisals afterward. This study employed the excitation transfer theory (i.e., model of suspense; Zillmann, 1980 ) as a basis and proposed a three-factor model.

Zillmann’s model of suspense shows that the resolution of threats and arousal are two important moderators in the association between fear experienced in movies and the enjoyment outcome after watching the movie. According to this model, audiences become aroused due to suspenseful content and experience negative emotions if a character they like is threatened with victimization. However, if the character has a satisfying ending or successfully overcomes the threats (by resolving them), audiences experience positive emotions, and the accumulated arousal intensifies the enjoyment. The model highlights the role of a satisfying resolution of previously encountered threats as a key factor in enjoyment. If the resolution is unsatisfying or unresolved, the arousal carries over to exacerbate disappointment or dysphoria.

This model being discussed focuses on fear, threat resolution, and arousal as key factors in understanding the enjoyment of frightening media. However, previous studies have not always taken arousal into account when examining the relationship between fear and enjoyment. Arousal is crucial for understanding how fear and threat resolution impact enjoyment because it plays a moderating role in this process. Additionally, while threat resolution has traditionally been viewed as important for explaining the appeal of horror content, research has shown that audiences can still enjoy horror even without resolution because the absence of a happy ending does not necessarily mean that viewers experience cognitive dysphoria.

Revising the excitation transfer theory, Lin et al. (2018) proposed a new three-factor model to explain why players enjoyed the horror game even when they experienced great fear in VR-horror games. Lin et al. (2018) argued that instead of the resolution of threat, horror self-efficacy might be a more precise construct to alter players’ negative emotions to enjoyment after the gameplay. In the case of “mediated” fright, self-efficacy is crucial to resolve the threat. Self-efficacy is a belief in one’s capacity to accomplish tasks necessary to achieve specific goals. For horror media, a similar concept is “mastering horror,” which refers to the ability to withstand horror content. Horror self-efficacy is the belief in one’s capacity to confront and endure the challenges presented by media. In video games, successfully coping with suspense and confronting immediate threats can be considered the “resolution” of the challenges posed. If viewers can successfully endure horror content, they experience “cognitive euphoria,” and their arousal level transfers the previous intensity of suspense into enjoyment when the game is over, regardless of whether they experience a satisfying conclusion to the threat.

Revising the model of suspense ( Zillmann, 1980 ), Lin et al. (2018) proposed a three-way interaction, including fear, horror self-efficacy, and arousal that affects enjoyment. Specifically, fear and horror self-efficacy have a significant interaction effect on enjoyment among players who experience high arousal, but not among those who experience low arousal. Among high-arousal players, those who have lower horror self-efficacy experience less enjoyment when they also have high levels of fear. However, horror self-efficacy does not affect enjoyment for players who experience low levels of fear.

In this lab study ( Lin et al., 2018 ), the primary factor that influences how individuals perceive negative feelings of fear while experiencing mediated horror content is their level of horror self-efficacy. Horror self-efficacy refers to an individual’s belief in their ability to successfully endure and confront frightening situations. The study showed that high levels of horror self-efficacy can transform a negative fear experience into a positive appraisal, leading to enjoyment of the horror content and a desire to play the game again in the future. The study also found that measurements of physiological arousal through skin conductance indicated increased emotional intensity, which further enhanced the experience of enjoyment.

Horror Video Games and Fear: Stimulus Characteristics

Horror game design: emotion and agency.

Scholars explored the design of horror game environment ( Habel & Kooyman, 2014 ; Vachiratamporn et al., 2013 ) that elicit fear from players. Scary events, fear elements, and interactivity are dimensions scholars explored in this area ( Habel & Kooyman, 2014 ; Lin, 2017 ; Lynch & Martins, 2015 ). Survival horror games have received attention in the literature because the genre is designed to induce players’ intense emotions such as fear. Affective gaming, incorporating a player’s emotional state to manipulate gameplay, has become a trend in early 2010s. Vachiratamporn et al. (2013) focused on players’ affective states prior to and after a scary event in a survival horror game. In this study, the researchers used an affect annotation tool to gather self-reported player affective states before (i.e., neutral, anxiety, suspense) and after (i.e., low-fear, mid-fear, high-fear) a scary event in a survival horror game. They also collected physiological data, such as brainwave signals, heart rate, and keyboard and mouse activity, to determine the potential for automatically detecting horror-related affect. The results showed that players were more likely to experience fear when they were in a suspense state compared to when they were in a neutral state, and heart rate data was the most effective in classifying player affect, which achieved up to 90% overall accuracy. This suggests that player affect can be used to adapt a scary event in survival horror games to evoke more fear from players.

Habel and Kooyman (2014) proposed a construct of “agency mechanics” as an innovative design element of survival horror video games. The authors argue that agency, or the player’s ability to make meaningful choices, is a key element in creating an immersive and engaging gameplay experience in the survival horror genre. They provide an overview of the history and evolution of survival horror games, as well as a discussion of the different types of agency mechanics that can be used in game design. These include player-controlled actions, environmental interactivity, and narrative choice. The authors also examine the psychological effects of agency on the player and how it contributes to the horror experience. Overall, the article provides insights into the importance of agency mechanics in the design of survival horror video games and offers practical guidance for game developers looking to create effective and engaging gameplay experiences.

Indeed, the agency mechanics reflect the game design to offer more interactivity for players to feel that they are the ones making all the decisions and choices in the game. Similar game design elements in the exercise context illustrated in Peng et al. (2012) also correspond to the agency. Guided by self-determination theory ( Ryan & Deci, 2006 ), agency is closely linked to the construct of autonomy. The need for autonomy, defined as the need to feel in control and make decisions, is an intrinsic need for human well-being. Narration choices, which is being able to select the character growth or the environment interactivity, are elements in game design to increase satisfaction of autonomy, which further leads to increased game enjoyment.

However, in horror games, games often “revel in the denial of agency provided to players, and it is precisely the abandonment of control that produces the pleasure of horror gaming’s “bounded experience of “fear”” ( Habel & Kooyman, 2014 , p. 11; Pinedo, 1997 , p. 38). Therefore, in order to produce frightening emotions, depriving their autonomy in the “survival context” is a core design element. Habel and Kooyman (2014) also raised the question of the dichotomy of game categories of “survival” (i.e., players being controlled by the game world) and “action” (i.e., players having control and power over the environment). These mechanics not only intensify the gaming experience but also heighten the survival horror elements, including action games. A good example of this is the Dead Space franchise, a very successful survival horror game that has sold more than 2 million copies, and the game effectively uses agency mechanics to elicit horror responses from players. For example, in the game series, the player is allowed conventional movement within a third-person perspective, which involves walking and running. One notable feature is the absence of controllable vehicles, preventing faster locomotion through the game world, and notably, there are no weaponized vehicles that would enable the player to deliver massive firepower. As a result, the basic opportunities for movement are limited compared to, for example, a first-person military shooter. Therefore, game design, particularly in horror games, would benefit greatly from more explicit attention to agency mechanics, which could improve player experience and enhance the overall quality of game design ( Habel & Kooyman, 2014 ). The authors also called upon the designers to break free from the dilemma of choosing between creating games for action or survival by focusing on player interactivity. This indicated that by adding agency mechanics of action games in survival games can enhance the fear induction process. For example, survival games limit one’s agency but by adding movements and agency in limited realms from the action games to survival scenes, a designer can create dynamic environments inducing fear through these two mechanisms. This approach is not limited by the popularity of action games or the niche audience of survival horror games, and allows for a more nuanced design focus.

This agency mechanic may be also related to an underlying mechanism of gameplay: character identification ( Klimmt et al., 2009 ). Character identification in video games is defined as the degree to which a player feels like the character in the game as well as a change of self conpcet mering the player’s self and the character. Game design elements such as interactivity contribute to character identification, and further leads to game enjoyment ( Hefner et al., 2007 ). Other literature ( Lin, 2013b ) also showed that interactivity (playing a game as high interactivity vs. watching a gameplay as low interactivity) led to greater character identification, which leads to greater player emotions and cognition to the game context. These mechanics are closely linked to the agency mechanic illustrated by Habel and Kooyman (2014) .

Stimulus Characteristics in Resident Evil and Negative Emotions

In addition to fear induction through game design, negative emotions in horror games have received attention in the literature. Lin et al. (2017) explored negative emotions including fear, anger, worry, disgust, and frustration in the violent horror game Resident Evil . They focus on the perspective of gamers and aims to examine and construct a detailed structure of their emotions. This structure includes the network of events triggered by the game and the personal beliefs and feelings of negative emotions experienced by the gamer. The study also investigates the correlation and level of interinfluence between the various factors. The results of the study indicate that the hierarchical value map, a visual representation of emotional network structure of gamers using different layers to show connected clusters of gamers’ emotions, provides a comprehensive understanding of the gamer emotional network structure. The key game-activating events, such as enemies, stage missions, boss-level monsters, horror atmosphere, and third-person view, lead to the formation of personal beliefs of character death, high difficulty, negative emotions, fear, being frightened, enemy ambush, and tough enemies, ultimately leading to negative emotions such as fear, worry, anger, disgust, and frustration. Additionally, the study found that the clusters of fear and anger had the greatest impact on gamers, and there were varying degrees of correlation and influence between the different factors.

Building on this work, Lin et al. (2017) identified the specific activating events and beliefs that caused negative emotions in players of Resident Evil . To achieve this, the study isolated the negative emotions and grouped them into emotional clusters, which included fear, worry, anger, disgust, and frustration. By doing this, the study provided a clear understanding of how the different variables interacted with each other to produce the negative emotions experienced by the players.

Among the emotion clusters, the worry cluster was associated with negative emotions caused by specific game plot and game-activating events such as stage missions in Resident Evil . Previous research has shown that games with high difficulty levels or time constraints can induce anxiety or worry in players ( Liu et al., 2009 ). In the case of Resident Evil , the study participants perceived the stage missions to be difficult, leading to the belief in high difficulty, which ultimately resulted in the negative emotion of worry when they were unable to overcome the challenges. Furthermore, participants noted that the game featured a continuous story plot with recurring themes of viral infection and killing, which caused negative emotions such as paranoia and made them worried about their own mental well-being.

The disgust cluster in Resident Evil players was associated with negative emotions caused by the third-person camera view and team cooperation. The constantly rotating camera view in the game made some players feel sick or dizzy, leading to a feeling of disgust. Additionally, poor support or wasteful actions from teammates in the game could also cause players to feel disgusted.

The fear cluster was found to be the most predominant negative emotion experienced by players in Resident Evil games. Previous studies have suggested that these games can create intense fear for players ( Zagalo et al., 2005 ). Zagalo et al. (2005) found that the emotion of fear had three components in Resident Evil . The plot of the game, which includes realistic and plausible storylines, could lead to depression or nightmares, triggering negative emotions and ultimately resulting in fear. The presence of zombies with weapons, concealed in the game’s horror atmosphere and soundtracks, which are designed to create tension and stress, could also cause players to feel frightened and trigger fear. Additionally, the realistic and creepy images in the game, including dimly lit tunnels, dilapidated houses, and vast forests, could stimulate players and lead to feelings of fear.

Stimulus Characteristics: Interactivity, Sounds, and Visual

The study regarding Resident Evil (Capcom) , provided an in-depth examination into the elements causing fear in a single game. Other literature explored the general elements in different games, in which the survival horror game genre was reported the most. Among this literature, interactivity, scary soundtracks in horror games, visual presentation, and environment were key elements causing fear in various horror games. Regarding these horror games, a study conducted by Lynch and Martins (2015) surveyed 269 university students and discovered that over half (53%) of the sample reported experiencing fear while playing games. Among 53 game titles, the participants listed the games that made them frightened, and the survival horror genre, including the Resident Evil and Silent Hill series, was at the top of the list and had a significant proportion of all games. In addition, first-person shooters, such as the Call of Duty and Halo series, also led to experiences of fear.

What elements caused players to fear in these horror games? Lynch and Martins (2015) asked participants to identify the elements that caused fear in video games. The participants ranked interactivity as the top element that caused fear, as they played an active role in the game’s narrative and content. Other elements that caused fear included game mechanisms like surprise and game environment elements like audio, darkness, and zombies. Participants reported that interactivity was a major factor in their fear responses and that they differentiated between two types of realism: graphic realism and manifest realism. Graphic realism, which refers to the immersive elements’ vivid sensory detail, was more often described as contributing to fright responses than manifest realism, which relates to the plausibility of game events in the real world. Additionally, participants described their responses to the types of fear-inducing elements commonly identified in research on traditional media, including abstract threats like biological and chemical hazards, violence, and death.

Graja et al. (2020) examined the visuals and sounds in horror games and explored how those elements cause arousal and tension. They hypothesize that modifying game elements such as visuals and sounds can enhance the gaming experience. The researchers conducted an experiment to investigate the players’ emotional reactions toward context alteration through an adapted version of the game P.T. (by Konami, 2014 ), which altered contexts including light effects, sounds, and in-game events. The experiment measured the participants’ physiological data through the galvanic skin response and their perceived emotion. The study found that the order in which the effects were presented had a significant impact on the emotional responses of the players. Furthermore, the study suggests that the psychological impact of the game can be enhanced not only by the visual horror but also by the gradual build-up of sounds.

These two elements of visual and sound in horror games were examined in-depth by other scholars. Klimmt et al. (2019) focused on examining the role of the soundtrack in two games for players’ enjoyment and horror emotions. In their second study focusing on a horror game, 59 young males played an episode of Alien: Isolation with or without the soundtrack music and reported their horror experience, spatial presence, and character identification. Results indicated that the presence of shocking soundtrack music has an effect on players’ horror experience. Presence or identification are not the underlying mechanisms of the effects of soundtrack on players’ horror experience.

In addition to emotions, arousal was also examined in horror games. In another study, Ballard et al. (2021) investigated the effects of frightening sound effects in a survival horror video game on cardiovascular and affective arousal and whether these factors were linked to a bias toward shooting people of color (POCs) first in a shooter task. They predicted that participants who played the horror game with scary sound effects would experience higher levels of cardiovascular arousal and fear compared to those who played without sound effects. Additionally, it was expected that an increase in cardiovascular arousal would result in a higher likelihood of participants shooting a POC first during the shooter task. The results supported the hypothesis that playing the survival horror game with scary sound effects leads to higher cardiovascular arousal. However, the study did not find any correlation between first-shot bias and cardiovascular arousal. Instead, the study found that first-shot bias was positively correlated with enjoyment of the shooter game. An exploratory analysis indicated that participants who played the game with scary sound effects were slightly more likely to shoot a POC first in the shooter task, but only when enjoyment of the game was included as a covariate. Although this effect was small, it indicated that the first-shot bias occurred when the scary sound effects were present after controlling one’s enjoyment of the game. These evidences indicated that sound element is an important factor contributing players’ fear emotion, through self-recalled questionnaire ( Lynch & Martins, 2015 ), self-reported emotion ( Klimmt et al., 2019 ), and through physiological signals ( Ballard et al., 2021 ).

Regarding virtual environment, McCall et al. (2022) explored types of virtual environments that elicit ambiguous threats. Previous studies have shown that feeling uncertain about when or where a threat may occur can cause anxiety. In this study, McCall et al. (2022) wanted to investigate other aspects of ambiguous threats by creating a virtual environment that did not contain any concrete threats but still induced a feeling of danger. By incorporating psychological research and principles of emotional game design, the authors developed a virtual world and tested its effects in three studies. Participants reported feeling uneasy and uncertain in the virtual environment, and their subjective and physiological responses were affected by the presentation of ambiguous cues that could be perceived as threatening. The results also suggest that this experience had an impact on participants’ memory of the virtual world and its storyline. Overall, this study shows that immersive virtual worlds can effectively create a complex experience of ambiguous threat that has both subjective and cognitive effects.

Stimulus Characteristics: Character’s Appearance and Facial Expressions

Research suggests that when playing video games, fear is more likely to be experienced from a first-person perspective, where the player interacts with the game environment directly. A study by Hofer et al. (2017) found that when an avatar expressed fear, it reduced the player’s sense of embodiment of the avatar and decreased their fear reactions. This suggests that the emotions expressed by an avatar may interfere with the player’s identification with the avatar and alter their self-concept. Therefore, first-person perspectives are likely to elicit greater fear in players than third-person perspectives in video games because the player cannot see the facial expressions of their own avatars. The same study also found that this effect only occurred during active gameplay and not while watching recorded gameplay. This suggests that the mechanisms by which video games induce fear may differ depending on how the audience engages with the content: playing versus watching.

The objective of this study ( Hofer et al., 2017 ) was to examine how an avatar’s emotional expressions affect players’ fear reactions during horror gameplay. In Study 1, the results showed that the emotional expressions of an avatar reduced players’ fear reactions. This effect was mediated by the level of embodiment that players experienced with their avatars. Specifically, avatar emotional expressions reduced the level of avatar embodiment, which, in turn, positively predicted players’ fear reactions. In Study 2, the findings of Study 1 were replicated. Additionally, it was discovered that the effects observed in Study 1 only occurred during interactive gameplay and not when players watched prerecorded footage of the game. This suggests that the effects of an avatar’s emotional expressions on players’ fear reactions are moderated by interactivity, through avatar embodiment. A moderated mediation model was supported by these results.

In another study, Tinwell et al. (2010) examined the relationship between the perceived strangeness of virtual characters and the perception of human likeness based on attributes of motion and sound among 100 participants. Thirteen video clips of virtual characters and one of a human were rated. The results showed that the uncanny phenomenon and the perceived fright of a character were exaggerated by attributes of motion and sound. The perceived strangeness of a character had strong correlations with how humanlike the voice sounded, how humanlike the facial expression appeared, and how synchronized the character’s sound was with lip movement. The least synchronized characters were perceived to be the most frightening. These findings provide an initial set of hypotheses for character facial rendering and vocalization in survival horror games that game designers can use to increase the fear factor. These empirical data showed that game characters’ appearance and their facial expressions are factors affecting players’ fear.

Technological Affordance: Interactivity

Interactivity was also explored as a dimension to examine audiences’ emotions toward a horror-themed video game. Interactivity in existing literature is often operationalized as playing (high interactivity) versus watching (low interactivity) a video game ( Lin, 2013a , 2013b ). Related literature has shown that greater level of interactivity in digital games leads to greater emotional responses such as aggression. In horror games, Madsen (2016) employed this play versus watch a video game to explore the direct or authentic versus indirect or passive fear. In this study, Madsen explored and compared the impact of interactivity or agency on the physiological responses of participants as a measure of direct fear while playing or watching a horror-themed video game. Madsen (2016) hypothesized that players, who have greater agency, would exhibit a higher physiological response, indicating a greater fear reaction. Change scores by subtracting baseline mean values from exposure mean values for each participant were calculated in measures of electrodermal activity (EDA), respiratory rate (RR), and heart rate (HR). Self-reported fear data from each participant were also collected. The results showed that players had significantly greater increases in EDA, RR, and HR change scores compared to watchers. However, there was no significant difference in self-reported fear between players and watchers. The researchers also reported change score t-tests for specific events that occur in the video game. Overall, the results suggest that agency had the effect of inducing a greater fear response and may be useful for researchers looking to ethically induce direct fear. This study also corresponded to other literature regarding interactivity on emotions.

Players’ Fright Reactions in Horror Games

In a survey conducted by Lynch and Martins (2015) asking university students to recall their horror game playing experiences, they reported experiencing greater cognitive than physical reactions to playing video games that induce fright. More specifically, over half of players (55%) were acutely aware of strange sounds, while more than one-third (33.6%) were similarly aware of strange shadows. Furthermore, players often experienced cognitive reactions, such as not being able to stop thinking about the game (28%), feeling afraid to be alone at home (19.6%), and being worried about the game (32.9%). Only a small number of university students reported physical reactions, such as nightmares (12.6%), headaches (2.8%), stomachaches (5.6%), or shaking (9.8%). Despite these negative reactions, nearly half of the participants (44.1%) enjoyed the experience of feeling scared.

Regarding potential individual differences, the survey results ( Lynch & Martins, 2015 ) showed that no gender differences were found regarding fear responses. However, male players enjoyed playing these games more than female players. The study also found that individuals with higher levels of sensation seeking were less likely to be scared by horror games. Meanwhile, trait empathy was found to only have a negative correlation with the enjoyment of horror games, but not with fear frequency or playing frequency.

The above sections focus on existing literature exploring elements in video games causing fear among players and their fright responses. Before moving on to the literature on VR horror games, the characteristics in VR are first reviewed for basic understanding of VR.

Technological Characteristics and Core Elements in VR

The distinguishing features of VR that set it apart from other media and video games are its head-mounted device, which presents two separate images to create a stereo virtual environment that users can explore. Additionally, users wear goggles that completely cover their field of vision, immersing them fully in the simulated environment. A complete VR system also includes an auditory and haptic controlling device with a tracking system, which allows users to identify the direction of events and interact with the environment through various actions, such as grabbing objects or firing guns. Advanced haptic devices provide feedback, enabling users to sense vibrations as real feedback.

The second feature that distinguishes VR from other media is its ability to offer room-scale experiences through high-end VR engines such as HTC Vive and Oculus Rift. This feature allows users to move around freely in a designated space and interact with the virtual environment through real-world actions, inducing the response-as-if-real phenomenon ( Slater, 2009 ). This means that users respond with genuine emotions and reactions to the VR environment, similar to their reactions in the real world. For instance, in a VR simulation, users may feel afraid to walk outside on a narrow plank after taking an elevator to the top of a building. Similarly, in a VR game, users may scream, step back, and fire their guns when they encounter zombies attacking them from behind ( Slater, 2009 ; VR simulation-“Plank”). These reactions are similar to real responses in the real world.

Slater (2009) identified three fundamental mechanisms in VR: place illusion (PI), plausibility illusion (PSI), and body ownership. PI is defined as “the strong illusion of being in a place in spite of the sure knowledge that you are not there” ( Slater, 2009 , p. 3553). It refers to the convincing illusion of being in a specific location despite knowing that one is not physically present there. Whereas PI focuses on the environment, PSI emphasizes the perception that the depicted events seem real. On the other hand, PSI pertains to the perception that the events being depicted in VR seem real even though one is aware that they are not. It is defined as “the illusion that what is happening is real even though you know that it is not real” ( Slater, 2009 , p. 3561). For instance, in a VR-horror game, one may realize that zombies do not exist in the real world, but still feel as if they are genuinely battling with them. Finally, body ownership is defined as “some degree of ownership over this virtual body—it comes to really seem to be your body (even though you know it cannot be)” ( Slater, 2009 , p. 3554). It refers to the feeling of possessing a virtual body in the VR environment, even though one knows that it is not real. While some VR experiences do not provide users with a virtual body, certain games and social VR applications such as Rec Room and Horizon World allow users to customize their avatar, including their virtual body and facial features.

Immersion in VR

To examine the key affordance of immersion in VR-horror games, researchers examine whether the immersion, operationalized as horror games in VR (high immersion) and horror video games (nonimmersive), influence players’ emotions and reactions. Some studies ( Pallavicini et al., 2019 ) compared emotional responses in VR games and nonimmersive video games and found that both types of games elicit fear responses. Those who played VR games indicated greater enjoyment and presence than regular digital games. Participants also displayed more intense emotional responses both in a self-reported questionnaire and a physiological index (heart rate and skin conductance). This finding corresponds to other studies ( Hemenover & Bowman, 2018 ; Lin, 2017 ), which find that the immersion in VR games elicits a greater degree of emotions than non-VR games.

In addition to emotions, immersion also affects players’ physiological response. Lemmens et al. (2022) investigated whether VR games elicited a stronger sense of presence and affected players’ emotional and physiological responses compared to playing on a traditional TV screen. Two experiments were conducted with a total of 128 students, in which the effects of playing a survival horror game or a first-person shooter (FPS) on a TV or in VR were compared. The results indicated that playing games in VR led to a stronger sense of presence, lower heart rate variability, and stronger subjective feelings of fear. Moreover, the feeling of presence mediated the effects of VR on fear. The impact of playing an FPS in VR on hostility was inconsistent, and VR gaming was not found to be more enjoyable than playing on TV. Regardless of the game or display medium, hostility levels increased significantly after playing. This study highlights that commercial VR games have the potential to influence players’ feelings of presence, emotional, and physiological states.

Due to these different findings, future research should continue exploring whether technological affordances such as immersion lead to different affective and physiological outcomes comparing VR and non-VR games. From this section, horror self-efficacy is an important mechanism to explain why players enjoy the horror games. This horror self-efficacy can be affected by the design of horror games by various elements which can effectively elicit fear.

Audience Response: Fear in VRHorror Games

Since the commercialization of VR in 2016 , scholars have studied players’ fear emotions and their coping strategies when playing VR-horror games. Lin (2017) conducted a study to investigate the immediate and next-day fear responses of university students to VR-horror games. The participants were asked to play a VR-horror game for four minutes in a laboratory, but 17 participants quit the game due to extreme fear after watching a video trailer and a VR trial. The study found that the fear responses were caused by two elements: PI elements, such as darkness, environmental sounds, and the sound of zombie steps, and PSI elements, such as when zombies were close to the participants and when they attacked the participants. The participants reported being more afraid of the PSI elements than the PI elements.

In another study ( Bender & Sung, 2021 ), the authors employed facial electromyography and skin conductance response to explore players’ affective responses such as fear and arousal through the physiological signals. They explored whether different levels of immersion in different gameplay modes may elicit greater affective responses and whether these heightened affective responses would significantly correlate with players’ enjoyment at the precise point of the affective response. Using a popular VR zombie game, Arizona Sunshine (from Vertigo Games), three specific scenarios were chosen as the experimental stimuli in this study: (a) an onboarding scenario (i.e., following the game plot in the beginning), (b) a dark abandoned mine (i.e., dark environment), and (c) a survival-based horde mode (i.e., when a herd of zombies swarm into the player, overrun, and kill). Each mode requires different gameplay from the players and the virtual environments are different in terms of their visual and audio aesthetic. The results showed that all three modes elicited significant heightened fear, arousal, and joy than the baseline.

This study ( Bender & Sung, 2021 ) also found that among all the game modes, the average amplitude of fear was significantly higher in Horde Mode (i.e., survival mode with unlimited zombies coming toward the player) compared to Onboarding Mode (i.e., the usual game plot mode), but there was no significant difference between Abandoned Mine Mode (i.e., dark environment) and Onboarding Mode. Moreover, both Horde Mode and Abandoned Mine Mode induced significantly higher levels of arousal compared to Onboarding Mode. Participants experienced a higher average amplitude of joy in Horde Mode than in Onboarding Mode and Abandoned Mine Mode, but there was no significant difference in the average amplitude of joy between Abandoned Mine Mode and Onboarding Mode. Among all events in the game, participants reported significantly higher fear when entering the dark room in the Abandoned Mine Mode. This event evoked approximately seven times higher levels of arousal than all other events in the game. Similarly, for joy, participants experienced significantly higher levels of joy when entering the dark room in the Abandoned Mine Mode than in all other events except for “over-run by zombie” and “over-run and die” in Horde Mode.

This study ( Bender & Sung, 2021 ) compared the immersion level in the same game, and showed that both “immersive gameplay modes” elicited significantly higher fear and arousal than the “less immersive gameplay mode.” This indicated that immersion is an important antecedent of users’ emotions in VR games. They also showed that fear and arousal were correlated significantly with users’ enjoyment, corresponding with results from Lin et al. (2018) .

In terms of fear and arousal, Voigt-Antons et al. (2021) employed another approach to study horror games. They investigated how the level of interactivity affects the sense of presence in natural environments and its impact on arousal and valence. Participants were first induced with fear using a VR-horror game, and then played a VR-nature game with no, limited, or extensive interaction to see how they recovered. The horror game successfully induced high arousal and low valence, as confirmed by a manipulation check. Scores on the Igroup Presence Questionnaire showed that more interaction with the virtual environment increased spatial presence. The experience of nature also had a beneficial effect. Scores on the Self-Assessment Manikin questionnaire for valence showed a significant increase in positive emotions in the conditions with extensive and limited interaction compared to the control group. The VR nature experience significantly reduced arousal and increased positive valence compared to the posthorror game ratings. Physiological responses supported these findings. The study suggests that health-related VR applications can improve mood levels by implementing plenty of interactions and increasing spatial presence, or leaving out any interactions altogether.

Audience Response: Players’ Coping Reaction in Horror Games

Regarding how players cope in VR-horror games, according to Lin’s (2017) study, four coping strategies were identified among players of VR-horror games. The first category is the approach strategy, which involves actively monitoring the environment and approaching threats. This strategy was more commonly used by male participants and those with high levels of sensation seeking, as well as players who were familiar with the FPS genre and enjoyed playing horror games. Female participants and those with low levels of sensation seeking were less likely to employ this strategy.

The second coping strategy is the disengagement avoidance strategy, which involves distracting oneself or closing one’s eyes. This strategy is more commonly used by females and those who are afraid of PSI elements. As expected, a preference for horror games negatively predicts the use of this strategy. In other words, those who prefer horror games would less likely adopted the disengagement avoidance strategy. Rather, they adopted the approach strategy.

The third strategy is the self-help coping strategy, which involves verbal communication such as talking to oneself, swearing, or screaming. This strategy is more commonly used by females, those who are sensation seeking and neurotic. However, familiarity with FPS games predicts less use of this strategy.

The fourth and final strategy is the cognitive avoidance strategy, which involves telling oneself that “this isn’t real.” This strategy is more commonly used by females and those who are experiencing VR content for the first time. Among these strategies, the approach strategy was used most frequently, followed by cognitive avoidance, physical and mental disengagement, and self-help coping strategies.

Regarding the overall immediate fear while playing the VR-horror game, Lin’s (2017) lab study showed that female players and individuals who have lower levels of sensation seeking, higher levels of neuroticism, low familiarity with FPS games, and a low preference for horror games experience greater fear while playing VR-horror games. The study also examined the participants’ fear reactions overnight and the following day. One participant reported having a dream related to the game, and three had nightmares. Some participants experienced the Tetris effect, where they feared being attacked from behind (just as in the game) and were afraid of darkness.

Horror games have become the significant and important game genre in the entertainment industry. VR Horror games can even be adopted as training content because the immersion allows players to feel as if they are the ones trying to survive and facing all the challenges. Horror games provide a unique platform to explore the human psyche, particularly how individuals respond to fear and anxiety. Understanding how players react to frightening situations in a safe and controlled virtual environment can offer valuable insights into human emotions and behavior. Moreover, understanding how individuals react to fear while playing video games and experiencing VR can help researchers and the industry create successful intervention and training materials. Studying horror games offers a multidisciplinary perspective, ranging from cultural and societal aspects to psychological and design considerations. By delving into this genre, researchers can gain a deeper understanding of human behavior, media effects, and interactive storytelling, benefiting various fields and industries. Fear elements, fright reactions, and coping reactions, as well as game design elements are reviewed in video games and in VR. VR has been widely used to elicit phobias in a safe virtual environment and as a means to address various phobias, including social phobias and arachnophobia. VR also provides immersive environment to train players’ coping reactions when facing threats. Developing programs through video games and VR that encourage the development of coping strategies for fear responses is crucial in present and future research efforts.

Acknowledgments

This work is supported under the grant #MOST 109-2423-H-004 -004 -SS4 from the Ministry of Science and Technology.

Primary Sources

  • Ballard, M. E. , Spencer, M. , Holtkamp, M. K. , Sakrison, E. , & Kindle, T. (2021). Horror videogame sound effects, cardiovascular arousal, and ethnic bias in the decision to shoot . The American Journal of Psychology , 134 (2), 175–186.
  • Bender, S. M. , & Sung, B. (2021). Fright, attention, and joy while killing zombies in virtual reality: A psychophysiological analysis of VR user experience . Psychology & Marketing , 38 (6), 937–947.
  • Graja, S. , Lopes, P. , & Chanel, G. (2020). Impact of visual and sound orchestration on physiological arousal and tension in a horror game . IEEE Transactions on Games , 13 (3), 287–299.
  • Habel, C. , & Kooyman, B. (2014). Agency mechanics: Gameplay design in survival horror video games . Digital Creativity , 25 (1), 44940.
  • Hemenover, S. H. , & Bowman, N. D. (2018). Video games, emotion, and emotion regulation: Expanding the scope. Annals of the International Communication Association , 42 (2), 125–143.
  • Hofer, M. , Hüsser, A. , & Prabhu, S. (2017). The effect of an avatar’s emotional expressions on players’ fear reactions: The mediating role of embodiment. Computers in Human Behavior , 75 , 883–890.
  • Kjeldgaard-Christiansen, J. , & Clasen, M. (2019). Threat simulation in virtual limbo: An evolutionary approach to horror video games . Journal of Gaming & Virtual Worlds , 11 (2), 119–138.
  • Klimmt, C. , Possler, D. , May, N. , Auge, H. , Wanjek, L. , & Wolf, A.-L. (2019). Effects of soundtrack music on the video game experience . Media Psychology , 22 (5), 689–713.
  • Lemmens, J. S. , Simon, M. , & Sumter, S. R. (2022). Fear and loathing in VR: The emotional and physiological effects of immersive games . Virtual Reality , 26 , 223–234.
  • Lin, H.-W. , Luarn, P. , & Lin, Y.-L. (2017). Hierarchical relationship of negative emotion perception from violent video games . Science, Technology and Society , 22 (2), 236–258.
  • Lin, J. H. T. (2017). Fear in virtual reality (VR): Fear elements, coping reactions, immediate and next-day fright responses toward a survival horror zombie virtual reality game. Computers in Human Behavior , 72 , 350–361.
  • Lin, J. H. T. , Wu, D. Y. , & Tao, C. C. (2018). So scary, yet so fun: The role of self-efficacy in enjoyment of a virtual reality horror game. New Media & Society , 20 (9), 3223–3242.
  • Lynch, T. (2018). Evolutionary formidability mechanisms as moderators of fear experience. In J. Breuer , D. Pietschmann , B. Liebold , & B. P. Lange (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology and digital games: Digital hunter-gatherers . Routledge.
  • Lynch, T. , & Martins, N. (2015). Nothing to fear? An analysis of college students’ fear experiences with video games. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media , 59 (2), 298–317.
  • Madsen, K. E. (2016). The differential effects of agency on fear induction using a horror-themed video game . Computers in Human Behavior , 56 , 142–146.
  • McCall, C. , Schofield, G. , Halgarth, D. , Blyth, G. , Laycock, A. , & Palombo, D. J. (2022). The underwood project: A virtual environment for eliciting ambiguous threat. Behavior Research Methods , 1–16.
  • Slater, M. (2009). Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 364 (1535), 3549–3557.
  • Tinwell, A. , Grimshaw, M. , & Williams, A. (2010). Uncanny behaviour in survival horror games . Journal of Gaming & Virtual Worlds , 2 (1), 3–25.
  • Vachiratamporn, V. , Legaspi, R. , Moriyama, K. , & Numao, M. (2013). Towards the design of affective survival horror games: An investigation on player affect . In E. Guerrero (Ed.), 2013 Humaine association conference on affective computing and intelligent interaction (pp. 576–581). IEEE.
  • Voigt-Antons, J.-N. , Spang, R. , Kojić, T. , Meier, L. , Vergari, M. , & Möller, S. (2021). Don’t worry be happy—Using virtual environments to induce emotional states measured by subjective scales and heart rate parameters . In L. O’Conner (Ed.), 2021 IEEE virtual reality and 3D user interfaces (VR) (pp. 679–686). IEEE.
  • Zagalo, N. , Torres, A. , & Branco, V. (2005, November 30–December 2). Emotional spectrum developed by virtual storytelling. In Virtual storytelling: Using virtual reality technologies for storytelling: Third international conference, ICVS 2005 (pp. 105–114). Springer.

Further Reading

  • Hefner, D. , Klimmt, C. , & Vorderer, P. (2007, September 15–17). Identification with the player character as determinant of video game enjoyment. In Entertainment computing–ICEC 2007: 6th international conference (pp. 39–48). Springer.
  • Ryan, R. M. , Rigby, C. S. , & Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motivation and Emotion , 30 , 344–360.
  • Vachiratamporn, V. , Legaspi, R. , Moriyama, K. , Fukui, K. I. , & Numao, M. (2015). An analysis of player affect transitions in survival horror games. Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces , 9 , 43–54.
  • Zillmann, D. , Hay, T. A. , & Bryant, J. (1975). The effect of suspense and its resolution on the appreciation of dramatic presentations. Journal of Research in Personality , 9 (4), 307–323.
  • Adolphs, R. (2013). The biology of fear. Current Biology , 23 (2), R79–R93.
  • Averill, J. R. , Malmstrom, E. J. , Koriat, A. , & Lazarus, R. S. (1972). Habituation to complex emotional stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 80 , 20–28.
  • Boyle, G. J. (1984). Effects of viewing a road trauma film on emotional and motivational factors. Accident Analysis & Prevention , 16 , 383–386.
  • Cantor, J. (2002). Fright reactions to mass media. In J. Bryant & D. Ziollmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 287–306). Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Cantor, J. (2009). Fright reactions to mass media. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects (pp. 303–319). Routledge.
  • Cantor, J. , & Nathanson, A. I. (1996). Children’s fright reactions to television news. Journal of Communication , 46 , 139–152.
  • Cantor, J. , & Sparks, G. G. (1984). Children’s fear responses to mass media: Testing some Piagetian predictions. Journal of Communication , 34 , 90–103.
  • Cantor, J. , Wilson, B. J. , & Hoffner, C. (1986). Emotional responses to a televised nuclear holocaust film. Communication Research , 13 , 257–277.
  • Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience , 15 (1), 353–375.
  • Easterling, D. V. , & Leventhal, H. (1989). Contribution of concrete cognition to emotion: neutral symptoms as elicitors of worry about cancer. Journal of Applied Psychology , 74 (5), 787.
  • Harrison, K. , & Cantor, J. (1999). Tales from the screen: Enduring fright reactions to scary media. Media Psychology , 1 , 97–116.
  • Hefner, D. , Klimmt, C. , & Vorderer, P. (2007). Identification with the player character as determinant of video game enjoyment. In L. Ma , M. Rauterberg , & R. Nakatsu (Eds.), International conference on entertainment computing 2007, lecture notes in computer science (Vol. 4740, pp. 39–48). Springer.
  • Hoffner, C. A. , & Levine, K. J. (2005). Enjoyment of mediated fright and violence: A meta-analysis. Media Psychology , 7 (2), 207–237.
  • Johnson, B. R. (1980). General occurrence of stressful reactions to commercial motion pictures and elements in films subjectively identified as stressors. Psychological Reports , 47 , 775–786.
  • Klimmt, C. , Hefner, D. , & Vorderer, P. (2009). The video game experience as “true” identification: A theory of enjoyable alterations of players’ self-perception. Communication Theory , 19(4), 351–373.
  • Lang, P. J. (1984). Cognition in emotion: Concept and action. Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior , 191 , 228.
  • Lin, J. H. (2013a). Do video games exert stronger effects on aggression than film? The role of media interactivity and identification on the association of violent content and aggressive outcomes. Computers in Human Behavior , 29 (3), 535–543.
  • Lin, J. H. (2013b). Identification matters: A moderated mediation model of media interactivity, character identification, and video game violence on aggression. Journal of Communication , 63 (4), 682–702.
  • Liu, C. , Agrawal, P. , Sarkar, N. , & Chen, S. (2009). Dynamic difficulty adjustment in computer games through real-time anxiety-based affective feedback. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction , 25 (6), 506–529.
  • Pallavicini, F. , Pepe, A. , & Minissi, M. E. (2019). Gaming in virtual reality: What changes in terms of usability, emotional response and sense of presence compared to non-immersive video games? Simulation & Gaming , 50 (2), 136–159.
  • Peng, W. , Lin, J. H. , Pfeiffer, K. A. , & Winn, B. (2012). Need satisfaction supportive game features as motivational determinants: An experimental study of a self-determination theory guided exergame. Media Psychology , 15 (2), 175–196.
  • Pinedo, I. C. (1997). Recreational terror: Women and the pleasures of horror film viewing . State University of New York Press.
  • Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. Cacioppo & R. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology (pp. 153–176). Guilford.
  • Ryan, R. M. , & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? Journal of personality , 74 (6), 1557–1586.
  • Sparks, G. G. , & Cantor, J. (1986). Developmental differences in fright responses to a television program depicting a character transformation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media , 30 , 309–323.
  • Tourangeau, R. , & Ellsworth, P. C. (1979). The role of facial response in the experience of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 37 , 1519–1531.
  • Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communications Monographs , 59 (4), 329–349.
  • Zillmann, D. (1980). Anatomy of suspense. In P. H. Tannenbaum (Ed.), The entertainment functions of television (pp. 133–163). Psychology Press.

Related Articles

  • Fear Arousal and Health and Risk Messaging
  • Media Entertainment and Emotions

Printed from Oxford Research Encyclopedias, Communication. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a single article for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 28 April 2024

  • Cookie Policy
  • Privacy Policy
  • Legal Notice
  • Accessibility
  • [66.249.64.20|185.39.149.46]
  • 185.39.149.46

Character limit 500 /500

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Horror Games: Genre Essay

Profile image of Hex L Kri

Related Papers

Bernard Perron

horror game video essay

University of Silesia in Katowice

Łukasz Schwartz

The aim of the thesis is to examine the idea of haunted media as in the context of video games with a focus on the genre of horror and using the framework of hauntology and hyperstition. In its conclusion, the thesis proposes an innovative perspective on the relationship between the player and the virtual world of the game, suggesting that from the perspective of the game world, it is the player, inhabiting the role of their character, who is effectively haunting the game.

garth mc intosh

Jedediah Bishop

Denise Schweigert

Fear is a basic reaction to a danger signal which is deeply rooted in any living individual’s brain. Hence, ‘being horrified’ as an emotional state should have an overall bad connotation, yet frightening content of any sort enjoys great popularity in pop culture, being depicted in all kind of media. People like getting shocked. From the Gothic Novel over horror movies of various subgenres to shocking videogames, being exposed to horror has always been fascinating for the human brain. Especially over the last decade, with rapidly developing technical possibilities, games provided a new and notably different approach to experiencing fear by, unlike literature or movies, actively involving the recipient. The most common type, and at the same time pioneer for horror portrayed in video games, is the genre of the Survival Horror. Even though there are numerous horror games, game studies seem to solely focus on this subgenre. There are nearly no reliable sources analyzing other horror game categories that emerged over the years - and even the research on survival horror is rather restricted to the two biggest and most popular game series in the franchise, namely Silent Hill and Resident Evil, with two existing considerable scientific essays. The focus of those studies lies mostly on fear evoked by monsters and supernatural events although the trend tends towards an establishment of psychological horror in video games. Even though there are notable differences, the resemblance to one of the earliest widely consumed horror medium cannot be denied: the Gothic Novel, or in this special case the Victorian Gothic. For these reasons, this paper tries to examine the field of psychological horror games and its roots a bit further. It analyzes the first-person gothic walking-simulator “Layers of Fear”, which blends various horror game characteristics with typical Victorian Gothic elements and heavily focuses on story and exploration from the point of view of a psychic painter. Plot-wise, his wife is his muse, but after she is disfigured in a house fire, the artist develops a schizophrenic disorder and is unable to paint. The goal of the game is to create a new magnum opus while exploring the dark past and happenings in the painter’s familiar environment. The game will be examined with regard to the Victorian Gothic narrative and therefore, the paper focuses on the similarities between the two genres in order to get a basic understanding how Layers of Fear evokes Horror. To get a fundamental overview, the first part of this paper deals with several definitions of fear and how it affects the mind. It then examines how horror is portrayed in literature, using gothic fiction as an example. The focus lies on Victorian gothic. The main part will consist of an analysis of the game “Layers of Fear“ and is divided into two sub-categories which reflect on the everlasting discussion ‘Narratology vs. Ludology’: content and form, frame, game design. Therefore, on the narration level, recurring Victorian gothic horror-motifs in the game are examined and directly compared to horror elements found in Oscar Wilde’s novel “The Picture of Dorian Gray”, since the novel is the main inspiration source for the aesthetics of the game. Of equal importance regarding horror aspects are game mechanics and design which will also be further explored. To conclude the paper, an outlook for the possible development of horror video games and their ways to trigger fear in the future is presented. All screenshots used in this paper are taken by myself to support my analysis.

Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture

Francis T. McAndrew

Why do some types of settings and some combinations of sensory information induce a sense of dread in humans? This article brings empirical evidence from psychological research to bear on the experience of horror, and explains why the tried-and-true horror devices intuitively employed by writers and filmmakers work so well. Natural selection has favored individuals who gravitated toward environments containing the “right” physical and psychological features and avoided those which posed a threat. Places that contain a bad mix of these features induce unpleasant feelings of dread and fear, and therefore have become important ingredients of the settings for horror fiction and films. This article ap­plies McAndrew and Koehnke’s (2016) theory of creepiness to the study of classic horror settings and explores the role played by architecture, isolation, association with death, and other environmental qualities in the experience of creepiness and dread.

Patrick Johnson

It has been said that fear is one of the most powerful and ancient emotions in the human race. It should come as no surprise then that horror as an art form has existed for centuries. Throughout history horror has let humans externalize their fears. It is my conjecture that the artists who make horror films and literature have, unintentionally, found a way to let people expose themselves to their very real fears by means of sophisticated metaphors. The aim of this study was twofold. First, I sought to demonstrate that horror is a reflection of the fears of the society and culture in which it was created. Secondly, I intend to show that it acts as a sort of subconscious, prolonged exposure that helps people deal with their societal fears. This study contained two main elements to test this hypothesis. The first is that participants monitored their fear levels in vivo while watching scenes from six different horror films. This was to test to see, first, if there was indeed an increase in fear from the start to the end and, second, if the end of each clip would result in a decrease in fear. This would indicate that the participants experienced some type of relief from their fear. The second element is that the participants were asked to retrospectively explain what made each scene scary. This was left vague to prevent influencing the answers, but the goal was to see if participants were relating these horror scenes with the real life fears that are represented in each of the films. The participants not only experienced a sense of relief at the end of each scene, but there was also some indication in the case of The Crazies that this may be because people are associating the film with real life fears. This study has shown that those who watch horror experience a type of relief after each scene, even if that scene does not bring about relief on its own. This is because horror is not an emotion that can be sustained during prolonged exposure. If viewers experienced a decline in horror after only 10 minutes in each scene, it is logical to assume that the decline would be even greater after the entire film was over. If audiences are experiencing this relief in the theater, it is also logical that they are experiencing this relief in their everyday lives.

Prescilla Garland

Studia Humanistyczne AGH

Katarzyna Marak

RELATED PAPERS

Claes Beckman

Buletin Agrohorti

Winarso Drajad Widodo

ACM Transactions on Multimedia Computing, Communications, and Applications

medigraphic.com

Pablo Iván Beltrán Ruiz

Trends in biotechnology

Christiane Woopen

BIOLEX REVISTA JURIDICA DEL DEPARTAMENTO DE DERECHO

Miguel Arturo Morales

yosua prawiro

Mariane de Almeida Bahiana

Frontiers in plant science

Muhammad Ali

BMC Pediatrics

The Journal of Haemophilia Practice

Brian P Flaherty

José Azcue

Indian journal of experimental biology

Amita Bhattacharya

Journal of Geoscience, Engineering, Environment, and Technology

Goji Pamungkas

Jurnal Pendidikan IPS Indonesia

I Gede Astra Wesnawa

maria vargas

Nature Chemical Biology

Sarah Lockie

Vincenzo Amoruso

American Psychologist

Shan Guisinger

Journal of the American Academy of Physician Assistants

Rachel DeWitt

Child's Nervous System

Llewellyn Padayachy

Bogoslovska smotra

Kristina Vujica

Claudio CERMELLI

Talat El-kersh

Harri Toivonen

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

VGL_Banner_wLogo.png

HORROR VIDEO GAMES: ESSAYS ON THE FUSION OF FEAR AND PLAY

Bernard perron, carl therrien, christian mccrea, clive barker, dan pinchbeck, ewan kirkland, guillaume roux-girard, inger ekman, laurie n. taylor, martin picard, matthew weise, michael nitsche, petri lankoski, richard rouse iii, simon niedenthal, tanya krzywinska.

In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror’s allure for gamers and the evolution of “survival” themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill are examined individually.

PUBLICATION DETAILS

Authors: #BernardPerron #CarlTherrien #ChristianMcCrea #CliveBarker #DanPinchbeck #EwanKirkland #GuillaumeRouxGirard #IngerEkman #LaurieNTaylor #MartinPicard #MatthewWeise #MichaelNitsche #PetriLankoski #RichardRouseIII #SimonNiedenthal #TanyaKrzywinska

Publishers: #McFarland (McFarland)

Languages: #English

Format: #Paperback #eBook

Accessibility: N/A

Year: #Year2009

Genres: #GameStudies #Horror

Public Figures: N/A

  • Game Studies
  • Non-Fiction

Related Posts

THE COMPLETE SUPER NINTENDO COVER ART COLLECTION: UNOFFICIAL USA/PAL COVER COMPENDIUM

THE 15 MOST IMPORTANT & INFLUENTIAL GAMES EVER

MASTERING NINTENDO VIDEO GAMES: TIPS, TRICKS, STRATEGIES

horror game video essay

  • Politics & Social Sciences
  • Social Sciences

Amazon prime logo

Enjoy fast, free delivery, exclusive deals, and award-winning movies & TV shows with Prime Try Prime and start saving today with fast, free delivery

Amazon Prime includes:

Fast, FREE Delivery is available to Prime members. To join, select "Try Amazon Prime and start saving today with Fast, FREE Delivery" below the Add to Cart button.

  • Cardmembers earn 5% Back at Amazon.com with a Prime Credit Card.
  • Unlimited Free Two-Day Delivery
  • Streaming of thousands of movies and TV shows with limited ads on Prime Video.
  • A Kindle book to borrow for free each month - with no due dates
  • Listen to over 2 million songs and hundreds of playlists
  • Unlimited photo storage with anywhere access

Important:  Your credit card will NOT be charged when you start your free trial or if you cancel during the trial period. If you're happy with Amazon Prime, do nothing. At the end of the free trial, your membership will automatically upgrade to a monthly membership.

Buy new: $29.95 $29.95 FREE delivery Saturday, May 4 on orders shipped by Amazon over $35 Ships from: Amazon.com Sold by: Amazon.com

Return this item for free.

Free returns are available for the shipping address you chose. You can return the item for any reason in new and unused condition: no shipping charges

  • Go to your orders and start the return
  • Select the return method

Buy used: $26.55

Kindle app logo image

Download the free Kindle app and start reading Kindle books instantly on your smartphone, tablet, or computer - no Kindle device required .

Read instantly on your browser with Kindle for Web.

Using your mobile phone camera - scan the code below and download the Kindle app.

QR code to download the Kindle App

Image Unavailable

Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

  • To view this video download Flash Player

Follow the authors

Christian McCrea

Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play Paperback – September 29, 2009

Purchase options and add-ons.

In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill are examined individually.

  • Print length 310 pages
  • Language English
  • Publisher McFarland & Company
  • Publication date September 29, 2009
  • Reading age 18 years and up
  • Dimensions 6 x 0.62 x 9 inches
  • ISBN-10 0786441976
  • ISBN-13 978-0786441976
  • See all details

Books with Buzz

Similar items that may deliver to you quickly

The World of Scary Video Games: A Study in Videoludic Horror (Approaches to Digital Game Studies)

Editorial Reviews

From the inside flap, from the back cover, about the author, product details.

  • Publisher ‏ : ‎ McFarland & Company (September 29, 2009)
  • Language ‏ : ‎ English
  • Paperback ‏ : ‎ 310 pages
  • ISBN-10 ‏ : ‎ 0786441976
  • ISBN-13 ‏ : ‎ 978-0786441976
  • Reading age ‏ : ‎ 18 years and up
  • Item Weight ‏ : ‎ 15.3 ounces
  • Dimensions ‏ : ‎ 6 x 0.62 x 9 inches
  • #4,983 in Video & Computer Games
  • #6,315 in Computer & Video Game Strategy Guides
  • #9,694 in Popular Culture in Social Sciences

About the authors

Christian mccrea.

Christian McCrea is a researcher writing on science fiction, film, videogames, animation and the popular digital arts. He is also a lecturer in the School of Design at RMIT University, Melbourne, Australia.

He has written book chapters on the Australian game industry, on watching Starcraft, on horror videogame aesthetics and many others. He writes regularly for magazines and other media, appears on podcasts, and tweets at @christianmccrea

His first full-length book, on David Lynch's Dune, will appear in early 2019 as part of Auteur Press's Constellations, a series of books on science fiction films.

Laurie N. Taylor

Laurie N. Taylor is the Associate University Librarian for Collections & Discovery at the University of Connecticut (UConn). She provides leadership, strategy, and support for the Collections & Discovery Area. She has held leadership positions in research libraries, including the University of Florida as Senior Director for Library Technology and Digital Strategies, Chair of the Digital Partnerships and Strategies Department, and the Editor-in-Chief of the LibraryPress@UF. She previously served as the Operational Lead and Digital Scholarship Director of the Digital Library of the Caribbean (dLOC). In 2018, Laurie was awarded the Caribbean Information Professional of the Year by the Association of Caribbean University, Research and Institutional Libraries (ACURIL). Laurie is new to New England and thrilled to grow her community here and to explore the area with her partner.

Customer reviews

Customer Reviews, including Product Star Ratings help customers to learn more about the product and decide whether it is the right product for them.

To calculate the overall star rating and percentage breakdown by star, we don’t use a simple average. Instead, our system considers things like how recent a review is and if the reviewer bought the item on Amazon. It also analyzed reviews to verify trustworthiness.

  • Sort reviews by Top reviews Most recent Top reviews

Top reviews from the United States

There was a problem filtering reviews right now. please try again later..

horror game video essay

Top reviews from other countries

horror game video essay

  • Amazon Newsletter
  • About Amazon
  • Accessibility
  • Sustainability
  • Press Center
  • Investor Relations
  • Amazon Devices
  • Amazon Science
  • Sell on Amazon
  • Sell apps on Amazon
  • Supply to Amazon
  • Protect & Build Your Brand
  • Become an Affiliate
  • Become a Delivery Driver
  • Start a Package Delivery Business
  • Advertise Your Products
  • Self-Publish with Us
  • Become an Amazon Hub Partner
  • › See More Ways to Make Money
  • Amazon Visa
  • Amazon Store Card
  • Amazon Secured Card
  • Amazon Business Card
  • Shop with Points
  • Credit Card Marketplace
  • Reload Your Balance
  • Amazon Currency Converter
  • Your Account
  • Your Orders
  • Shipping Rates & Policies
  • Amazon Prime
  • Returns & Replacements
  • Manage Your Content and Devices
  • Recalls and Product Safety Alerts
  • Conditions of Use
  • Privacy Notice
  • Consumer Health Data Privacy Disclosure
  • Your Ads Privacy Choices

an image, when javascript is unavailable

‘Alien: Rogue Incursion’ Horror VR Game From 20th Century Games to Launch Holiday 2024, First Trailer Revealed

By Jennifer Maas

Jennifer Maas

TV Business Writer

  • ‘Alien: Rogue Incursion’ Horror VR Game From 20th Century Games to Launch Holiday 2024, First Trailer Revealed 2 days ago
  • New AMC Networks CEO Kristin Dolan’s 2023 Pay Reaches $14.5 Million 2 days ago
  • Roku Bests Wall Street’s Q1 Earnings Forecast, Adds 1.6 Million Active Accounts to Reach 81.6 Million 3 days ago

Yaphet Kotto Sigourney Weaver Alien Tribute

“ Alien ” franchise VR action-horror game “ Alien: Rogue Incursion ” will launch this holiday season from 20th Century Games and studio Survios.

The new single-player will be available across PlayStation VR2, PCVR via Steam and Meta Quest 3.

Based on the iconic film franchise led by Sigourney Weaver, “Alien: Rogue Incursion” marks the first VR game set in the world of “Alien,” which is made up of the original 1979 film produced by 20th Century Films, as well as its three sequel movies, two “Alien vs. Predator” films, two “Prometheus” films, and the upcoming “Alien” TV series from Noah Hawley and FX, and forthcoming film “Alien: Romulus.”

Popular on Variety

“Our team at Survios are huge fans of ‘Alien’ and have been building ‘Alien: Rogue Incursion’ for a long time, honing our ability to pair the most technically advanced, immersive, and engaging VR experiences with best-in-class franchises,” TQ Jefferson, chief product officer at Survios, said. “This fully original story embraces all our favorite elements from 45 years of ‘Alien,’ from kinetic action and heart-pounding exploration to our terrifyingly resourceful Xenomorphs that will truly make your skin crawl. We can’t wait for fans to get their hands on it this holiday season.”

Watch the trailer for new VR game “Alien: Rogue Incursion” below.

More From Our Brands

Miranda lambert surprises stagecoach 2024 with reba mcentire, debuts new song ‘wranglers’, inside a $3.3 million one-bedroom condo in l.a.’s famed sierra towers, vince mcmahon lists final tko shares for sale, be tough on dirt but gentle on your body with the best soaps for sensitive skin, fire country: did luke’s bombshell blow up [spoiler]’s future michael trucco teases what’s ahead, verify it's you, please log in.

Quantcast

(Stanford users can avoid this Captcha by logging in.)

  • Send to text email RefWorks EndNote printer

Horror video games : essays on the fusion of fear and play

Available online, at the library.

horror game video essay

Green Library

More options.

  • Find it at other libraries via WorldCat
  • Contributors

Description

Creators/contributors, contents/summary, bibliographic information, browse related items.

Stanford University

  • Stanford Home
  • Maps & Directions
  • Search Stanford
  • Emergency Info
  • Terms of Use
  • Non-Discrimination
  • Accessibility

© Stanford University , Stanford , California 94305 .

Icon image

Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

About this ebook, ratings and reviews.

horror game video essay

  • Flag inappropriate

horror game video essay

About the author

Rate this ebook, reading information, more by bernard perron.

Thumbnail image

Similar ebooks

Thumbnail image

IMAGES

  1. Top 10 Ultimate Best Horror Games to Play in 2019

    horror game video essay

  2. Toasty Games Essay: Bloodborne's Cosmic Horror

    horror game video essay

  3. The 25 best horror games on PC

    horror game video essay

  4. Best Horror Games of the Past Decade

    horror game video essay

  5. The resurgence of the PS1 horror game

    horror game video essay

  6. THE RITUAL (Indie Horror Game) on Steam

    horror game video essay

VIDEO

  1. Hidden Gems hidden among horrible horror games. [VIDEO ESSAY]

  2. ПЕРВЫЙ ИСПУГ С ВЕБКОЙ -▶ CRY OF FEAR

  3. CRY OF FEAR WALKTHROUGH PART 6

  4. CRY OF FEAR WALKTHROUGH PART 3

  5. CRY OF FEAR WALKTHROUGH PART 10

  6. CRY OF FEAR WALKTHROUGH PART 1

COMMENTS

  1. Video Essays About Horror Games : r/HorrorGaming

    I don't know of a channel that specializes in horror games only, but I know a few high quality videos that I think you'd love. SOMA Lore series. Resident Evil 3 An Evolution of Fear. The Genius Design of Mr. X in RE2 Remake. The Most Profound Moment in Gaming History. 8.

  2. Visage is a Most Disturbing Horror Game

    The Best Horror Experience In DecadesThis video analysis looks at a lil'game called Visage, a modern horror piece that has taken inspiration from PT and sile...

  3. Looking for good horror video essay channel recommendations

    It's not a video essay but try the book "Danse macabre" by Stephen king. It's an essay on horror and the influence of contemporary societal fears and anxieties on the genre. It was republished on February 23, 2010, with an additional new essay entitled "What's Scary". Well worth a read.

  4. Detention

    Hey, this video took a while to make so you better like it :DDetention is not the usual horror game, instead of the visual and sounds taking center stage, it...

  5. Why HOUSE Is Such A UNIQUE Horror Game

    Todays horror games kind of all follow the same trend. 3D along with the game being a mascot horror(Examples: Poppy playtime, Garten of ban ban and many othe...

  6. Horror Video Games and the "Active-Passive" Debate

    In Perron's edited compendium of essays regarding horror video games subtitled Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play (2009), much of the argumentation orbits debate regarding the definition and creation of the experience of horror compared between an ostensibly passive cinema reception (from whence the games take most of their conventions) and the ostensibly more active reception of ...

  7. Horror Video Games : Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

    Books. Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play. In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call ...

  8. Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear

    Horror Video Games: Essay on the Fusion of Fear and Play attempts to fill that void. The collection is divided into two parts: "Approaching the Genre" and "Encountering the Games." The first section takes a more theoretical approach and broadly looks at the horror genre in video games, while the second section is dedicated to examining specific ...

  9. r/horror on Reddit: Any especially good video essays, articles or books

    This playlist has a good variety of video essays on horror that are pretty good. Some are about specific pieces of media but several delve into general analysis of horror. ... For more than a decade /R/HORROR has been reddit.com's gateway to all things Horror: from movies & TV, to books & games. Members Online. Late Night With the Devil ...

  10. Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

    Bernard Perron (Editor) In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill are examined ...

  11. Horror video games : essays on the fusion of fear and play

    "In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill are examined individually"--Provided ...

  12. Virtual Reality Horror Games and Fear in Gaming

    Players' Appeal to Horror Games. Regarding video games (Kjeldgaard-Christiansen & Clasen, 2019), researchers argue that a combination of evolutionary theory and game studies can explain why players are drawn to horror video games (Lynch, 2018).Horror games allow players to simulate fear scenarios of uncertainty and danger, challenging them to assess and negotiate potential threats.

  13. (DOC) Horror Games: Genre Essay

    Horror Games: Genre Research Essay There are a lot of video game enthusiasts that just don't understand the appeal of horror games. Many users play for the thrill of the adrenaline rush. The scares are like riding a roller coaster: they're safe and tested every day, and don't actually pose any danger.

  14. Why Do Horror Games Stop Being Scary?

    With so many horror game series out there, there have been so many that have become less scary over time. Why is this? Why do scary games stop being scary? L...

  15. Horror Video Games: Essays on The Fusion of Fear and Play

    In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill are examined individually.

  16. Femininity IS Horror : Horror Games, Girlhood and Women

    A delightfully polarizing term for women who game. This is a community space for ladies to hang out, talk about gaming, and game together. We also discuss topics around women in geek culture and debrief about experiences that occur as a result of their gender. Or you know, just post some bad ass makeup tutorials inspired by video games.

  17. Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

    In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill are examined individually.

  18. 'Alien: Rogue Incursion' Horror VR Game From 20th Century Games to

    "Alien" franchise VR action-horror game "Alien: Rogue Incursion" will launch this holiday season from 20th Century Games and studio Survios. The new single-player will be available across ...

  19. Videogames video essays by women? : r/GirlGamers

    r/GirlGamers. A delightfully polarizing term for women who game. This is a community space for ladies to hang out, talk about gaming, and game together. We also discuss topics around women in geek culture and debrief about experiences that occur as a result of their gender. Or you know, just post some bad ass makeup tutorials inspired by video ...

  20. Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

    In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of...

  21. The Most Terrifying Game Isn't A Horror Game...

    In this video we'll take a look at one of the most terrifying games I've ever played. Oddly enough it's not a horror game but an extremely dark military shoo...

  22. Horror video games : essays on the fusion of fear and play

    Publisher's summary. A theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games. It includes 14 essays that explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of 'survival' themes. It individually examines the techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu, and Silent Hill.

  23. Horror in Video Games

    oh no i accidentally made a scary video!! ! and now im SCARED??!!!! wow!!! that's so cool!!!!! :)Support me for a kiss https://patreon.com/nakeyjakeyold s...

  24. Horror Video Games: Essays on the Fusion of Fear and Play

    In this in-depth critical and theoretical analysis of the horror genre in video games, 14 essays explore the cultural underpinnings of horror's allure for gamers and the evolution of "survival" themes. The techniques and story effects of specific games such as Resident Evil, Call of Cthulhu,and Silent Hillare examined individually.