• 1.1 Etymology
  • 1.2 Pronunciation
  • 1.3.1 Synonyms
  • 1.3.2 Derived terms
  • 1.3.3 Translations
  • 2.1 Etymology
  • 2.2 Pronunciation
  • 2.3.1 Declension

English [ edit ]

Etymology [ edit ].

Recorded since 1596, from Middle French hypothese , from Late Latin hypothesis , from Ancient Greek ὑπόθεσις ( hupóthesis , “ base, basis of an argument, supposition ” , literally “ a placing under ” ) , itself from ὑποτίθημι ( hupotíthēmi , “ I set before, suggest ” ) , from ὑπό ( hupó , “ below ” ) + τίθημι ( títhēmi , “ I put, place ” ) .

Pronunciation [ edit ]

  • ( UK ) IPA ( key ) : /haɪˈpɒθɪsɪs/ , /hɪˈpɒθɪsɪs/ , /həˈpɒθɪsɪs/ , /-əsəs/ , /-əsɪs/
  • ( US ) IPA ( key ) : /haɪˈpɑː.θə.sɪs/

Noun [ edit ]

hypothesis ( plural hypotheses )

  • 2001 September 27, Terrie E. Moffitt, Avshalom Caspi, Michael Rutter, Phil A. Silva, Sex Differences in Antisocial Behaviour: Conduct Disorder, Delinquency, and Violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study ‎ [1] , Cambridge University Press , →ISBN , page 151 : This hypothesis goes by many names, including group resistence, the threshold effect, and the gender paradox. Because the hypothesis holds such wide appeal, it is worth revisiting the logic behind it. The hypothesis is built on the factual observation that fewer females than males act antisocially.
  • 2005 , Ronald H. Pine, http://www.csicop.org/specialarticles/show/intelligent_design_or_no_model_creationism , 15 October 2005: Far too many of us have been taught in school that a scientist, in the course of trying to figure something out, will first come up with a " hypothesis " (a guess or surmise—not necessarily even an "educated" guess). ... [But t]he word " hypothesis " should be used, in science, exclusively for a reasoned, sensible, knowledge-informed explanation for why some phenomenon exists or occurs. An hypothesis can be as yet untested; can have already been tested; may have been falsified; may have not yet been falsified, although tested; or may have been tested in a myriad of ways countless times without being falsified; and it may come to be universally accepted by the scientific community. An understanding of the word " hypothesis ," as used in science, requires a grasp of the principles underlying Occam's Razor and Karl Popper's thought in regard to " falsifiability "—including the notion that any respectable scientific hypothesis must, in principle, be "capable of" being proven wrong (if it should, in fact, just happen to be wrong), but none can ever be proved to be true. One aspect of a proper understanding of the word " hypothesis ," as used in science, is that only a vanishingly small percentage of hypotheses could ever potentially become a theory.
  • ( general ) An assumption taken to be true for the purpose of argument or investigation .
  • ( grammar ) The antecedent of a conditional statement .

Synonyms [ edit ]

  • supposition
  • educated guess
  • See also Thesaurus:supposition

Derived terms [ edit ]

  • alternative hypothesis
  • aquatic ape hypothesis
  • Avogadro's hypothesis
  • conspiracy hypothesis
  • continuum hypothesis
  • cosmic censorship hypothesis
  • documentary hypothesis
  • efficient market hypothesis
  • ergodic hypothesis
  • expectations hypothesis
  • Fisher hypothesis
  • Gaia hypothesis
  • generalized continuum hypothesis
  • God hypothesis
  • Griesbach hypothesis
  • hypothesize
  • hypothetical
  • hypothetically
  • interface hypothesis
  • just-world hypothesis
  • level-ordering hypothesis
  • mafia hypothesis
  • Medea hypothesis
  • Monro-Kellie hypothesis
  • null hypothesis
  • Omphalos hypothesis
  • Out of India hypothesis
  • ovulatory shift hypothesis
  • permanent income hypothesis
  • Prout's hypothesis
  • Rare Earth hypothesis
  • Red Queen hypothesis
  • Riemann hypothesis
  • Sapir-Whorf hypothesis
  • Schinzel's hypothesis H
  • sexy son hypothesis
  • simulation hypothesis
  • swoon hypothesis
  • trickle-down hypothesis
  • trickle down hypothesis
  • Wellhausen's hypothesis
  • working hypothesis
  • zombie hypothesis

Translations [ edit ]

Latin [ edit ].

Borrowed from Ancient Greek ὑπόθεσις ( hupóthesis , “ hypothesis ” , noun ) .

  • ( Classical ) IPA ( key ) : /hyˈpo.tʰe.sis/ , [hʏˈpɔt̪ʰɛs̠ɪs̠]
  • ( modern Italianate Ecclesiastical ) IPA ( key ) : /iˈpo.te.sis/ , [iˈpɔːt̪es̬is]

hypothesis   f ( genitive hypothesis or hypotheseōs or hypothesios ) ; third declension

Declension [ edit ]

1 Found sometimes in Medieval and New Latin.

  • There is also genitive plural hypotheseōn .
  • The genitive singular is also spelled hypotheseωs and the genitive plural hypotheseωn .

root of word hypothesis

  • English terms derived from Proto-Indo-European
  • English terms derived from the Proto-Indo-European root *dʰeh₁-
  • English terms borrowed from Middle French
  • English terms derived from Middle French
  • English terms derived from Late Latin
  • English terms derived from Ancient Greek
  • English 4-syllable words
  • English terms with IPA pronunciation
  • English terms with audio links
  • English lemmas
  • English nouns
  • English countable nouns
  • English nouns with irregular plurals
  • en:Sciences
  • English terms with quotations
  • Latin terms borrowed from Ancient Greek
  • Latin terms derived from Ancient Greek
  • Latin 4-syllable words
  • Latin terms with IPA pronunciation
  • Latin lemmas
  • Latin nouns
  • Latin third declension nouns
  • Latin feminine nouns in the third declension
  • Latin terms spelled with Y
  • Latin feminine nouns
  • English entries with language name categories using raw markup
  • Requests for translations into Burmese
  • Urdu terms with non-redundant manual transliterations
  • Requests for translations into Russian
  • Requests for review of French translations
  • Requests for review of Icelandic translations
  • Requests for review of Persian translations
  • Requests for review of Romanian translations
  • Requests for review of Swedish translations
  • Requests for review of Turkish translations
  • Latin nouns with red links in their inflection tables

Navigation menu

  • Daily Crossword
  • Word Puzzle
  • Word Finder
  • Word of the Day
  • Synonym of the Day
  • Word of the Year
  • Language stories
  • All featured
  • Gender and sexuality
  • All pop culture
  • Grammar Coach ™
  • Writing hub
  • Grammar essentials
  • Commonly confused
  • All writing tips
  • Pop culture
  • Writing tips

Advertisement

[ hahy- poth - uh -sis , hi- ]

  • a proposition, or set of propositions, set forth as an explanation for the occurrence of some specified group of phenomena, either asserted merely as a provisional conjecture to guide investigation working hypothesis or accepted as highly probable in the light of established facts.
  • a proposition assumed as a premise in an argument.
  • the antecedent of a conditional proposition.
  • a mere assumption or guess.

/ haɪˈpɒθɪsɪs /

  • a suggested explanation for a group of facts or phenomena, either accepted as a basis for further verification ( working hypothesis ) or accepted as likely to be true Compare theory
  • an assumption used in an argument without its being endorsed; a supposition
  • an unproved theory; a conjecture

/ hī-pŏth ′ ĭ-sĭs /

, Plural hypotheses hī-pŏth ′ ĭ-sēz′

  • A statement that explains or makes generalizations about a set of facts or principles, usually forming a basis for possible experiments to confirm its viability.
  • plur. hypotheses (heye- poth -uh-seez) In science, a statement of a possible explanation for some natural phenomenon. A hypothesis is tested by drawing conclusions from it; if observation and experimentation show a conclusion to be false, the hypothesis must be false. ( See scientific method and theory .)

Discover More

Derived forms.

  • hyˈpothesist , noun

Other Words From

  • hy·pothe·sist noun
  • counter·hy·pothe·sis noun plural counterhypotheses
  • subhy·pothe·sis noun plural subhypotheses

Word History and Origins

Origin of hypothesis 1

Synonym Study

Example sentences.

Each one is a set of questions we’re fascinated by and hypotheses we’re testing.

Mousa’s research hinges on the “contact hypothesis,” the idea that positive interactions among rival group members can reduce prejudices.

Do more research on it, come up with a hypothesis as to why it underperforms, and try to improve it.

Now is the time to test your hypotheses to figure out what’s changing in your customers’ worlds, and address these topics directly.

Whether computing power alone is enough to fuel continued machine learning breakthroughs is a source of debate, but it seems clear we’ll be able to test the hypothesis.

Though researchers have struggled to understand exactly what contributes to this gender difference, Dr. Rohan has one hypothesis.

The leading hypothesis for the ultimate source of the Ebola virus, and where it retreats in between outbreaks, lies in bats.

In 1996, John Paul II called the Big Bang theory “more than a hypothesis.”

To be clear: There have been no double-blind or controlled studies that conclusively confirm this hair-loss hypothesis.

The bacteria-driven-ritual hypothesis ignores the huge diversity of reasons that could push someone to perform a religious ritual.

And remember it is by our hypothesis the best possible form and arrangement of that lesson.

Taken in connection with what we know of the nebulæ, the proof of Laplace's nebular hypothesis may fairly be regarded as complete.

What has become of the letter from M. de St. Mars, said to have been discovered some years ago, confirming this last hypothesis?

To admit that there had really been any communication between the dead man and the living one is also an hypothesis.

"I consider it highly probable," asserted Aunt Maria, forgetting her Scandinavian hypothesis.

Related Words

  • explanation
  • interpretation
  • proposition
  • supposition

More About Hypothesis

What is a hypothesis .

In science, a hypothesis is a statement or proposition that attempts to explain phenomena or facts. Hypotheses are often tested to see if they are accurate.

Crafting a useful hypothesis is one of the early steps in the scientific method , which is central to every field of scientific experimentation. A useful scientific hypothesis is based on current, accepted scientific knowledge and is testable.

Outside of science, the word hypothesis is often used more loosely to mean a guess or prediction.

Why is hypothesis important?

The first records of the term hypothesis come from around 1590. It comes from the Greek term hypóthesis , meaning “basis, supposition.”

Trustworthy science involves experiments and tests. In order to have an experiment, you need to test something. In science, that something is called a hypothesis . It is important to remember that, in science, a verified hypothesis is not actually confirmed to be an absolute truth. Instead, it is accepted to be accurate according to modern knowledge. Science always allows for the possibility that new information could disprove a widely accepted hypothesis .

Related to this, scientists will usually only propose a new hypothesis when new information is discovered because there is no reason to test something that is already accepted as scientifically accurate.

Did you know … ?

It can take a long time and even the discovery of new technology to confirm that a hypothesis is accurate. Physicist Albert Einstein ’s 1916 theory of relativity contained hypotheses about space and time that have only been confirmed recently, thanks to modern technology!

What are real-life examples of hypothesis ?

While in science, hypothesis has a narrow meaning, in general use its meaning is broader.

"This study confirms the hypothesis that individuals who have been infected with COVID-19 have persistent objectively measurable cognitive deficits." (N=81,337) Ventilation subgroup show 7-point reduction in IQ https://t.co/50xrNNHC5E — Claire Lehmann (@clairlemon) July 23, 2021
Not everyone drives. They can walk, cycle, catch a train, tram etc. That’s alternatives. What’s your alternative in your hypothesis? — Barry (@Bazzaboy1982) July 27, 2021

What other words are related to hypothesis ?

  • scientific method
  • scientific theory

Quiz yourself!

True or False?

In science, a hypothesis must be based on current scientific information and be testable.

Words and phrases

Personal account.

  • Access or purchase personal subscriptions
  • Get our newsletter
  • Save searches
  • Set display preferences

Institutional access

Sign in with library card

Sign in with username / password

Recommend to your librarian

Institutional account management

Sign in as administrator on Oxford Academic

hypothesis noun

  • Hide all quotations

What does the noun hypothesis mean?

There are seven meanings listed in OED's entry for the noun hypothesis , two of which are labelled obsolete. See ‘Meaning & use’ for definitions, usage, and quotation evidence.

Entry status

OED is undergoing a continuous programme of revision to modernize and improve definitions. This entry has not yet been fully revised.

How common is the noun hypothesis ?

How is the noun hypothesis pronounced, british english, u.s. english, where does the noun hypothesis come from.

Earliest known use

The earliest known use of the noun hypothesis is in the late 1500s.

OED's earliest evidence for hypothesis is from 1596, in the writing of Earl of Essex.

hypothesis is a borrowing from Greek.

Etymons: Greek ὑπόθεσις .

Nearby entries

  • hypothecarious, adj. 1726–
  • hypothecary, adj. 1656–
  • hypothecate, v. 1693–
  • hypothecation, n. 1681–
  • hypothecative, adj. 1856–
  • hypothecator, n. 1828–
  • hypothecium, n. 1866–
  • hypothenar, adj. 1706–
  • hypothermia, n. 1886–
  • hypothermic, adj. 1898–
  • hypothesis, n. 1596–
  • hypothesist, n. 1788–
  • hypothesize, v. 1738–
  • hypothesizer, n. 1833–
  • hypothetic, adj. & n. a1680–
  • hypothetical, adj. & n. 1588–
  • hypothetically, adv. 1628–
  • hypothetico-deductive, adj. 1912–
  • hypothetico-deductively, adv. 1953–
  • hypothetico-disjunctive, adj. & n. a1856–
  • hypothetist, n. 1852–

Thank you for visiting Oxford English Dictionary

To continue reading, please sign in below or purchase a subscription. After purchasing, please sign in below to access the content.

Meaning & use

Pronunciation, compounds & derived words, entry history for hypothesis, n..

hypothesis, n. was first published in 1899; not yet revised.

hypothesis, n. was last modified in July 2023.

Revision of the OED is a long-term project. Entries in oed.com which have not been revised may include:

  • corrections and revisions to definitions, pronunciation, etymology, headwords, variant spellings, quotations, and dates;
  • new senses, phrases, and quotations which have been added in subsequent print and online updates.

Revisions and additions of this kind were last incorporated into hypothesis, n. in July 2023.

Earlier versions of this entry were published in:

OED First Edition (1899)

  • Find out more

OED Second Edition (1989)

  • View hypothesis in OED Second Edition

Please submit your feedback for hypothesis, n.

Please include your email address if you are happy to be contacted about your feedback. OUP will not use this email address for any other purpose.

Citation details

Factsheet for hypothesis, n., browse entry.

  • Dictionaries home
  • American English
  • Collocations
  • German-English
  • Grammar home
  • Practical English Usage
  • Learn & Practise Grammar (Beta)
  • Word Lists home
  • My Word Lists
  • Recent additions
  • Resources home
  • Text Checker

Definition of hypothesis noun from the Oxford Advanced Learner's Dictionary

  • to formulate/confirm a hypothesis
  • a hypothesis about the function of dreams
  • There is little evidence to support these hypotheses.
  • formulate/​advance a theory/​hypothesis
  • build/​construct/​create/​develop a simple/​theoretical/​mathematical model
  • develop/​establish/​provide/​use a theoretical/​conceptual framework
  • advance/​argue/​develop the thesis that…
  • explore an idea/​a concept/​a hypothesis
  • make a prediction/​an inference
  • base a prediction/​your calculations on something
  • investigate/​evaluate/​accept/​challenge/​reject a theory/​hypothesis/​model
  • design an experiment/​a questionnaire/​a study/​a test
  • do research/​an experiment/​an analysis
  • make observations/​measurements/​calculations
  • carry out/​conduct/​perform an experiment/​a test/​a longitudinal study/​observations/​clinical trials
  • run an experiment/​a simulation/​clinical trials
  • repeat an experiment/​a test/​an analysis
  • replicate a study/​the results/​the findings
  • observe/​study/​examine/​investigate/​assess a pattern/​a process/​a behaviour
  • fund/​support the research/​project/​study
  • seek/​provide/​get/​secure funding for research
  • collect/​gather/​extract data/​information
  • yield data/​evidence/​similar findings/​the same results
  • analyse/​examine the data/​soil samples/​a specimen
  • consider/​compare/​interpret the results/​findings
  • fit the data/​model
  • confirm/​support/​verify a prediction/​a hypothesis/​the results/​the findings
  • prove a conjecture/​hypothesis/​theorem
  • draw/​make/​reach the same conclusions
  • read/​review the records/​literature
  • describe/​report an experiment/​a study
  • present/​publish/​summarize the results/​findings
  • present/​publish/​read/​review/​cite a paper in a scientific journal
  • Her hypothesis concerns the role of electromagnetic radiation.
  • Her study is based on the hypothesis that language simplification is possible.
  • It is possible to make a hypothesis on the basis of this graph.
  • None of the hypotheses can be rejected at this stage.
  • Scientists have proposed a bold hypothesis.
  • She used this data to test her hypothesis
  • The hypothesis predicts that children will perform better on task A than on task B.
  • The results confirmed his hypothesis on the use of modal verbs.
  • These observations appear to support our working hypothesis.
  • a speculative hypothesis concerning the nature of matter
  • an interesting hypothesis about the development of language
  • Advances in genetics seem to confirm these hypotheses.
  • His hypothesis about what dreams mean provoked a lot of debate.
  • Research supports the hypothesis that language skills are centred in the left side of the brain.
  • The survey will be used to test the hypothesis that people who work outside the home are fitter and happier.
  • This economic model is really a working hypothesis.
  • speculative
  • concern something
  • be based on something
  • predict something
  • on a/​the hypothesis
  • hypothesis about
  • hypothesis concerning

Take your English to the next level

The Oxford Learner’s Thesaurus explains the difference between groups of similar words. Try it for free as part of the Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary app

root of word hypothesis

  • Cambridge Dictionary +Plus

Definition of hypothesis – Learner’s Dictionary

Your browser doesn't support HTML5 audio

(Definition of hypothesis from the Cambridge Learner's Dictionary © Cambridge University Press)

Translations of hypothesis

Get a quick, free translation!

{{randomImageQuizHook.quizId}}

Word of the Day

veterinary surgeon

formal for vet

Dead ringers and peas in pods (Talking about similarities, Part 2)

Dead ringers and peas in pods (Talking about similarities, Part 2)

root of word hypothesis

Learn more with +Plus

  • Recent and Recommended {{#preferredDictionaries}} {{name}} {{/preferredDictionaries}}
  • Definitions Clear explanations of natural written and spoken English English Learner’s Dictionary Essential British English Essential American English
  • Grammar and thesaurus Usage explanations of natural written and spoken English Grammar Thesaurus
  • Pronunciation British and American pronunciations with audio English Pronunciation
  • English–Chinese (Simplified) Chinese (Simplified)–English
  • English–Chinese (Traditional) Chinese (Traditional)–English
  • English–Dutch Dutch–English
  • English–French French–English
  • English–German German–English
  • English–Indonesian Indonesian–English
  • English–Italian Italian–English
  • English–Japanese Japanese–English
  • English–Norwegian Norwegian–English
  • English–Polish Polish–English
  • English–Portuguese Portuguese–English
  • English–Spanish Spanish–English
  • English–Swedish Swedish–English
  • Dictionary +Plus Word Lists
  • Learner’s Dictionary    Noun
  • Translations
  • All translations

Add hypothesis to one of your lists below, or create a new one.

{{message}}

Something went wrong.

There was a problem sending your report.

Go to the homepage

Definition of 'hypothesis'

IPA Pronunciation Guide

Video: pronunciation of hypothesis

Youtube video

hypothesis in American English

Hypothesis in british english, examples of 'hypothesis' in a sentence hypothesis, related word partners hypothesis, trends of hypothesis.

View usage over: Since Exist Last 10 years Last 50 years Last 100 years Last 300 years

In other languages hypothesis

  • American English : hypothesis / haɪˈpɒθɪsɪs /
  • Brazilian Portuguese : hipótese
  • Chinese : 假设
  • European Spanish : hipótesis
  • French : hypothèse
  • German : Hypothese
  • Italian : ipotesi
  • Japanese : 仮説
  • Korean : 가설
  • European Portuguese : hipótese
  • Spanish : hipótesis
  • Thai : สมมุติฐาน

Browse alphabetically hypothesis

  • hypothermia
  • hypothermic
  • hypothesis states
  • hypothesis suggests
  • hypothesis testing
  • All ENGLISH words that begin with 'H'

Related terms of hypothesis

  • Gaia hypothesis
  • null hypothesis
  • initial hypothesis
  • View more related words

Quick word challenge

Quiz Review

Score: 0 / 5

Tile

Wordle Helper

Tile

Scrabble Tools

Image

  • Search Menu
  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Urban Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Culture
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Media
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Lifestyle, Home, and Garden
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Society
  • Law and Politics
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Neuroanaesthesia
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Oncology
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Medical Ethics
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business History
  • Business Strategy
  • Business Ethics
  • Business and Government
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic History
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • International Political Economy
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Theory
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Politics and Law
  • Public Administration
  • Public Policy
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Developmental and Physical Disabilities Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

The Oxford Handbook of Language Production

  • < Previous chapter
  • Next chapter >

The Oxford Handbook of Language Production

9 The Morphology of Words

James P. Blevins is Reader in Morphology and Syntax in the University of Cambridge. His current research is concerned mainly with the structure and complexity of inflectional systems. These issues are approached from the standpoint of contemporary word and paradigm models, interpreted from a largely information-theoretic and discriminative perspective.

  • Published: 01 July 2014
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

Morphology is usually understood as the branch of linguistics that investigates word structure, a topic of central relevance to the systematic study of language and language processing. The Western grammatical tradition begins with the identification of words as the smallest meaningful elements of speech, a conception that survives largely intact in contemporary word-based models of morphology and grammar. Synchronic, historical, and behavioral evidence also suggests that words are not only organized into syntagmatic units but also into paradigmatic collections. On the syntagmatic dimension, words are composed of morphs and themselves form parts of larger syntactic constructions. Orthogonal to these structures, inflected and derivational forms exhibit an organization into inflectional paradigms and larger morphological families. This chapter outlines some of the linguistic issues that arise in describing words and their structure.

Morphology is usually understood as the branch of linguistics that investigates “word structure.” One dimension of this investigation focusses on “the study of morphemes and their arrangement into words” ( Nida, 1949 , p. 1). A second, complementary, dimension examines “the forms of words in different uses and constructions” ( Matthews, 1991 , 3). Words are central to both of these investigations, reflecting the position they have occupied in models of language description and analysis since classical antiquity. The Western grammatical tradition begins with the identification of words as the smallest meaningful elements of speech, a conception that survives largely intact in contemporary word-based models of morphology and grammar. A similar perspective is represented in reference and pedagogical traditions that describe the lexical resources of a language primarily in terms of its word stock. Even Bloomfield (1933) freely conceded the practical value of words for dictionaries and reference grammars.

For the purposes of ordinary life, the word is the smallest unit of speech. Our dictionaries list the words of a language; for all purposes except the systematic study of language, this procedure is doubtless more useful than would be a list of morphemes. (p. 178)

The treatment of words as the smallest meaningful units of language is also implicit in procedures of phonemic analysis, which distinguish allophones from phonemes in terms of whether a given phone distinguishes the meaning of two words. The salience of words reinforces their role as the primary exponent of form variation in a language. Words are the smallest grammatical units that are demarcated with any consistency in the speech stream. They are also the smallest units that can function as utterances in isolation. Stages of first language acquisition are commonly defined in terms of the number of words per utterance. Behavioral correlates of word frequency, measured both in terms of “types” and “tokens,” have been observed in psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic studies of language processing. Synchronic, historical, and behavioral evidence also suggests that words are not only organized into syntagmatic units but also into paradigmatic collections. On the syntagmatic dimension, words are composed of morphs and themselves form parts of larger syntactic constructions. Orthogonal to these structures, inflected and derivational forms exhibit organization into inflectional paradigms and larger morphological families.

Word structure is thus of central relevance to the “systematic study of language” and language processing. To set the context for the experimental and computational studies presented in subsequent sections, this chapter outlines some of the linguistic issues that arise in describing words and their structure.

A treatment of words and word structure must begin by acknowledging that the definition of words is far from settled. Despite this lack of consensus, there are points of broad agreement. Most contemporary accounts recognize the usefulness of distinguishing three types of units, each corresponding to an established sense of the notion “word.” Phonological words or wordforms are simple sequences of phonemes (or graphemes). Grammatical words are phonological words with a morphosyntactic interpretation. Lexical words or lexemes correspond to the units that would be entered in a dictionary. Each of these units has a characteristic role in the morphology of a language, and each determines a corresponding notion of “word count.” Yet the partial overlap between these notions also raises descriptive and analytic issues that need to be clarified.

Grammatical and Phonological Words

The notion of “word” proposed in Bloomfield (1926) provides the classic definition of the “grammatical” word. Bloomfield’s notion of a “minimimum free form” also comes closest to capturing the sense of word as it is normally understood.

A minimum free form is a word . A word is thus a form which may be uttered alone (with meaning) but cannot be analyzed into parts that may (all of them) be uttered alone (with meaning). (p. 156)

On this interpretation of “word,” the three homophonous occurrences of cut in (1) are different words, realizing the preterite, past participle, and infinitive forms of the verb cut . This is the notion of word that is most relevant to measures of token frequency, which treat each occurrence of a form as a separate “token.”

(1) a. He cut the flowers. ( preterite ) b. He has cut the flowers. ( past participle ) c. He will cut the flowers. ( infinitive )

The Bloomfieldian notion of a “minimimum free form (with meaning)” also captures the sense of “word” that is mainly relevant for (extended) “word and paradigm” models ( Robins, 1959 ; Anderson, 1992 ) and for other “word-based” approaches ( Blevins, 2006 ). As Robins (1959 , p. 120) puts it, the word in this sense is “a grammatical abstraction,” but it is a maximally useful abstraction.

The word is a more stable and solid focus of grammatical relations than the component morpheme by itself. Put another way, grammatical statements are abstractions, but they are more profitably abstracted from words as wholes than from individual morphemes. (p. 128)

In most languages, the abstraction of words is facilitated by cues that enhance their perceptual salience. Open-class items are often subject to a minimum word constraint, whether measured in terms of moras, syllables, or metrical feet, and there is experimental evidence that speakers exploit these constraints in the segmentation of continuous speech ( Norris, McQueen, Cutler, & Butterfield, 1997 ). Words often define the positions at which stress, pitch, or other suprasegmental features are realized, and word edges maybe marked by processes, such as boundary lengthening ( Bybee, 2001 ). The perceptual salience of words is further enhanced by the fact that words (unlike subword units, such as phonemes or morphemes) may stand on their own as independent utterances. In addition, if there is any basis to notions like “the one-word stage” ( Dromi, 1987 ), it would appear that the word is the canonical utterance during early stages of language acquisition. As one might expect, the functional load of individual cues varies across languages, reflecting general differences in phonological systems, so that no single cue identifies words cross-linguistically. This leads, as Robins (1959) acknowledges, to discrepancies between the notion of “word” relevant for the description of grammatical relations, and the sense of “word” that is marked phonetically.

The qualification “grammatical” reflects this contrast between the Bloomfieldian word and “phonological” (or, by extension, “orthographic”) words. A more concise term for minimal free forms without a fixed meaning or function is “wordform.” In example (1) above, the phonological word cut realizes the preterite, past participle, and infinitive forms of the verb cut . The same wordform realizes other forms of cut, as well as the corresponding noun in he has a cut .

Wordforms are, by definition, the units that are most clearly demarcated in the speech stream by phonetic cues. As sequences of phonemes (or graphemes), they are also the most straightforward units to distinguish and count. Although wordforms are not traditionally of significant grammatical interest, they are central to many current frequency-based models. In accounts that treat phonetic reduction as a frequency effect, it is often wordform frequency that is taken to determine the rate or extent of reduction. Hence in the frequency-based model of Bybee (2010) , the operative notion of “words” corresponds to “wordforms.”

A robust finding that has emerged recently in quantitative studies of phonetic reduction is that high-frequency words undergo more change or change at a faster rate than low-frequency words, (p. 20) [S]ubstantial evidence has recently been reported showing that phonetic reduction occurs earlier and to a greater extent in high-frequency words than in low-frequency ones...[I]f we postulate that reduction occurs online as words are used, then words that are used more often are exposed to reduction processes more often and thus undergo change at a faster rate. (p. 37)

Although grammatical words maybe realized by single wordforms, the correlation between these units is often disrupted by phonological or syntactic processes. Cliticization creates sequences in which multiple grammatical words correspond to a single phonological word. In example (2a), the contracted phonological word he’s corresponds to the grammatical words he and has . Separable particles illustrate the converse case, in which a single grammatical word such as German ablehnen “reject” is realized by the wordforms lehnen and ab in (2b).

(2) a. He’s cut the flowers. b. Sie lehnen den Vorschlag ab. they “lean” the proposal “away” “They reject the proposal.”

Cues and correlations

The divergence between grammatical and phonological word has led some scholars to question the status and even the usefulness of the notion “word” in the analysis of a grammatical system. 1 Although this question has attracted renewed attention in the recent literature, it articulates a longstanding criticism of traditional word-based approaches. Thus the difficulty of demarcating words is taken by Bloomfield (1914) as evidence for the primacy of the sentence.

it has long been recognized that the first and original datum of language is the sentence—that the individual word is the product of a theoretical reflection that ought not to be taken for granted. (p. 38)

One need not dispute the epistemological priority of utterances or downplay the discrepancies between phonological and grammatical words to realize that these discrepancies arise precisely because there are phonetic cues that, with varying degrees of reliability, mark word boundaries or otherwise guide the segmentation of utterances into words. The existence of mismatches should not obscure the fact that the two notions of “word” overlap, at least partially, in many languages, and that this overlap permits speakers to isolate grammatical words. Although grammatical words may be imperfectly demarcated, subword units—including, significantly, roots—are rarely if ever cued at all by phonetic properties. There is no discrepancy between the “grammatical morpheme” and the “phonological morpheme” for the simple reason that there is no such thing as a “phonological morpheme.” Hence the objection that grammatical words are not reliably and invariantly cued in the speech stream provides no motivation for shifting the focus of morphological analysis onto units smaller than the word (such as morphemes), since these units require an even greater degree of abstraction from the speech signal. The observation that words are abstractions just falls under the broader generalization that all linguistic units smaller than utterances are abstracted from larger sequences of connected speech.

It is also possible that much of the debate about the alignment of grammatical and phonological words is fundamentally misconceived. Nearly all linguistic approaches to this issue frame the problem in terms of specifying reliable and cross-linguistically valid cues for demarcating grammatical words. Underlying these approaches is the assumption that there should be some set of necessary and sufficient conditions for defining words in a given language, or across languages. However, in parallel to theoretical and descriptive studies, there exists a large and diverse psycholinguistic and computational literature on word segmentation and recognition. This literature includes the work on identifying “uniqueness points” in Marslen-Wilson and Welsh (1978) and Marslen-Wilson and Tyler (1980) , neural network-based predictive models ( Elman 1990 ), and statistical models of word segmentation ( Goldwater, Griffiths, & Johnson, 2009 ). The models of word segmentation developed in these studies are based on the observation that entropy (roughly, uncertainty about the segments that follow) declines as more of a word is processed, then peaks again at word boundaries. These sorts of prediction-based approaches suggest that the search for invariant cues may be futile and that the observable phonetic properties that have been assumed to be definitional are secondary cues for what is an essentially statistical notion.

Observations about predictability at word boundaries are consistent with two different kinds of assumptions about what constitutes a word: either a word is a unit that is statistically independent of other units, or it is a unit that helps to predict other units (but to a lesser degree than the beginning of a word predicts its end). ( Goldwater et al., 2009 , p. 22)

From this perspective, the phonological word might correspond to phonetic sequences between entropy “peaks,” with phonetic cues representing one source of entropy reduction. The correlation between phonological and grammatical words could be similarly probabilistic rather than defined by discrete criteria.

Grammatical and phonological words represent two kinds of word tokens. In examples (1) and (2), there are four occurrences of the wordform cut , corresponding to two occurrences of the past participle cut , and one occurrence each of the preterite and infinitive. Given that grammatical words are defined as wordforms with fixed meanings, their identification depends on assumptions about the parts of speech and morphosyntactic features in a language. For example, a classification that recognizes distinct perfect and passive participles in English will treat the two occurrences of cut in (3) as distinct grammatical words. On an account that recognizes a unitary past participle in perfect and passive constructions, there will be two occurrences of a past participle cut . More generally, the identification of grammatical words is always defined relative to a grammatical analysis (or, in the case of corpora, a set of grammatical “tags”).

(3) a. He has cut the flowers. ( perfect/past participle ) b. The flowers have been cut. ( passive/past participle )

Lexemes and Lemmas

Insofar as occurrences of grammatical words are tokens of a common word “type,” they imply another, more abstract notion of “word.” This notion of “word type” usually goes under the name lexeme . Matthews (1972 , p. 160) characterizes the lexeme in this sense as “the lexical element...to which the forms in [a] paradigm as a whole...can be said to belong.” 2 In a later discussion of the same point, Matthews (1991 , p. 26) suggests that a lexeme is “a lexical unit and is entered in dictionaries as the fundamental element in the lexicon of language.” Lexemes are thus reminiscent of the lexicographer’s notion of a lemma, which is the citation form of an item or the headword under which it is listed in a dictionary. 3 The connection between these notions is reinforced by the fact that lexemes are conventionally represented by the citation form of an item in small caps (i.e., by the lemma of the item). However, whereas a lemma is a distinguished form, for example, the infinitive cut , a lexeme is a set of grammatical words .

The contrast between wordforms, grammatical words, and lexemes resolves a systematic ambiguity in the use of the term “word” as applied to the pure form cut , the preterite cut , and the lexeme cut . Not all items give rise to a full three-way split, since different notions of word may coincide in particular cases. There is usually at least a partial correlation between grammatical words and wordforms. For closed-class categories, the distinction between lexeme and grammatical word may not be especially relevant or useful, since a preposition or conjunction will usually be associated with a single grammatical word. Much the same will be true of open-class categories in an isolating language, such as Vietnamese, if nouns and verbs are represented by single grammatical words .

Paradigms and Families

Determining which grammatical words are part of a lexeme also introduces the kinds of issues that confront lexicographers when they attempt to distinguish primary “word entries” from “word senses” in a language. These issues bear in a direct way on the delineation of inflectional and derivational processes. From a traditional perspective, inflectional processes are said to define (or, more generally, relate) forms of a lexeme (i.e., grammatical words), whereas derivation derives (or relates) lexemes. It is typically assumed that the grammatical words that comprise a lexeme must belong to the same word class, preserve a core lexical semantics and argument structure, and even share a set of “intrinsic” features that are invariant for a given item. Inflectional processes are accordingly regarded as monotonic processes that add features to the invariant properties that can be associated with stems. In contrast, processes that alter class or argument structure are treated as derivational, since these changes create new lexemes.

Yet traditional descriptive practices are not entirely consistent with this split, as noted in a number of recent works, including Booij (1996) , Haspelmath (1996) , and Spencer (1999) . In particular, conjugational paradigms often include categorial hybrids, such as participles, which exhibit adjectival properties, and gerunds or masdars, which exhibit nominal properties. The inclusion of passive participles also introduces argument structure variation into conjugational paradigms. This inconsistency undermines the coherence of the notion “lexeme,” along with associated conceptions of the inflection/derivation divide, if, as these authors assume, the notions “lexeme” and “inflectional paradigm” are taken to coincide.

Alternatively, one can interpret traditional descriptions as inexplicit rather than inconsistent, and reconstruct lexemes as sets of forms that occupy an intermediate position between inflectional paradigms and morphological families. This interpretation has the additional benefit of resolving a systematic ambiguity in the use of the term “paradigm.” Nouns and adjectives typically have a unified set of forms, inflected for case, number, definiteness, and so forth, which can be consolidated within a single declensional paradigm. In contrast, the inflected forms of verbs often exhibit a suborganization into sets of forms that share the same tense/aspect/mood features and vary in their agreement features. The term “paradigm” is applied to these smaller sets when one refers to the present indicative paradigm of French parler “to speak” and to the full set of inflected forms when one refers to the paradigm of parler toute court. To resolve this ambiguity, the term “paradigm” can be reserved for the first, smaller set of forms and the term “lexeme” applied to the complete set of inflected forms. Hence the present indicative paradigm of parler comprises a subset of the lexeme parler . The third member in this concentric classification is the “morphological family,” which contains a lexeme, such as parler, and any lexemes-related parler .

A coherent split between inflection and derivation can then be maintained by assigning related forms to the appropriate form class associated with an item. Consider first the challenges presented by categorial hybrids. If active participles are treated as neutralizing the categorial distinction between verbs and adjectives, they can be included in the lexeme of a verb provided that the lexeme is defined as a set of grammatical words with noncontrastive (rather than identical) values for word class and other intrinsic features. A similar analysis extends to other elements that neutralize rather than alter word class distinctions. However, a derived deverbal nominal will, irrespective of how productively it is formed, comprise a separate lexeme in the morphological family of a verb.

The classification of passive forms likewise depends on the treatment of argument structure. Passive forms will be included in the lexeme of a verb if the forms in a lexeme need only share a common logical (or thematic) argument structure. If the words that make up a lexeme must also have common surface valence (subcategorization) demands, then active and passive words will belong to different lexemes but again to a common morphological family. Either choice restores coherence to a traditional perspective in which lexemes occupy a position between inflectional paradigms and morphological families. 4 A lexeme such as cut will contain a set of grammatical words, including finite and nonfinite verb forms. However, cut can itself be located within a morphological family that includes the predictable agentive nominal lexeme cutter, which contains the singular grammatical word cutter and its plural counterpart cutters .

Endocentric and Exocentric Processes

Given a systematic contrast between lexemes and morphological families, the somewhat overloaded contrast between “inflection” and “derivation” can be construed in terms of a distinction between endocentric or “within-lexeme” processes, and exocentric or “cross-lexeme” processes. Characteristics that correlate with the endocentric-exocentric split need not then enter into the definition of this contrast. The fact that inflection operates over a closed, relatively uniform space makes inflectional processes interpredictable and productive. From the class of an open-class item, it is usually possible to determine the features that are distinctive for the class and predict the number of forms of the item; apart from irregular items, paradigms and lexemes are comparable in size and structure within a word or inflection class. 5 The closed, uniform feature space of an inflectional system thus defines a “grid” populated by interpredictable forms. 6

The uniform organization of inflectional paradigms contrasts starkly with the more variable structure exhibited by families of derivational forms. Exocentric processes that can change word class, valence, or other intrinsic properties clearly do not define a finite set of forms within a uniform feature space. From the class of an item, one cannot in general predict the number and type of derivational formations in which it occurs. Given a list of derivational processes active in a language, it is of course possible to assign a uniform family of “potential” forms to all of the members of a word class. Yet the uniformity achieved is deceptive, because it collapses a critical distinction between those forms that are established in a language and those that are merely possible in principle. The status of derivational forms that are possible but unattested is similar to that of acceptable phoneme sequences that “merely” lack a conventionalized meaning. Moreover, defining expanded derivational paradigms does not make it any easier for a speaker to predict the derivational formations that are attested and in use within a language. The issue is perhaps clearer in connection with compounds. Of the infinitely many possible noun compounds in English only a comparatively small number are established, and a speaker cannot predict the set of established compounds containing an item from the item itself.

These contrasts between inflection and derivation are reflected in the ways that morphological systems are described and analyzed. It is rare for an individual inflected form to be described as established on its own. The availability of a form correlates instead with the productivity of a whole pattern. Conversely, notions like “morphological gap,” “suppletion,” and even, to a large degree, “syncretism” are mainly or exclusively applied to inflectional paradigms. Derivational families do not usually have “gaps” because they do not define implicational relations over a closed and uniform space that give rise to expectations about the existence of specific forms, even where the shape would be predictable. Suppletion can likewise only arise where there are definite assumptions about the shape of particular members of a form set. Whereas inflectional paradigms generate strong assumptions of this nature, derivational families do not. Syncretism presupposes a similar structure, as syncretic patterns imply the existence of independent cells that can be associated with fully or partially identical forms. But the notion of a derivational cell is not clearly defined in the absence of features that specify a morphosyntactic grid within which to place the cells.

Although there is a strong correlation between endocentricity and predictability, it is neither possible (nor desirable) to define inflectional/endocentric processes in terms of predictability or productivity. It is possible for endocentric processes to be unpredictable and for exocentric processes to be predictable. Neither of these patterns raises practical or theoretical difficulties. An unpredictable inflected form is usually described as sporadic or lexically idiosyncratic and must, in the general case, be learned on an item-by-item basis. The “past subjunctive” forms of German fall into this category, as do comitative, abessive, or instructive case forms in Finnish. Exocentric processes can likewise be highly predictable while defining new lexemes, with their own inflectional paradigms.

For example, agentive nominals in - er can be formed for virtually any verb in English, so that the predictability of agentive nominals in - er approaches that of inflected verb forms. Nevertheless, there are two important differences between an agentive nominal, such as walker, and an inflected form, such as walks . The first is that walker is associated with its own nominal paradigm, containing singular walker and plural walkers . Treating the process that defines -er nominals as inflectional/endocentric would make the lexeme walk a categorially incoherent collection of inflected verb and noun forms. The second difference is that inflected forms are associated with a specific paradigm within a lexeme, whereas derivational forms tend to be associated with the lexeme. This distinction can be illustrated even in the simple conjugational system of English. Whereas walks is associated with the present indicative paradigm, walker is not the agentive nominal of any particular paradigm but of the verb lexeme walk .

Implications

The contrast between wordforms, grammatical words, and lexemes clarifies the principal senses of the notion “word.” The distinction between paradigms, lexemes, and families in turn defines the paradigmatic organization relevant to the split between endocentric and exocentric morphological processes. The resulting classification reconstructs a traditional perspective in a form that is compatible with, and even to some degree supported by, recent psycholinguistic studies.

Traditional claims about the “psychological reality” of words and paradigmatic structure receive a measure of support from experimental studies of frequency effects on lexical processing. A number of studies have shown that the frequency of inflected forms and the size of derivational families have a robust effect on lexical processing. One line of research has investigated the correlation between response latencies for inflected forms and the token frequency of the elements of their inflectional paradigms ( Taft, 1979 ; Baayen, Lieber, & Schreuder, 1997 ; Hay, 2001 ). A second line of research has demonstrated that the processing of an item is facilitated by the semantically transparent items in its morphological family and inhibited by the semantically opaque items ( Schreuder & Baayen, 1997 ; de Jong, 2002 ; Moscoso del Prado Martín, 2003 ). A third line of research attempts to provide a single information-theoretic measure that subsumes the token-frequency effects relevant to inflectional paradigms and the type-frequency effects relevant to morphological families ( Kostić, Marković, & Baucal, 2003 ; Moscoso del Prado Martín, Kostić, & Baayen, 2004 ). More recent studies ( Milin, Filipović Ðurdjević, & Moscoso del Prado Martín, 2009 ; Milin, Kuperman, Kostić, & Baayen, 2009 ) have also shown that speaker responses are conditioned in part by the relation between relative frequencies. In particular, speakers appear to be sensitive to the relation between the probability distribution of the inflected forms of a lexical item and the probability distribution of exponents within the inflection class to which the item belongs. The more that these distributions diverge, the longer are the response latencies and the higher are the error rates in lexical decision tasks.

The traditional contrast between inflection and derivation may likewise contribute to an explanation for the observation that the processing of inflectional forms is more sensitive to token frequencies, whereas the processing of derivational formations is more sensitive to the type frequencies ( Baayen et al., 1997 ). The relevant assumption is just that members of a given paradigm or inflection class have a comparable number of forms, whereas the size of derivational families may vary by orders of magnitude. Hence, if the type frequencies for inflected forms are relatively constant, only token frequencies will be distinctive.

Experimental methods and results are described greater detail in later sections. However, two points are worth mentioning here. The first is that all available research suggests that speakers are sensitive to frequency and distributional relations, information that is almost completely absent from theoretical models. The second is that the processing of a form appears to be influenced (whether facilitated or inhibited) by related forms, an effect that conflicts with the theoretical idealization that individual forms are defined in isolation. Morphological family effects seem to indicate that related forms are not only present as elements of a speaker’s mental lexicon but that they are in some sense “activated” in the processing of a given form. These effects appear particularly compatible with models that link families of wordforms into networks of elements with shared formal, grammatical, and/or semantic properties ( Bybee, 1985 , 2010 ).

Word Structure

The models of morphology that grew out the classical Greco-Roman tradition recognized no grammatical unit intervening between sounds and words, and did not even see the need to isolate roots ( Law, 1998 , p. 112). However, since at least the early Neogrammarian period ( Schleicher, 1859 ) the study of morphology has been centrally concerned with the morphotactic organization of words. The recurrent structural patterns exhibited by words in turn defined the familiar models of morphological typology initially proposed in Sapir (1921) . The following sections summarize this organization, from patterns through types.

Morphotactic patterns fall broadly into affixal (concatenative) and nonaffixal (nonconcatenative) classes. Affixal patterns are characterized by the presence (or absence) of a formative. Nonaffixal patterns may be distinguished by a stress pattern, a tonal melody, a length contrast, an accentual pattern, or another change in the properties of a segment or sequence of segments. 7

The most common type of affixation is suffixation. Thus the pattern represented by the suffixation of - s in English books is more common than the prefixation of re- in reread , and both are more prevalent than other types of affixation. Infixation tends to arise as a prosodically conditioned variant of prefixation or suffixation. Hence, few infixes do not have prefixal or suffixal variants. In English, additional emphasis can be placed on expletives, such as bloody , by interposing them between two prosodic feet of the noun that they are modifying: bloody unlikely~un-bloody-likely ( McMillan, 1980 ; McCarthy, 1982 ). In Tagalog, the “actor focus” marker um is prefixed to vowel-initial verb root alís in (4a), but infixed after the initial consonant of the consonant-initial root bása in (4b).

(4) Infixation in Tagalog ( Aspillera 1981 , p. 45f.) a. alís ~ um alís “to leave” b. bása ~ b um ása “to read”

Circumfixation (or parafixation) is the most sparsely attested type of affixation, and many cases invite a reanalysis in terms of successive affixation. In German, for example, the formation of the perfect participle is sometimes described in terms of circumfixation. A regular verb like sagen “say” has a basic stem sag- and a perfect participle gesagt . However, there is no clear motivation for a circumfix ge-...-t , given that the preterite series is based on a stem sagt , so that gesagt can be formed by prefixing ge- to this stem ( Blevins, 2003 ). More plausible cases of circumfixation are illustrated by the Georgian superlative marker u...esi in (5a) and the Chuckchee negative marker e... ke in (5b).

(5) Circumfixation in Georgian and Chuckchee a. q’ru ‘deaf ~ u -q’ru- esi “the most deaf” ( Tschenkéli, 1958 , p. 225) b. tejkev- ə k “fight” ~ e -tejkev- ke -it- ə k “not to fight” ( Comrie, 1981 , p. 247)

Reduplication is sometimes regarded as a type of parasitic affixation that copies all or part of a base form. The future in Tagalog is formed by reduplicating the first syllable of a root, for example, a in al.ís in (6a), but bá in bd.sa in (6b).

(6) Reduplication in Tagalog ( Aspillera, 1981 ) a. alís ~ a alís “leave. fut” b. bása ~ ba bása “read. fut”

Nonaffixal Patterns

Nonaffixal patterns are fairly heterogeneous, and correspond to any phonetic contrast that is stable and discriminable enough to be morphologized. Subtraction (or truncation) involves the loss of a segment, usually under prosodic conditions. The formation of Hidatsa imperatives in (7a) illustrates a subtractive pattern cited by Harris (1942 , p. 110). The Papago forms in (7b) provide another familiar case, in which perfectives lack the final -m of the imperfectives.

(7) Truncation in Hidatsa and Papago a. cicic “he jumped” ~ cic “jump!” ika.c “he looked” ~ ika “look!” b. him ~ hi “walking,” hihim ~ hihi “walking. pl ” ( Zepeda, 1983 )

In patterns of ablaut (or vowel modification), a morphological contrast is signaled by a change in vowel quality. In German, present, preterite, and participial forms of strong verbs exhibit the residual ablaut patterns in (8a) and (8b). In the Lezgian examples in (8c) and (8d), the inessive form of a noun “is marked by lowering the final vowel of the oblique stem” ( Haspelmath, 1993 ).

(8) Ablaut in German and Lezgian a. singe “sing. 1sg.pres ” ~ sang “ 1sg.pret ” ~ gesungen “ part .” b. nehme “take. 1sg . pres ” ~ nahm “ 1sg.pret ” ~ genommen “ part .” c. čarxú “rock” ~ čarxá “ ines.sg ” d. arčí “paper” ~ čarčé “ ines.sg ”

The phenomenon of morphological gradation is illustrated by Estonian, which exhibits a three-way quantity distinction between Q1 (short), Q2 (long), and Q3 (overlong) syllables. In the morphological system, the contrast between Q2 and Q3 distinguishes weak and strong (quantitative) grade. In the class of nouns represented in (9), the genitive and partitive singular forms differ solely in grade: a weak genitive singular contrasts with a strong partitive singular. 8

(9) Grade in Estonian declensions ( Mürk 1997 ; Blevins, 2008 ) a. luku “lock. gen.sg ” (Q2) ~ ‘lukku ‘lock. part.sg (Q3) b. kooli “school. gen.sg ” (Q2) ~ ‘kooli “school. part.sg ” (Q3)

Although the contrasts in these examples cannot be keyed to segmental formatives, the elements of each of the patterns can be associated with a particular morphological function or meaning. Even this constancy is missing in exchange patterns, in which a morphological contrast is encoded by a symmetrical or “reversable” opposition. In Spanish, the contrast between indicative and subjunctive mood is marked by a process of a~e “vowel reversal” ( Matthews, 1991 , p. 198) illustrated in (10a) and (10b). The contrast between the Estonian partitive singulars and plurals in (10c) and (10d) is encoded by an i ~ e alternation.

(10) Exchange patterns in Spanish and Estonian a. compra “buy.3 sg.ind ” ~ compre “buy.3s g.sub ” b. come “eat.3 sg.ind ” ~ coma “eat. 3s g.sub ” c. ‘kooli “school. part.sg ” ~ ‘koole “school. part.pl ” d. ‘lille “flower. part.sg ” ~ lilli “flower. part.pl .”

Dinka exhibits a similar pattern of “variable interpredictability,” in which suprasegmental tonal contrasts distinguish absolutive from oblique case.

The oblique is distinguished from the absolutive in virtually all monosyllabic nouns that have a short vowel...and in most disyllabic nouns with the prefix à- and a short root vowel...The rule for such nouns is that if the absolutive has a low root tone...then the oblique gets a falling root tone, and if the absolutive has a high or falling root tone...then the oblique gets a low root tone. ( Andersen , 2002 , p. 9)

The near-minimal contrast between àjìt ~ àjît and àrêw ~ àrèw in Figure 9.1 illustrates the alternation between low-falling and falling-low tonal melodies.

methathesis represents an extreme version of a nonaffixal strategy in which a morphological contrast is expressed by a process that rearranges the relative order of segments or formatives. In Rotuman ( Churchward, 1940 ; Blevins & Garrett 1999 , p. 510) CV metathesis distinguishes the CV “complete phase” (corresponding roughly to a definite form) from the VC “incomplete phase.”

(11) a. sesé va ~ sesé av “erroneous” b. tí ko ~ tí ok “flesh”

Morphological types

Although different patterns and strategies often predominate in different parts of a language, it is common to classify languages by positioning them relative to a number of ideal morphological types. At one extreme are languages of the isolating type, in which each morpheme functions as a grammatical word. Modern English approaches this ideal, at least insofar as inflectional morphology is concerned, although Southeast Asian languages, such as Vietnamese, are usually regarded as paradigm examples of the isolating type. At the other extreme are agglutinative languages, in which words consist of a sequence of discrete formatives, each of which realizes a single property. Many inflectionally complex languages contain structures that conform to this ideal. For example, a Finnish noun, such as taloissansa “in their houses,” can be assigned the agglutinative analysis in (12a). However, in few languages is this transparency maintained through the entire morphology. In Finnish, agglutination breaks down already in the nominative plural talot “houses” in (12b), which lacks the distinct plural marker -i- and the discrete case marker found in other forms.

(12) Agglutination in Finnish ( Karlsson, 1999 ) a. talo-i-ssa-nsa “house- pl-ines-3pl.poss ” b. talo-t “house- nom.pl ”

The “fusion” of nominative case and plural number in the single suffix -t in (12a) is—like the realization of third person and plural by the ending -nsa in (12a)—a salient characteristic of languages of the ( in)flectional type. Fusional patterns are ubiquitous within Indo-European languages, and particularly well represented in Latin and Ancient Greek. As Matthews (1972 , p. 132ff.) shows, the Latin verb re:ksisti: “you had ruled” ( rexisti in the standard pedagogical orthography) exhibits the many-many relations between features and forms displayed in Figure 9.2 . The ending -ti: exhibits a fusional pattern, realizing perfective aspect, second person and singular number. The perfective feature exhibits a converse “fis-sional” pattern, as it is realized by each of the formatives -s-, -is- , and -ti: .

Morphological analysis of Latin re:ksisti: . Adapted from Matthews (1972) .

Morphological analysis of Greek elelýkete . Adapted from Matthews (1991) .

Nor is this an extreme or unrepresentative example. Classical Greek exhibits even more tangled feature-form associations, as illustrated by the verb elelykete “you had unfastened,” in Figure 9.3 below. Just as re:ksisti: occupies the second person perfective active cell of rego “rule,” elelykete realizes the second person past perfective indicative active cell in the paradigm of lyo “unfasten.” But as Matthews (1972 , 1991 ) observes, the scattered realization of aspect and voice confounds any attempt to impose an agglutinative structure on these forms. Simpler cases of extended exponence are illustrated by the combination of a suppletive future stem ir- and a future ending -ai in French irai “go.1 sg .” More “exuberant” patterns are likewise attested in Batsbi, as shown by Harris (2009) . 9

Morphological analysis

It may be useful to bring this summary of the linguistic properties of words to a close with a brief discussion of some of the analytical issues that arise in assigning wordforms a morphotactic structure or associating them with sets of paradigmatic alternatives. Interestingly, the interaction between paradigmatic alternatives and morphotactic structure plays a significant role in morphological systems. The implicit “competition” between alternatives gives rise to a type of “paradigmatic deduction” that allows forms to be interpreted based on the absence of a marker. Noun paradigms in English provide a simple example. Plural nouns in English are marked by the suffix /z/ (represented orthographically as -s). There is, however, no marking of singular number, and none is needed, given that a singular noun is unambiguously identified by the lack of a plural marker. A zero morph adds no information to what speakers can already deduce from the absence of any realized exponent. Similar patterns are even more typical of more intricate paradigms. As Anderson (1992 , p. 87) notes, Georgian verbal paradigms provide a striking illustration of the fact that that “information may sometimes be conveyed not by constituents that are present in the structure of a given word, but precisely by the fact that certain other material is absent .”

Cases of what are sometimes termed “gestalt exponence” likewise highlight the grammatical and semantic contribution of the morphotactic arrangement of formatives. The patterns of cumulative and extended exponence illustrated in the section on morphological types, together with the paradigmatic deduction described in English and Georgian, suggest that the properties of words are not, at least in any straightforward or direct way, reducible to the properties of their parts. A range of patterns cannot be made to conform to an agglutinative ideal in which the grammatical meaning of a word consists of discrete units of meaning that correspond to subword formatives. Instead, morphological systems tend to reuse common parts in different contexts in such a way that the arrangement of these parts contributes a “constructional” meaning. Expanding on the discussion of gradation in (9) will clarify how combinations of elements can have distinctive meanings within a language. The first four columns in Table 9.1 contain the singular grammatical case forms of nouns that exhibit productive “weakening” gradation in Estonian. The nominative forms of these nouns consist of a strong (Q3) stem, identified by the double consonant -kk . The remaining forms consist of a strong stem, or a weak stem in -k , followed by one of the “theme vowels” a, e, i , and u . 10

Let us consider the locus of the property “partitive singular.” This property can only be associated with subword units if the forms in Table 9.1 are analyzable into smaller meaningful parts. The partitive singulars of this class contain two “recurrent partials”: a strong stem and a theme vowel. Thus sukka can be analyzed as sukk + a, kukke as kukk + e, pukki as pukk + i , and lukku as lukk + u . But partitive case cannot be associated either with strong stems or with theme vowels in isolation. The strong stems sukk, kukk, pukk , and lukk cannot be analyzed as partitive, because these same stems realize the nominative singular when they occur without a theme vowel, and also underlie the second “short” illative singular forms. Partitive case also cannot be associated with the theme vowels, because the same vowels occur in the genitive and illative singular forms.

Hence partitive case is an irreducibly word-level feature that is realized by the combination of a strong stem and a theme vowel. This type of gestalt exponence or “constructional” exponence ( Booij, 2005 ) is difficult to describe if stems and theme vowels are represented separately. Because the grammatical meanings associated with strong stems and theme vowels are context-dependent, these elements cannot be assigned discrete meanings that “add up” to partitive singular when they are combined. From a traditional perspective, this context-dependence underscores the difference between “analyzability” and morphemic “decomposition.” An individual wordform is often analyzable into parts that recur elsewhere in its inflectional paradigm or in the morphological system at large. But these parts may function solely to differentiate larger forms, so that the minimal parts that distinguish a pair of wordforms cannot be associated with the difference in grammatical meaning between the wordforms. To return to the patterns in Table 9.1 , the theme vowel -u distinguishes the partitive singular lukku “lock” from the nominative singular lukk . In isolation, however, the vowel -u neither realizes a specific case value nor expresses “the grammatical difference” between nominative and partitive. Exactly the same is true of the grade contrast between partitive singular lukku and genitive singular luku .

These examples illustrate some of the fundamental challenges that arise in decomposing wordforms into their component parts. Given a set of wordforms, it is often possible to segment them into roots or stems and derivational and inflectional exponents. Yet disassembling a word into its parts raises chronic and even recalcitrant problems in many languages. Perhaps the most extreme analytical problem is associated with the patterns of stem syncretism that Matthews (1972) calls “Priscianic” or “parasitic” and Aronoff (1994) later terms “morphomic.” A pair of Latin examples discussed in Matthews (1991) are summarized in the quotations below. The first case involves a correspondence between present active infinitives and imperfect subjunctives. The second example concerns the relation between past passive and future active participles.

For any Verb, however irregular it may be in other respects, the Present Infinitive always predicts the Imperfect Subjunctive. For the Verb “to flower,” florere → florerem; for the irregular Verb “to be,” esse → essem, and so forth without exception. (p. 195) There are a few exceptions; but, in general, if the stem of the Past Participle is x , no matter how irregular it may be, that of the Future Participle is x with -ūr- added. (p. 200)

The challenge posed by these patterns is that stems such as florere and esse (and past participle stems like amāt “to love”) are non–meaning-bearing units. Hence, once isolated, they cannot be assigned features and meanings that they contribute to determine the features and meanings of the larger units that they underlie. The difficulties that arise in attempting to assign these stems grammatical meanings merely highlights the fact that the value of these units of forms resides solely in the predictions that they sanction about other forms.

The descriptive value of form-based predictions is illustrated by the kind of implicational network that they define within the paradigm of lukk “ lock” in Table 9.2 . The nominative singular lukk underlies the partitive singular lukku , which is in turn identical to the short illative singular lukku and underlies the partitive plural lukkusid and the genitive plural lukkude . The genitive singular luku underlies the nominative lukud and the illative through comitative singular, whereas the genitive plural lukkude underlies the illative through the comitative plural.

In sum, the word occupies a central position in the morphotactic, paradigmatic, and implicational organization of a language. The traditional assignment of words to paradigms, lexemes, and families complements the analysis of wordforms into smaller parts, ultimately reflecting interrelated “outward-looking” and “inward-looking” perspectives on the most basic grammatical unit of a language.

See the papers in Dixon and Aikhenvald (2002) for extended discussion of this issue.

An earlier use of “lexeme” is found in Hockett (1958 , p. 169ff.), who uses the term to designate sequences that always occur as grammatical forms in a context where they are not part of any larger unit that also invariably occurs as a grammatical form. This usage is now largely obsolete.

There is an alternative interpretation of the term “lemma” still in circulation. In psycholinguistic models of speech production, lemmas are often construed as abstract conceptual entries that represent “the nonphonological part of an item’s lexical information” ( Levelt, 1989 ).

Moreover, on either alternative, “morphosemantic” alternations ( Sadler & Spencer, 1998 ) like causativization will relate distinct lexemes within a larger morphological family.

This predictability underlies the descriptive success of the realization-based models of inflection set out in Matthews (1972 , 1991 ), Anderson (1992) , Aronoff (1994) , and Stump (2001) .

The patterns exhibited by this network provide the analogical base for exemplar-based models of both the traditional ( Paul, 1920 ) and modern ( Bybee, 2010 ) variety.

Some theories of morphology regard affixal and nonaffixal patterns as different in character ( Carstairs-McCarthy, 1994 , 2005 ). Other approaches argue that “there is no theoretically significant difference between concatenative and non-concatenative inflection” ( Stump, 2005 , p. 284).

Since the Q3 quantity of partitive singular kooli is not marked orthographically (as by the consonant doubling in lukku ), Q3 is marked diacritically by a single quotation mark.

The Polysynthetic patterns found in many Amerindian languages represent an orthogonal dimension of morphological variation, as these patterns essentially exhibit a distinctive division of grammatical labor. In many of the familiar languages of Europe and Asia, lexical meaning is expressed by roots or stems, and grammatical meanings by affixes and other types of exponents.

The choice of theme vowel is a lexical property of an Estonian noun, and is not conditioned by phonological properties of the stem or by morphosyntactic features of the noun.

Andersen, T. ( 2002 ). Case inflection and nominal head marking in Dinka.   Journal of African Languages and Linguistics , 23 , 1–30.

Google Scholar

Anderson, S. R. ( 1992 ). A-morphous morphology . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Preview

Aronoff, M. ( 1994 ). Morphology by itself: Stems and inflectional classes . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Aspillera, P. S. ( 1981 ). Basic Tagalog . Manila: M & L Licudine Enterprises.

Baayen, R. H. , Lieber, R. , & Schreuder, R. ( 1997 ). The morphological complexity of simple nouns.   Linguistics , 35 , 861–877.

Blevins, J. , & Garrett, A. ( 1999 ). The origins of consonant-vowel metathesis.   Language , 74 , 508–556.

Blevins, J. P. ( 2003 ). Stems and paradigms.   Language , 79 , 737–767.

Blevins, J. P. ( 2006 ). Word-based morphology.   Journal of Linguistics , 42 , 531–573.

Blevins, J. P. ( 2008 ). Declension classes in Estonian.   Linguistica Uralica , 44 , 241–267.

Bloomfield, L. ( 1914 ). Sentence and word.   Transactions of the American Philological Society , 45 , 65–75. Reprinted in Hockett (1970), 38–46.

Bloomfield, L. ( 1926 ). A set of postulates for the science of language.   Language , 2 , 153–164. Reprinted in M. Joos (1957), Readings in linguistics I . Chicago: University of Chicago Press (26–37).

Bloomfield, L. ( 1933 ). Language . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Booij, G. ( 1996 ). Inherent verses contextual inflection and the split morphology hypothesis. In G. Booij & J. van Marie (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1995 (pp. 1–16). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Booij, G. ( 2005 ). Compounding and derivation: Evidence for constructional morphology. In W. U. Dressier , P. Kastovsky , & F. Rainer (Eds.), Morphology and its demarcations (pp. 109–132). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Bybee, J. L. ( 1985 ). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form . Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Bybee, J. L. ( 2001 ). Phonology and language use . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bybee, J. L. ( 2010 ). Language, usage and complexity . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Carstairs-McCarthy, A. ( 1994 ). Inflection classes, gender, and the principle of contrast.   Language , 70 , 737–788.

Carstairs-McCarthy, A. ( 2005 ). Affixes, stems and allomorphic conditioning in Paradigm Function Morphology.   Yearbook of Morphology , 2 , 253–281.

Churchward, C. M. ( 1940 ). Rotuman grammar and dictionary . Sydney, Australia: Methodist Church of Australasia.

Comrie, B. ( 1981 ). The languages of the Soviet Union . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

de Jong, N. H. (2002). Morphological families in the mental lexicon (Unpublished doctoral thesis). University of Nijmegen, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.

Dixon, R. M. W. , & Aikhenvald, A. Y. (Eds.). ( 2002 ). Word: A cross-linguistic typology . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dromi, E. ( 1987 ). Early lexical development . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Elman, J. L. ( 1990 ). Finding structure in time.   Cognitive Science , 14 , 179–211.

Erelt, M. , Erelt, T. , & Ross, K. ( 2000 ). Eesti keele käsiraamat . Tallinn: Eesti Keele Sihtasutus.

Goldwater, S. , Griffiths, T. L. , & Johnson, M. ( 2009 ). A Bayesian framework for word segmentation: Exploring the effects of context.   Cognition , 112 , 21–54.

Harris, A. C. ( 2009 ). Exuberant exponence in Batsbi.   Natural Language and Linguistic Theory , 27 , 267–303.

Harris, Z. S. ( 1942 ). Morpheme alternants in linguistic analysis.   Language , 18 , 169–180. Reprinted in M. Joos (1957), Readings in linguistics I . Chicago: University of Chicago Press (109–115).

Haspelmath, M. ( 1993 ). A grammar of Lezgian . Berlin: Mouton.

Haspelmath, M. ( 1996 ). Word-class-changing inflection and morphological theory. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1995 (pp. 43–66). Kluwer, Dordrecht.

Hay, J. ( 2001 ). Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative?   Linguistics , 39 , 1041–1070.

Hockett, C. F. ( 1958 ). A course in modern linguistics . New York: MacMillan.

Hockett, C. F. (Ed.). ( 1970 ). A Leonard Bloomfield anthology . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Karlsson, F. ( 1999 ). Finnish: An essential grammar . London: Routledge.

Kostić, A. , Marković, T. , & Baucal, A. ( 2003 ). Inflectional morphology and word meaning: Orthogonal or co-implicative domains? In R. H. Baayen & R. Schreuder (Eds.), Morphological structure in language processing (pp. 1–44). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Law, V. ( 1998 ). The Technē and grammar in the Roman world. In V. Law & I. Sluiter (Eds.), Dionysius Thrax and the Technē Grammatikē (pp. 111–120). Münster: Nodus Publikationen.

Levelt, W. J. M. ( 1989 ). Speaking: From intention to articulation . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Marslen-Wilson, W. , & Tyler, L. K. ( 1980 ). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding.   Cognition , 8 , 1–71.

Marslen-Wilson, W. D. , & Welsh, A. ( 1978 ). Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuous speech.   Cognitive Psychology , 10 , 29–63.

Matthews, P. H. ( 1972 ). Inflectional morphology: A theoretical study based on aspects of Latin verb conjugation . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Matthews, P. H. ( 1991 ). Morphology . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

McCarthy, J. J. ( 1982 ). Prosodic structure and expletive infixation.   Language , 58 , 574–590.

McMillan, J. ( 1980 ). Infixing and interposing in English.   American Speech , 55 , 168–177.

Milin, P. , Filipović Ðurdjević, D. , & Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. ( 2009 a). The simultaneous effects of inflectional paradigms and classes on lexical recognition: Evidence from Serbian.   Journal of Memory and Language , 60 , 50–64.

Milin, P. , Kuperman, V. , Kostić, A. , & Baayen, R. H. ( 2009 b). Words and paradigms bit by bit: An information-theoretic approach to the processing of inflection and derivation. In. J. P. Blevins & J. Blevins (Eds.), Analogy in grammar: Form and acquisition (pp. 214–253). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. (2003). Paradigmatic structures in morphological processing: computational and cross-linguistic studies (Unpublished doctoral thesis). University of Nijmegen, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.

Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. , Kostić, A. , & Baayen, R. H. ( 2004 ). Putting the bits together: An information-theoretical perspective on morphological processing.   Cognition , 94 , 1–18.

Mürk, H. W. ( 1997 ). A handbook of Estonian: Nouns, adjectives and verbs . Indiana University Uralic and Altaic Series, v. 163. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University.

Nida, E. A. ( 1949 ). Morphology: The descriptive analysis of words . Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

Norris, D. , McQueen, J. M. , Cutler, A. , & Butterfield, S. ( 1997 ). The possible-word constraint in the segmentation of continuous speech.   Cognitive Psychology , 34 , 191–243.

Paul, H. ( 1920 ). Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte . Tübingen: Max Niemayer Verlag.

Robins, R. H. ( 1959 ). In defence of WP.   Transactions of the Philological Society , 58 , 116–144. Reprinted in Transactions of the Philological Society , 99 , 116–144.

Sadler, L. , & Spencer, A. ( 1998 ). Morphology and argument structure. In A. Spencer & A. M. Zwicky (Eds.), Handbook of morphology (pp. 206–236). Blackwell.

Sapir, E. ( 1921 ). Language . New York: Harcourt Brace.

Schleicher, A. ( 1859 ). Zur Morphologie der Sprache. Volume 1, Memoires de L’Académie de St Pétersbourg . Leipzig: Leopold Voss.

Schreuder, R. , & Baayen, R. H. ( 1997 ). How complex simplex words can be.   Journal of Memory and Language , 37 , 118–139.

Spencer, A. ( 1999 ). Transpositions and argument structure. In G. Booij & J. van Marie (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1998 (pp. 73–101). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Stump, G. T. ( 2001 ). Inflectional morphology: A theory of paradigm structure . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stump, G. T. ( 2005 ). Some criticisms of Carstairs-McCarthy’s conclusions.   Yearbook of Morphology , 2 , 283–303.

Taft, M. ( 1979 ). Recognition of affixed words and the word frequency effect.   Memory and Cognition , 7 , 263–272.

Tschenkéli, K. ( 1958 ). Einführung in die georgische Sprache . Zurich: Amirani Verlag.

Zepeda, O. ( 1983 ). A Papago grammar . Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.

  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Institutional account management
  • Rights and permissions
  • Get help with access
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Etymology

hypothetical (adj.)

"founded on or characterized by a hypothesis, conjectural," 1580s, from Latinized form of Greek hypothetikos "pertaining to a hypothesis," from hypothesis (see hypothesis ). Hypothetic (1670s) is less common. Related: Hypothetically .

Entries linking to hypothetical

1590s, "a particular statement;" 1650s, "a proposition, assumed and taken for granted, used as a premise," from French hypothese and directly from Late Latin hypothesis , from Greek hypothesis "base, groundwork, foundation," hence in extended use "basis of an argument, supposition," literally "a placing under," from hypo- "under" (see hypo- ) + thesis "a placing, proposition" (from reduplicated form of PIE root *dhe- "to set, put"). A term in logic; narrower scientific sense is from 1640s.

Trends of hypothetical

More to explore, share hypothetical.

updated on October 09, 2020

Trending words

  • 3 . romance
  • 6 . government
  • 7 . beautiful

Dictionary entries near hypothetical

hypothermia

hypothesise

hypothesize

hypothetical

  • English (English)
  • 简体中文 (Chinese)
  • Deutsch (German)
  • Español (Spanish)
  • Français (French)
  • Italiano (Italian)
  • 日本語 (Japanese)
  • 한국어 (Korean)
  • Português (Portuguese)
  • 繁體中文 (Chinese)
  • Skip to Content
  • Skip to Footer

Please enable JavaScript for this site to work properly.

The "Ge" Hypothesis

Quick summary.

The Greek root word ge , commonly used in the English prefix geo- , means “earth.” This Greek root is the word origin of a good number of English vocabulary words, including ge ology, ge ography, and ge ometry. The Greek root word ge is easily recalled through the English word ge ology, which is the study of the “earth.”

The Greek root word ge and English prefix geo- mean “earth.” Soon you’ll be saying “golly gee whiz” when you find out the number of words in English that contain this Greek root!

Since Earth is our home, it would make sense for humans to study that home. Geo logy is the study of the physical or solid “Earth.” Geo logists are those scientists who study that solid “Earth.” The study of geo graphy, on the other hand, deals with the lands of our “Earth,” including the boundaries between and features of those lands and the people who inhabit all of Earth’s different countries.

The measurement of the “Earth” or geo metry was born when the Greek mathematician Euclid decided he needed a way to measure his planet. Not to be outdone by Euclid, Greek astronomers such as Aristarchus proposed a heliocentric model of the Universe which radically stated that the Earth revolved around the sun, which went completely against the commonly held belief of a geo centric model where the “Earth” was the center of the Universe around which all other celestial bodies revolved. The notions of peri ge e and apo ge e came later, when it was discovered that the “Earth” does not revolve around the sun in a perfect circle, but rather travels through space in an ellipse; this advancement enabled astronomers to realize that “Earth” has a peri ge e in its orbit when it is closest to the sun, and a corresponding apo ge e, or point in “Earth’s” orbit when it’s furthest away from the sun.

Mother “Earth” was known as Ga ia to the ancient Greeks. Her name was made popular in the 1970s via the Ga ia Hypothesis, which suggests that living organisms and inorganic matter interact to keep planet “Earth” healthy. Ironically, a man named Ge orge Wald spoke at the first Ga ia conference; the name Ge orge, which comes from the Greek word “farmer,” was so formed because a “farmer” is someone who works with the “earth” to grow things. The state of Ge orgia, or etymologically an area on the map for “farmers,” was named after King Ge orge the II of Britain, who most likely was never a “farmer!”

And so here ends our exploration of the Greek root word ge and English prefix geo- , a linguistic journey which has taken us around the “Earth” and beyond!

  • geology : study of the physical or solid “Earth”
  • geologist : one who studies the solid parts of the “Earth”
  • geography : study of the nations and peoples of the “Earth”
  • geometry : mathematics which measures the features of the “Earth”
  • geocentric : of an “Earth”-centered Universe
  • Gaia : in Greek mythology, the original “Earth” Mother
  • George : from the Greek word for “farmer,” or worker in the “earth”
  • Georgia : land of “farmers,” an eponym based upon King George II of Britain

Related Rootcasts

The fascinating parts of words.

Morphology is the study of how words are put together by using morphemes, which include prefixes, roots, and suffixes. Parsing the different morphemes in a word reveals meaning and part of speech. For instance, the word “invention” includes the prefix in- + the root vent + the suffix -ion , from which is formed the noun “invention.”

Etymology: Word Origins

Etymology is that part of linguistics that studies word origins. English vocabulary words are formed from many different sources, especially Latin and Greek. By determining the origins of the morphemes in English words, one is better able to remember and determine the dictionary definitions of words.

Differentiated vocabulary for your students is just a click away.

We use cookies! 🍪

We use cookies to provide you with the best online experience. By using this site, you agree to our privacy policy and terms of use .

Get Started

Hi there! Tell us a little bit about yourself to begin.

I'm an educator

looking to use Membean at my school or district.

I'm a parent

looking for my kid(s) to use Membean.

I'm interested in: Select... Homeschooling Independent Learning

I'm an individual

wanting to study for a test or to improve my vocabulary.

I'm studying for: Select... SAT GRE Personal Use

Word of the Day

What it means.

An homage is something that is done to honor someone or something. It is often used with the word pay (as in “pay homage”) to mean “to respect or honor.”

// Her latest book is an homage to her favorite city.

// The paintings in the new art gallery pay homage to women artists of the past.

See the entry >

homage in Context

“The series also pays homage to Detroit with its brutal winters, chicken spots and fur-draped residents. It’s a city grounded in Black culture, which is only now beginning to reemerge after years of negligence.” — Aramide Tinubu, Variety , 21 Mar. 2024

Build your vocabulary! Get Word of the Day in your inbox every day.

Face your fears.

clown-mask

  • Arachnophobia is the fear of what multi-legged creatures?
  • Octopuses Centipedes
  • Spiders Ants

Spell It

Hear a word and type it out. How many can you get right?

Play Blossom: Solve today's spelling word game by finding as many words as you can using just 7 letters. Longer words score more points.

You can make only 12 words. Pick the best ones!

Did You Know?

In medieval times, a person could officially become a vassal of a king or lord by publicly announcing allegiance to him in a formal ceremony. In that ritual, known as homage (from the Latin root homo- , meaning “man”), the subject (who was usually but not always a man) knelt and placed his hands between those of his lord, symbolically surrendering himself and putting himself at the lord’s disposal and under his jurisdiction. A bond was thus forged between the two; the vassal’s part was to revere and serve his lord, and the lord’s role was to protect and provide for the vassal and his family. The symbolism attached to the word proved irresistible, and homage quickly broadened to apply with the meaning “respect or honor” in a variety of contexts. Today, a singer can pay homage to someone who influenced their career, and a recipe can be an homage to a chef’s hometown.

Test Your Vocabulary with M-W Quizzes

roulette-wheel-thirteen

13-Letter Word Quiz

brown chihuahua sitting on the floor with squinting eyes looking at the camera

What Did You Just Call Me?

burrowing owl athene cunicularia tilts its head outside its burrow on marco island florida

Guess the Curious Origins of These Everyday Words

name that thing flower edition

Name That Flower

number-twelve-balloon

12-Letter Words Quiz

girl-pencil-test

Challenging Standardized Test Words Vol. 3

Name That Synonym

Fill in the blanks to complete a synonym of homage : _ b e _ s _ _ c e.

More Words of the Day

Tendentious, noblesse oblige.

Play Quordle: Guess all four words in a limited number of tries.  Each of your guesses must be a real 5-letter word.

Can you solve 4 words at once?

Subscribe to America's largest dictionary and get thousands more definitions and advanced search—ad free!

Games & Quizzes

Play Missing Letter: a crossword with a twist. Each of the 25 puzzle words start with a different letter of the alphabet. Which letter is missing?

IMAGES

  1. 13 Different Types of Hypothesis (2024)

    root of word hypothesis

  2. What is the meaning of the word HYPOTHESIS?

    root of word hypothesis

  3. How to Write a Hypothesis: The Ultimate Guide with Examples

    root of word hypothesis

  4. How to Write a Hypothesis

    root of word hypothesis

  5. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis in 6 Simple Steps

    root of word hypothesis

  6. SOLUTION: How to write research hypothesis

    root of word hypothesis

VIDEO

  1. Unlocking Words: The Power of Word Roots

  2. One Sample Hypothesis Testing

  3. What Is A Hypothesis?

  4. Definition and Nature of Hypothesis by Dr. Sandhu in Urdu

  5. Hypothesis Meaning In Hindi With Examples @VocabularyGuru

  6. Hypothesis Meaning in Kannada

COMMENTS

  1. hypothesis

    hypothesis. (n.) 1590s, "a particular statement;" 1650s, "a proposition, assumed and taken for granted, used as a premise," from French hypothese and directly from Late Latin hypothesis, from Greek hypothesis "base, groundwork, foundation," hence in extended use "basis of an argument, supposition," literally "a placing under," from hypo- "under ...

  2. hypothesis

    hypothesis (plural hypotheses) ( sciences) Used loosely, a tentative conjecture explaining an observation, phenomenon or scientific problem that can be tested by further observation, investigation and/or experimentation. As a scientific term of art, see the attached quotation. Compare to theory, and quotation given there.

  3. Hypothesis Definition & Meaning

    hypothesis: [noun] an assumption or concession made for the sake of argument. an interpretation of a practical situation or condition taken as the ground for action.

  4. hypotheses

    Entries linking to hypotheses. hypothesis (n.) 1590s, "a particular statement;" 1650s, "a proposition, assumed and taken for granted, used as a premise," from French hypothese and directly from Late Latin hypothesis, from Greek hypothesis "base, groundwork, foundation," hence in extended use "basis of an argument, supposition," literally "a ...

  5. HYPOTHESIS Definition & Meaning

    Hypothesis definition: a proposition, or set of propositions, set forth as an explanation for the occurrence of some specified group of phenomena, either asserted merely as a provisional conjecture to guide investigation (working hypothesis ) or accepted as highly probable in the light of established facts.. See examples of HYPOTHESIS used in a sentence.

  6. Hypothesis

    The hypothesis of Andreas Cellarius, showing the planetary motions in eccentric and epicyclical orbits.. A hypothesis (pl.: hypotheses) is a proposed explanation for a phenomenon.For a hypothesis to be a scientific hypothesis, the scientific method requires that one can test it. Scientists generally base scientific hypotheses on previous observations that cannot satisfactorily be explained ...

  7. Etymonline

    Tremendous thanks and appreciation to all of you. The online etymology dictionary (etymonline) is the internet's go-to source for quick and reliable accounts of the origin and history of English words, phrases, and idioms. It is professional enough to satisfy academic standards, but accessible enough to be used by anyone.

  8. hypothesis, n. meanings, etymology and more

    The earliest known use of the noun hypothesis is in the late 1500s. OED's earliest evidence for hypothesis is from 1596, in the writing of Earl of Essex. hypothesis is a borrowing from Greek. Etymons: Greek ὑπόθεσις. See etymology.

  9. hypothesis noun

    1 [countable] an idea or explanation of something that is based on a few known facts but that has not yet been proved to be true or correct synonym theory to formulate/confirm a hypothesis a hypothesis about the function of dreams There is little evidence to support these hypotheses. Topic Collocations Scientific Research theory. formulate/advance a theory/hypothesis

  10. hypothesis

    The hypothesis predicts that children will perform better on task A than on task B. The results confirmed his hypothesis on the use of modal verbs. These observations appear to support our working hypothesis. a speculative hypothesis concerning the nature of matter; an interesting hypothesis about the development of language

  11. HYPOTHESIS

    HYPOTHESIS meaning: 1. an idea or explanation for something that is based on known facts but has not yet been proved…. Learn more.

  12. HYPOTHESIS

    HYPOTHESIS definition: 1. an idea or explanation for something that is based on known facts but has not yet been proved…. Learn more.

  13. Hypothesis Definition & Meaning

    Hypothesis definition: An unproved theory, proposition, supposition, etc. tentatively accepted to explain certain facts or (working hypothesis) to provide a basis for further investigation, argument, etc. ... Greek hupothesis proposal, supposition from hupotithenai hupothe-to suppose hupo-hypo-tithenai to place dhē-in Indo-European roots .

  14. HYPOTHESIS definition

    HYPOTHESIS meaning: a suggested explanation for something that has not yet been proved to be true. Learn more.

  15. Hypothesis

    hypothesis, something supposed or taken for granted, with the object of following out its consequences (Greek hypothesis, "a putting under," the Latin equivalent being suppositio ). Discussion with Kara Rogers of how the scientific model is used to test a hypothesis or represent a theory. Kara Rogers, senior biomedical sciences editor of ...

  16. HYPOTHESIS definition in American English

    hypothesis in American English. 1. a proposition, or set of propositions, set forth as an explanation for the occurrence of some specified group of phenomena, either asserted merely as a provisional conjecture to guide investigation ( working hypothesis) or accepted as highly probable in the light of established facts. 2.

  17. 9 The Morphology of Words

    Abstract. Morphology is usually understood as the branch of linguistics that investigates word structure, a topic of central relevance to the systematic study of language and language processing. The Western grammatical tradition begins with the identification of words as the smallest meaningful elements of speech, a conception that survives ...

  18. (PDF) Research Hypothesis

    The etymology of the word "hypothesis" is traced back to Latin and Greek roots, emphasizing its foundation and groundwork. A hypothesis is an educated guess or prediction about the relationship ...

  19. hypothetical

    1590s, "a particular statement;" 1650s, "a proposition, assumed and taken for granted, used as a premise," from French hypothese and directly from Late Latin hypothesis, from Greek hypothesis "base, groundwork, foundation," hence in extended use "basis of an argument, supposition," literally "a placing under," from hypo-"under" (see hypo-) + thesis "a placing, proposition" (from reduplicated ...

  20. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis

    5. Phrase your hypothesis in three ways. To identify the variables, you can write a simple prediction in if…then form. The first part of the sentence states the independent variable and the second part states the dependent variable. If a first-year student starts attending more lectures, then their exam scores will improve.

  21. World Hypotheses

    World Hypotheses: A Study in Evidence, by Stephen C. Pepper (1942), presents four relatively adequate world hypotheses (or world views or conceptual systems) in terms of their root metaphors: formism (similarity), mechanism (machine), contextualism (historical act), and organicism (living system). In World Hypotheses, Pepper demonstrates the ...

  22. Rootcast: The "Ge" Hypothesis

    The "Ge" Hypothesis. The Greek root word ge and English prefix geo- mean "earth.". Soon you'll be saying "golly gee whiz" when you find out the number of words in English that contain this Greek root! Since Earth is our home, it would make sense for humans to study that home. Geo logy is the study of the physical or solid "Earth.".

  23. Word of the Day: Homage

    In that ritual, known as homage (from the Latin root homo-, meaning "man"), the subject (who was usually but not always a man) knelt and placed his hands between those of his lord, symbolically surrendering himself and putting himself at the lord's disposal and under his jurisdiction. A bond was thus forged between the two; the vassal's ...