• Search Menu
  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Urban Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Culture
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Media
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Lifestyle, Home, and Garden
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Society
  • Law and Politics
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Neuroanaesthesia
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Oncology
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Medical Ethics
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business History
  • Business Strategy
  • Business Ethics
  • Business and Government
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic History
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • International Political Economy
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Theory
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Politics and Law
  • Public Administration
  • Public Policy
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Developmental and Physical Disabilities Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

Culturally Competent Research: Using Ethnography as a Meta-Framework

  • < Previous
  • Next chapter >

Culturally Competent Research: Using Ethnography as a Meta-Framework

1 Introduction

  • Published: January 2013
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

This chapter explains the need for cultural competency in our growing diverse society. Noting that diversity is not limited to ethnic or racial domains, it defines diversity thoroughly and provide examples. It defines what is culturally competent research and what is not. The authors present a cultural competency continuum that shows at one end “cultural destructiveness” and at the other end “cultural proficiency.” The chapter concludes by identifying and discussing a 5-component framework for conducting culturally competent research: valuing diversity, conducting cultural self-assessment, managing the dynamics of difference, acquiring and integrating cultural knowledge, and adapting to diversity and cultural contexts.

Signed in as

Institutional accounts.

  • GoogleCrawler [DO NOT DELETE]
  • Google Scholar Indexing

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Sign in through your institution

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Our books are available by subscription or purchase to libraries and institutions.

  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Penn Arts & Sciences Logo

  • University of Pennsylvania
  • School of Arts and Sciences
  • Penn Calendar

Search form

Penn Arts & Sciences Logo

Social and Cultural Psychology

The aim of social and cultural psychology is to understand the way people behave in social situations, as well as the way they think about and feel about the broader social world, with a focus on uncovering both proximal and distal explanations of such phenomena.  Within the areas of social and cultural psychology, Penn faculty have a number of specializations. They are engaged in research on moral psychology, ethics, and moral judgment ( Baron , Farah , Goodwin , Rozin ), ethnopolitical conflict ( Rozin , Lustick ), and social decision processes ( Baron , Bhatia , Bicchieri , Schweitzer ), among other topics.  The approaches taken to study these topics vary, and include experimental, survey, and interview methods.  For additional information about specific areas of study, please consult the faculty/ lab webpages.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Dev Cogn Neurosci
  • v.45; 2020 Oct

The building blocks of social competence: Contributions of the Consortium of Individual Development

Caroline junge.

a Departments of Developmental and Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands

Patti M. Valkenburg

b Amsterdam School of Communication Research ASCoR, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Maja Deković

c Department of Clinical Child and Family Studies, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands

Susan Branje

d Department of Youth and Family, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands

Associated Data

Social competence refers to the ability to engage in meaningful interactions with others. It is a crucial skill potentially malleable to interventions. Nevertheless, it remains difficult to select which children, which periods in a child’s life, and which underlying skills form optimal targets for interventions. Development of social competence is complex to characterize because (a) it is by nature context- dependent; (b) it is subserved by multiple relevant processes that develop at different times in a child’s life; and (c) over the years multiple, possibly conflicting, ways have been coined to index a child’s social competence. The current paper elaborates upon a theoretical model of social competence developed by Rose-Krasnor (Rose- Krasnor, 1997; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ), and it makes concrete how underlying skills and the variety of contexts of social interaction are both relevant dimensions of social competence that might change over development. It then illustrates how the cohorts and work packages in the Consortium on Individual Development each provide empirical contributions necessary for testing this model on the development of social competence.

1. Introduction

Social competence can be characterized as the effectiveness of a child to engage in social interactions with peers and adults ( Fabes et al., 2006 ; Rubin et al., 1998 ). It is the behavioral manifestation of a child’s emotional and regulatory competencies while interacting with other people. Social competence does not represent a fixed quality but should be viewed as a construct that in itself marks development: Society expects more sophisticated interactions with older children. When children are growing up, interaction contexts beyond the home environment gain importance and become increasingly broader. Moreover, being effective in a variety of social interactions requires children to master many skills that underlie social competence, such as perspective taking, social problem solving, and emotion regulation, which possibly also differ in developmental stadia. Knowledge about (a) these underlying skills, (b) the interaction contexts, and (c) these developmental stadia all contribute to a better understanding of social competence, which is why we consider these three types of knowledge as relevant dimensions, that is, as crucial building blocks of social competence.

Although research on social competence has made great progress in understanding underlying skills and relevant interaction contexts in key periods in children’s lives (see e.g., Rubin et al., 2009 ; Bukowski et al., 2018 ), how these building blocks of social competence connect to each other over the course of development is less well understood: still missing is a detailed model of the development of social competence from infancy to adolescence. The aim of the Consortium on Individual Differences (CID) is to contribute to such a model that captures the development of social competence in a changing society.

In what follows next, we first describe why the field is in need of a developmental model of social competence (Section 2 ). We then give a brief overview of the development of social competence from infancy to adolescence (Section 3 ). In Section 4 , we explain the approach that CID takes towards building a developmental model, which is an elaboration upon a theoretical model of social competence developed by Rose‐Krasnor (1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ). In Section 5 , we show how each of the cohorts and the individual work packages from CID are contributing pieces of evidence to steer the theoretical model. Finally, in Section 6 , we conclude by suggesting how the cohorts and work packages in CID can complement each other in building a developmental model.

2. Why it is crucial to have a better understanding of the development of social competence

Developing social competence is essential for future functioning in society and for reducing risk of behavioral and emotional problems. Indeed, there is ample evidence that variation in social competence in childhood is linked to prowess in other domains in present and later life. For instance, people who as children easily develop good relationships with others are more likely to grow into adults with better health (they live longer; are more resilient to mental health problems, and function better in society; Luthar, 2006 ; Masten and Coatsworth, 1995 ). Socially competent children are more likely to advance in academics ( Caprara et al., 2000 ; Denham, 2006 ; Wentzel, 1991 ), or rate themselves as happier ( Ryan and Deci, 2001 ). Reversely, deviances in social competence can be a symptom for many forms of psychopathology emerging in child development. If social competence appears deviant, many other problems are typically observed, such as peer rejection (in ADHD; Larson et al., 2011 ), social anxiety ( La Greca and Lopez, 1998 ), bullying and aggression ( Warden, and Mackinnon, 2003 ; for overviews, see Happé and Frith, 2014 ; Trentacosta and Fine, 2010 ). Together, this suggests that the construct of social competence is a key factor in explaining individual variation, both in typical and atypical child populations.

The construct of social competence is a developmental construct: it emerges from meaningful interactions with various others in a variety of contexts ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ). Such interactions shape children’s competence: children learn how to behave in their social worlds both through direct instruction as well as by observing others in interactions. As a result, the type and quality of interactions children experience become increasingly more varied and complex over time. Moreover, children’s concepts of the relevance of interactions mark clear progression. Clearly, the construct of social competence changes over time, but a unified model of how social competence emerges from infancy to adolescence remains missing.

There are several reasons why we need a better understanding how social competence unfolds. First, indexes of social competence from early childhood have been shown to be predictive of social competence later in life (e.g., Howes, 1987 ; Monahan and Steinberg, 2011 ; Rubin et al., 1998 ; but see Masten et al., 1995 ). In fact, there appears to be a Matthew effect for social competence: for example, those competent in making friends early in life are becoming more competent in forming friendships, while the less-competent ones are becoming even less competent in forming friendships ( Flannery and Smith, 2017 ; Ladd, 1999 ; Monahan and Steinberg, 2011 ). Research further documents reciprocal links across various underlying skills of social competence. For example, positive experiences in building friendships early in life foster the development of prosocial behavior, which in turn increases the chance to form friendships later in life ( Flannery and Smith, 2017 ; Ladd, 1999 ). Such self-reinforcing links between the underlying skills of social competence underscore the need to view the development of social competence as a dynamic, complex process in which children are actively regulating their own experiences and creating their own contexts ( Sameroff, 2010 ). Yet to fully grasp the complexity of the development of social competence we need to better understand how and when social competence becomes self-reinforcing along development. Researchers should therefore start building and testing more elaborate models of social competence that take into account the interplay between development, the complexity of different underlying skills, and the variety of social contexts that together shape social competence.

A second reason why it is crucial to develop a clearer picture of how social competence unfolds is that social competence can be malleable, and open to interventions. Yet optimization of interventions in childhood requires not only identifying which underlying skills of social competence are well-suited targets, but also selecting optimal periods to administer such interventions, and should be tailored to a child’s stage of social competence. Knowledge on when to start an intervention is essential since developmental models such as the developmental cascades models assume that adaptive and maladaptive behaviors can result in spreading effects over time across various levels ( Cicchetti, 2002 ). Optimal interventions should ideally result in the interruption of negative cascades and the promotion of positive cascades ( Masten and Cicchetti, 2010 ). Thus, it is essential to develop a model of social competence that makes explicit not only how different underlying skills connect with different stages of social competence (the ‘hows’), but also how social competence changes over development (the ‘whens’).

The third and final reason why it is important to develop get a better picture on how social competence unfolds is that children’s social contexts (the ‘wheres’) have changed dramatically in the past two decades. One key change is that most Western infants and toddlers now have extensive experiences with peers and other adults prior to school entry. In fact, unlike earlier generations, most of today’s infants are in some form of day care away from their primary caregiver(s). How does this change affect the formation of peer relations and social competence ( Hay et al., 2018 )?

Another key change involves the rapid changes in children’s and adolescents’ media environments. In the 1970s the average age that a child started watching television was at 4 years of age. But due to the rise of prosocial and educational baby TV and apps (and parents’ tendencies to embrace such media), the onset of media exposure is now dropped to three and five months of age ( Valkenburg and Piotrowski, 2017 ). Developmentally appropriate educational media may support cognitive learning (e.g., numeracy, literacy), but could also improve underlying skills of social competence (e.g., prosocial behaviour), particularly when adults are involved with the content their children consume ( Courage and Howe, 2010 ). Furthermore, increasingly more interactions in childhood and adolescence take place online. What are the consequences of this? Do skills in social competence generalize easily to those required in online social interaction or does effectively communicating in digital interactions require an additional set of skills? Or does the larger amount of online interaction hamper development of complex underlying skills of social competence, such as emotion recognition and perspective taking? This is something research only starts exploring ( Blumberg et al., 2019 ).

3. Sketching the development of social competence

Before we can explain how CID aims to build theory on the development of social competence, it is essential to provide an overview of how social competence develops across childhood. In Table 1 we therefore define each period in childhood and list the main characteristics in marking the development of social competence. Please note that this overview is neither inclusive nor complete—it only serves to outline the highlights of each period in relation to social competence.

Each age period comes with its own characteristics of social competence.

Certainly not surprising, it appears that any period in a child’s life is fundamental in contributing to social competence ( Rubin et al., 2009 ), albeit for different reasons. For example, while in infancy social interaction skills typically evolve within the family context (e.g., Jones et al., 2014 ), childhood highlights the dominating force of peers within the classroom ( Masten and Coatsworth, 1995 ), and adolescence is the period in which most relevant social interactions mainly take place in cliques ( Moffitt, 1993 ; Weiss, 1986 ). In addition, a skill such as perspective taking emerges in early childhood but only reaches mature levels in adolescence, when adolescents have learned to appreciate that others can have different opinions ( Selman, 1980 ). Although each period comes with its own developmental tasks, most central issues continue to be of importance throughout development ( Waters and Sroufe, 1983 ). For instance, the significant association between the quality of parent-child relationship and children’s social competence is not moderated by age ( Groh et al., 2014 ). A developmental model of social competence should thus not only view its development as a set of discrete stages, but also consider the factors that continue to bear on its development.

4. Towards a developmental model of social competence

How should we now start building a developmental model of social competence? We propose to build on an existing theoretical model: the prism model of social competence put forward by Linda Rose‐Krasnor (1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ). This model does not focus on the development of social competence, but describes the different elements required for establishing good social interaction. We will first briefly summarize the prism model, before we outline how CID makes the prism model more concrete by adding a developmental framework.

The prism model has three hierarchical layers of analysis of social competence and one depth- dimension (context). The top layer of analysis is the theoretical one, which concerns social competence defined as effectiveness in interaction ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ). This definition allows us to maintain the same definition from infancy to adolescence. The second layer contains the indexical level and relates to the various ways in which social competence can be measured ( Flannery and Smith, 2017 ). The bottom layer of the prism model is the skills- dimension, which lists those underlying skills that are important across the many different contexts in which social interactions take place, such as emotion regulation and perspective taking skills. Finally, the depth- dimension of the prism model reflects the various kinds of contexts (home vs school; parent vs. peers; online vs. offline) in which interaction takes place.

In the next sessions, we explain how CID implements and provides data for the indexical layer as well as the skills- and context-dimensions in more detail. See Fig. 1 for a schematic representation of our proposed model based on Rose‐Krasnor (1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ).

Fig. 1

CID’s adaptation from Rose-Krasnor’s model of social competence ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ), adding a developmental perspective.

4.1. The indexical layer

The indexical layer encompasses the numerous ways researchers employ to quantify social competence, each of which characterize aspects of social competence or underlying skills of social competence (cf., Fabes et al., 2009 ; Flannery and Smith, 2017 ). The cohort studies in CID mainly rely on questionnaires as these are one of the easiest, fastest and most common ways to collect information about social competence in large groups of children ( El Mallah, 2020 ; Halle and Darling-Churchill, 2016 ). Most questionnaires are standardized, normed and internationally known questionnaires that can be filled in by either parents, teachers or children themselves.

4.2. The skills-dimension

The skills dimension is concerned with the foundational skills and motivations underlying social competence that are primarily individual in nature. It is at the skills level that developmental change might be considered most prominent and open to interventions ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ). However, there is no consensus on what one considers vital skills, partly because it is often difficult to tease apart underlying crucial skills from manifestations of social competence itself. Take for instance social perspective taking, which can be viewed both as an index of social competence, as well as a necessary skill from which social competence thrives. Table 2 lists the skills that various researchers find crucial for social competence ( Crick and Dodge, 1994 ; Halberstadt et al., 2001 ; Hay et al., 2004 ; Raver and Zigler, 1997 ; Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ). Although this list should not be considered as complete, it shows the variety of skills involved in social competence.

An overview of studies that list various skills as relevant processes to social competence.

Crucially, while Table 2 serves to highlight that there is no consensus in what one considers vital skills for social competence, it also reveals points of intersection. By focusing on those skills that are repeatedly listed we assume that these skills reflect the key foundations for social competence. We selected a set of five skills that serve as possible indicators in representing children’s (potential for) social competence. Below we motivate our choice in more detail. We begin with providing a definition and signaling its agreement with other researchers from Table 2 . We then give a brief overview of development, and end with how interventions targeted to this skill are beneficial for social competence.

4.2.1. Social encoding

Social encoding is the skill that requires a child to attend to the social interaction partner and to interpret meaningful cues from this person, such as emotions. We see the relevance of social encoding to social competence also in other researchers’ inventories of necessary skills (albeit phrased somewhat differently): as ‘encoding social situations’ ( Crick and Dodge, 1994 ), as ‘awareness and identification’ ( Halberstadt et al., 2001 ) and as ‘joint attention’ ( Hay et al., 2004 ). Some researchers suggest that newborns’ early interest in faces may be ‘the gateway to social expertise’ ( Jones et al., 2014 ). There is evidence that already seven-month- olds can differentiate between facial expressions ( Leppänen and Nelson, 2009 ), although the decoding of human faces continues to develop into adolescence (e.g., Cohen Kadosh et al., 2013 ; cf. Blakemore, 2008 ). Our proposal that social encoding is one of the key foundations of social competence is supported by interventions demonstrating that social encoding lead to modest improvements in children’s social competence ( Trentacosta and Fine, 2010 ).

4.2.2. Social problem solving

Social problem solving ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ) can be considered a logical continuation of the previous skill (social encoding), as it centers on responding in such a way to achieve social goals, such as solving conflicts with peers or gaining access to peer play. This skill is also listed by some as ‘social decision making’ ( Crick and Dodge, 1994 ). From early childhood up to adolescence, as children function increasingly in groups, social decision making assumes importance and often revolve around conflict resolution. One way to end conflicts is to react with anger or aggression, which often links to negative outcomes of social competence such as peer rejection ( Card and Little, 2006 ; Von Salisch and Zeman, 2018 ; Werner and Crick, 1999 ). This is not only true for behavior at the playground, but also holds for on-line behavior: cyber aggression is related to higher rates of loneliness and lower rates of friendships ( Schoffstall and Cohen, 2011 ). There are developmental shifts in the type of aggression that children can show in conflicts ( Laursen and Pursell, 2009 ), and when children use aggression strategically, it might actually be considered beneficial ( Hawley et al., 2007 ). Like social encoding, social problem solving is a skill susceptible to interventions aimed at improving social competence ( Denham and Almeida, 1987 ; for a recent meta-analysis, see Merrill et al., 2017 ).

4.2.3. Emotion regulation

If there is one skill that all researchers included in Table 2 consider vital to social competence, it is emotion regulation ( Hay et al., 2004 ; Raver and Zigler, 1997 ; also referred to as ‘arousal regulation’; Crick and Dodge, 1994 ; as ‘affect regulation’; Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ; or as ‘self-regulation’; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ; Vink et al., 2020 ). Being unable to exert control over one’s emotions, behaviors and arousals while interacting with others is a clear sign of obtrusive, unpleasant behavior that is typically disliked by most people. Indeed, there is ample evidence linking poor regulation skills to negative indices of social competence, in particular peer problems (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2001 ; Holmes et al., 2016 ; cf. Eisenberg et al., 2010 ). As inter alia Vink and colleagues describe ( Vink et al., 2020 ), emotion regulation is an umbrella term that covers both effortful control as well as executive functions and executive control (see also Nigg, 2017 ). In early infancy, children’s responses are at first mainly reactive rather than pro-active ( Ruff and Rothbart, 1996 ). Processes related to executive functions also come to the scene, mainly in toddlerhood onwards; for instance, inhibitory control emerges around 24–26 months ( Kochanska et al., 2000 ), whereas improvements in executive control appear most pronounced in early childhood ( Carlson, 2005 ). A recent review on the development of emotion regulation ( Eisenberg et al., 2010 ) reveals that children make great advances in their ability to exert control over their emotions in the preschool years while it improves more slowly into adulthood. Importantly, while individual differences in emotion regulatory skills are rather stable, they can serve as a mediator between parenting and children’s problem behaviors ( Belsky et al., 2007 ; Van Dijk et al., 2017 ). Moreover, interventions targeted at promoting self-regulation or regulating emotions result in more socially competent students ( Domitrovich et al., 2007 ; Low et al., 2015 ).

4.2.4. Communication

Communicative competence refers to the ability to use language effectively and appropriately in different social situations ( Hymes, 1979 ). Developing good communication skills ( Raver and Zigler, 1997 ; Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ; Rose-Krasnor and Denham, 2009 ) is of course also essential in ‘competent responding’ required for sustaining positive engagement in interactions ( Crick and Dodge, 1994 ). Although communication involves also nonverbal understanding ( Raver and Zigler, 1997 ), it is language that is indispensable to communication. Although language shows marked improvements in all aspects from infancy till early childhood (e.g., Clark, 2003 ), it is in particular a child’s pragmatic abilities (e.g., concerned with how children use language in interactions) that prove most relevant to social competence. For instance, children who scored low in pragmatic abilities (e.g., offered fewer requests for explanations or clarifications, initiated fewer conversations, and showed inappropriate turn-taking behaviors) were more likely to be rejected by their peers ( van der Wilt et al., 2018 ). In addition, children with developmental language disorder often experience peer problems or display problem behaviors (e.g., Curtis et al., 2018 ; Forrest et al., 2018 ; Van den Bedem et al., 2018 ), but this is related to pragmatic rather than structural problems with language ( St. Clair et al., 2011 ; Van den Bedem et al., 2019 ). Interventions aimed at improving pragmatic skills prove beneficial in promoting social competence and reducing peer problems ( Adams et al., 2012 ; Bierman et al., 2013 ; Coplan and Weeks, 2009 ).

4.2.5. Empathy

Empathy is a broad concept which generally entails the skill of identifying with another by taking another person’s perspective (cognitive empathy) as well as sharing the emotions of others (affective empathy). Empathy thus acknowledges the awareness that other people may have different emotions and feelings, but also allows for responding appreciatively, both of which are important prerequisites for maintaining social interactions (Eisenberg Fabes, & Spinrad, 2006). Because empathy is a highly valued trait in interactions, it is relevant for a myriad of social competence indices such as sustaining relationships, forming friendships, and peer popularity ( Eisenberg et al., 2015 , 2006 ; Spinrad and Gal, 2018 ).

Both affective empathy (responding to other person’s emotions, for instance via imitation) and cognitive empathy (‘social perspective taking’; Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ) are considered vital skills for social competence ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ). We see the relevance of empathy to social competence also acknowledged by other researchers in Table 2 : Hay and colleagues list it as ‘imitation’ as well as ‘causal understanding’ ( Hay et al., 2004 ); Rose‐Krasnor (1997 ) as ‘empathy’ and ‘perspective taking’.

It is possible that affective and cognitive empathy have different developmental paths. For affective empathy, it appears that even neonates can already imitate other’s facial expression (e.g., contagious crying, Simner, 1971 ). There is evidence that affective empathy in childhood and adolescence is an important underlying skill of social competence (e.g., Van der Graaff et al., 2014 ; van Hoorn et al., 2016 ). There is also protracted development in social perspective taking, which starts at a later age ( Rose‐Krasnor, 1997 ; Selman, 1980 ). Toddlers begin social perspective taking by recognizing the separation between self and others. That is, they are developing a theory mind, which is the awareness that others can hold different feelings or opinions from themselves ( Wellman, 1992 ; Wellman et al., 2011 ). Across childhood (2–12 years) children who possess an advanced theory of mind often display higher levels of social competence ( Imuta et al., 2016 ). Yet preschoolers might still find it difficult to act upon it as their own feelings might be a more dominating force. It is only by late childhood that children learn to view oneself from another person’s perspective. Early adolescence sees the development of mutual and third- person perspective, and late adolescence is characterized by taking into account perspectives beyond the immediate interaction as it considers the relevance of one’s current interaction to social norms.

Interventions targeting the skill of empathy often start with improving in social-emotional understanding and prosocial behaviors in class-room settings (that is, in early and late childhood). Such interventions reveal small but positive effects for fostering social competence, visible in indices such as peer nominations and teacher ratings ( Durlak et al., 2011 ; Malti et al., 2016 ).

To conclude, for the skills-dimension CID identifies five skills underlying social competence, each of which are complex constructs of themselves.

4.3. The context-dimension

The context dimension stresses the variety of relevant contexts in which interactions usually can take place in Western society. Such contexts do not only concern the setting of an interaction (“a situation in time and place”; Hartup, 2009 , p.8), but also with whom a child is interacting. While researchers acknowledge the variety in the skills contributing to social competence, few make explicit the variety of contexts that shape social competence. To demonstrate the richness of contexts of these interactions it is helpful to characterize them using pairs of dichotomies. Below we give four useful dichotomies, and sketch development.

4.3.1. Home versus school

It is at home that children will build the first set of meaningful interactions, with their caretaker(s) and with the other members of the household (e.g., siblings). Consequently, the home provides the foundation for social interactions. Nevertheless, this is not the only context in which some infants learn to interact with others. In Western societies such as the Netherlands, the majority of infants and toddlers regularly experience a form of daycare or play groups, which provides opportunities to learn to interact within small and stable groups of age mates ( Hay et al., 2018 ). Most people agree that providing such additional contexts can be beneficial for a child’s development of social competence, but how or when to cater for this is poorly understood. Next, while infants and toddlers differ in how much of the home context provides the dominant social context relative to other contexts, it is in early and late childhood that for all children the classroom setting gradually becomes a dominant social context. This is why indices on social competence collected in childhood often revolve around group dynamics in the classroom setting, such as (perceived) peer popularity and peer rejection ( Asher and McDonald, 2009 ). In adolescence, the major social context is still dominated by peers, but this time from their own cliques and clubs rather than the classroom.

4.3.2. Offline versus online

Early in infancy most of the interactions take place offline, in close proximity of other persons. With the rise of social media, children come into contact with multiple forms of online interactions from an early age. Indeed, there is evidence that even infants can also learn from persons via on-line interactions, as long as there is social contingency between the child and the other (i.e., when turn-taking occurs naturally, and not artificially; Roseberry et al., 2014 ). Children in middle childhood and adolescents increasingly use social media tools to communicate or play with their friends, peers, or partners they meet on game or other types of platforms ( Valkenburg and Piotrowski, 2017 ). A recent survey from the Netherlands has shown the majority of adolescents use two or three social media platforms, such as Instagram, YouTube, and Snapchat, in a complementary way ( van Driel et al., 2019v ). Today’s adolescents are amongst the first cohorts of young individuals who have grown up using mobile devices and social media; unlimited access to digital technologies enables them to be in constant contact with their peers and to engage in various social activities, such as playing games, creating audiovisual content, and sharing knowledge ( Salmela-Aro et al., 2017 ). However, about ten percent of current social media users has been identified as compulsive social media users ( van den Eijnden et al., 2016v ). Furthermore, positive correlations between compulsive use of technology and comorbid psychiatric disorders have been reported ( Andreassen, 2015 ). Since social media are a relatively new phenomenon, many questions regarding their potential impact on social competence and mental health remain unanswered ( Pantic, 2014 ). Therefore, more research in this field is required, and we hope that CID may provide some initial answers.

4.3.3. With adults versus peers

In a child’s life the most important adults are the caretakers (usually, the parents), and from childhood onwards, the teachers as well. (In both cases the child typically cannot control these social interaction partners). There is ample evidence that both parents’ ( Feldman et al., 2013 ; Groh et al., 2014 ) as well as teachers’ characteristics ( Wentzel, 2009 ) provide opportunities of interactions that contribute to a child’s social competence. From early childhood onwards, age-matched peers become increasingly the favored choice of interaction partners, as children learn to play and interact with peers. Although children with good social competence can interact easily in both contexts, children with poorer social competence (e.g., shy, withdrawn) find it often easier to interact with adults than with age-matched peers. Therefore, whereas adults might judge a child to be socially competent, one might reach different conclusions when observing a child interacting in situations with other peers.

4.3.4. With friends versus nonfriends

Already preschoolers can distinguish between friends and nonfriends ( Howes, 2009 ). Friendships centers around concepts of similarity: children like to play with others who are like themselves. However, our definition of social competence also requires that good social competence skills may come to the surface in contexts when the interaction partner is not familiar to the child. That is, how does the child interact when the other is not a friend, and the child therefore may not feel at ease with? Children who are shy in talking to others, or even experience social phobia, are at increased risk of developing poor social competence. It is therefore also important to consider social competence in the context of interaction partners the child is not friends with ( Asher and McDonald, 2009 ).

5. CID contributions to the developmental model for social competence

In the current paper we set out explaining that one of the aims of CID is to grasp the development of social competence. CID is a consortium of Dutch researchers aiming to understand the extent and relevance of individual differences in development. There are four main themes of research in CID, grouped into 4 work packages accordingly. Each work package focuses on different aspects of explaining individual differences across development. Two longitudinal cohorts were set up: the YOUth cohorts to sample neurocognitive development (Work Package 1;Onland- Moret et al., 2020), and the Leiden-CID cohorts (‘L-CID’) to test interventions in twins (Work Package 2; Crone et al., 2020 ). Work Package 3 unites four current cohorts established prior to CID ( Branje et al., 2020 ): Generation R (‘Gen-R’, Kooijman et al., 2016 ); Netherlands Twin Register (‘NTR’, Boomsma et al., 2006 ); RADAR (e.g., Branje and Meeus, 2018 ; Crocetti et al., 2017 ) and TRAILS ( Ormel et al., 2012 ). Finally, Work Package 4 focuses on advanced statistical modelling and animal models. CID thus encompasses six Dutch large-scale longitudinal cohort studies capturing child development through repeated measurements while it also houses the tools and methods required to address the complexity in developmental research. In what follows next, we provide more information how each of the cohorts (Section 5.1 ) and work packages (Section 5.2 ) provide building blocks towards building this model.

5.1. Contributions from the cohorts in CID

The cohorts participating in CID aim to help building a developmental model of social competence that integrates the ‘whens’ (age periods), ‘whats’ (indexical layer), ‘hows’ (skills-dimension) and ‘wheres’ (context- dimension) of social competence (See Fig. 1 ). More specifically, all cohorts in CID provide information about the ‘whens’ and ‘whats’ as all sample the development of social competence, albeit they differ in how exactly. It is one of the strengths of CID that all cohorts employ multiple indices collected at various moments in a child’s life to capture a child’s current stage of social competence. Table 3 lists for all cohorts which questionnaires they include to index social competence and it provides information about the age ranges that are covered; the frequency of administration, and the respondent filling in the questionnaires (amongst others children, parents, teachers).

An overview of the questionnaires that tap social competence and skills underlying social competence, for each of the cohorts involved in CID, with ages in years sampled in brackets.

Cohorts : Gen R = Generation R; l -CID = leiden Consortium on Individual Development; NTR = Netherlands Twin Register; RADAR = Research on Adolescent Development and Relationships; TRAILS = Tracking Adolescents’ Individual Lives’ Survey; YOU-th = Youth of Utrecht.

Indices : ASQ = Ages & Stages Questionnaire – Social Emotional -2 ( Squires et al., 2002 ); CBCL: = Child Behavior CheckList ( Achenbach, 1991 ; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001 ); IRI = Interpersonal Reactivity Index ( Davis, 1983 ); ITSEA = Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment ( Briggs-Gowan et al., 2004 ); NRI = Network Relationships Inventory ( Furman and Buhrmester, 1985 ); SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire ( Goodman, 1997 , 2001 ); SSRS = Social Skills Rating System ( Gresham and Elliott, 1990 ).

* Prosocial subscale from the Revised Self-Report of Aggression and Social Behavior Measure ( Morales and Crick, 1998 ).

** – only the prosocial scale from the SDQ.

c self-report; p parent report; t teacher report; f friend report; s sibling report; i partner report.

1 = collected every year; 2 = collected every two years; 3 = collected every three years.

Indeed, one of the strengths of CID is that its variety in questionnaires allows us to sample every period of child development, starting with infancy (YOUth cohort) and toddlerhood (L-CID cohort). This is in contrast with most studies that only begin measuring social competence once children go to school ( Parker et al., 2006 ). Because the CID cohorts cover each period in child development, we can examine not only direct and long-term outcome measures of social competence, but also precursors to social competence in younger children.

Another strength of CID is that these questionnaires are filled in by a variety of raters (children themselves, parents, teachers, or others), as the source of ratings might be prone to rater bias ( Jones and Yudron, 2016 ). It is important to consider the source of ratings (for example, parent-report vs. self-report), as the source often makes a difference on the factor loadings of the assumed underlying construct (e.g., Goodman, 2001 ; Van Roy et al., 2008 ).

Although the cohort studies in CID use a variety of indices of social competence, two are used in virtually all cohorts: the Strengths and Difficulties questionnaire (SDQ: Goodman, 1997 ; for Dutch: Van Widenfelt et al., 2003 ) and the Child Behavior Checklist (ASEBA CBCL; Achenbach, 1991 ; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001 ; for Dutch: Verhulst et al., 1996 ). Both questionnaires are relevant indices of social competence; they measure different underlying skills and complement each other. Whereas the SDQ measures key underlying skills such as prosocial behavior and friendship behaviors, the CBCL focuses on atypical (problematic) behavior in social interactions. Because all our cohorts employ at least one of these two questionnaires, CID is eventually able to collapse indices of social competence across cohorts that share the same indices (see also Zondervan-Zwijnenburg et al., 2020 for a similar approach to combining multiple cohort data on questionnaires related to behavioral control).

Besides the indexical layer, the cohorts participating in CID also address each of the five identified skills from the skills dimension, via repeated measurements collected in multiple ways spanning development from infancy into adulthood: through questionnaires, experimentally, and in parent-child interaction tasks. In Table 1 from the supplementary information we further delineate how each of the CID cohorts captures these five skills we consider relevant building blocks of social competence. Other articles in this special issue discuss some of the tasks and questionnaires in more detail.

With information adding to the development of skills we can ultimately understand the interplay between different indices, different subserving skills, and different contexts. This is crucial as social competence is a complex developmental construct. Take for instance the development of the underlying skills. While each of these skills show development, they often differ in their trajectories, and operate at different time scales at which they are more influential for social competence than others (e.g., Happé and Frith, 2014 ). We therefore do not assume that development in each skill proportionally continues to shape the development of social competence but rather that weights will change over time.

We illustrate the different time courses by comparing the communication versus empathy skills. Each of these skills have shown to be crucial, but how do they compare to each other in their relevance to social competence? Communication requires in particular a good command of pragmatics in order to confer meaning appropriately for social interactions. For communication skills we assume that pragmatic development has profound influences on social competence in childhood ( van der Wilt et al., 2019 ), but that the additional relevance of language development for social competence might reach a plateau over the following years, before it again assumes importance when friendships in adolescence center on intimacy & self-disclosure ( Troesch et al., 2016 , but see Curtis et al., 2018 ). Nevertheless, our changing society might also add further relevance to communication skills, as interactions increasingly take place online. It is unclear for instance how children with or without developmental language delays fare in digital media contexts that does not require immediate responses ( Drago, 2015 ).

In contrast to the relevance of communication skills to social competence in the early years, cognitive empathy is a skill that shows marked development in the adolescent years ( van der Graaff et al., 2014 ). We therefore expect this skill to continue to grow in importance to social competence, possibly peaking in adolescence, as this is the period when social perspective taking becomes sophisticated ( Selman, 1980 ) and when peer influence becomes a major force in social decision making (e.g., Crone and Dahl, 2012 ).

The above illustrations are mainly speculations. With the evidence gathered so far, we can only isolate the time course of the skills to underlying social competence and provide estimates how their relevance changes over time. What is still missing is evidence that reveals how a range of underlying skills across development together shape the development of social competence. Moreover, given that there is development both in the skills underlying social competence as well as in the different aspects characterizing social competence, such data will also unravel whether there are bidirectional relationships between skills and outcome measures. To illustrate, a recent study shows that while empathy predicts development in friendship quality, the reverse also holds: friendship quality drives empathy development ( Van den Bedem et al., 2019 ). Because CID repeatedly collects information on a wide range of skills (Table 1 from S.I.) concerning the same children from whom we also collect indices of their social competence ( Table 3 ), we aim to eventually contribute the evidence required for a better understanding how these skills work in tandem towards the development of social competence.

5.2. Contributions from the work packages in CID

Above we listed how the cohorts within CID examine the building blocks of social competence, as we make concrete how social competence emerges out of a variety in skills and contexts. Even so, fully capturing (the range in) the development of social competence requires integrating biological, psychological, and environmental factors, as well as insights into how these processes influence one another over time ( Beauchamp and Anderson, 2010 ; Karmiloff-Smith, 2017 ; Karmiloff-Smith et al., 2014 ). Further, in-depth understanding of individual differences in social competence begs a more detailed understanding of each of the descriptive levels of analysis, ranging from the molecular to the behavioral level, and how these levels link to each other both at the same time and across development. However, to date it has been virtually impossible to predict which combinations of factors at which times explain individual variability in the development of social competence.

One of the main reasons why there is not yet such a detailed account is that while different strands of research provide relevant blocks of knowledge, these remain limited as they typically do not cross beyond the boundaries of their own scholarly discipline. To illustrate, developmental studies often rely on longitudinal studies to investigate how psychological child characteristics and environmental factors contribute to a child’s well-being in real-life ( Bronfenbrenner, 2005 ), but these studies often do not include a biological or neurocognitive levels through which factors affect social competence (but see Crone et al., 2020 ). In contrast, biologically-oriented models provide us with a detailed mechanistic understanding of genes, neural function, or brain maturation relevant to the development of social cognition (e.g., Bakermans‐Kranenburg and Van IJzendoorn, 2007 ; Blakemore, 2008 ; Happé and Frith, 2014 ; Johnson et al., 2015 ; Robinson et al., 2008 ; Werker and Hensch, 2015 ) but they do not take into account child characteristics such as emotion regulation. It is here that the CID proves instrumental to building a developmental model of social competence as it accommodates the various disciplines of research that examine the development of social competence in both online and offline interactions as well as possess the statistical knowledge to integrate these findings.

As noted above, social competence is a developmental outcome measure that is reciprocal in the long-term. This makes social competence an example of a developmental cascade as it reflects behavior that can prove adaptive for some while having maladaptive consequences for others. Masten and Cicchetti (2010) identify five strands of research that would inform and optimize interventions required to promote positive cascades but to interrupt negative cascades: all of which are available in the work packages in CID.

One of the proposed strands is that research should determine when the cascade of social competence begins and accelerates to optimize the timing of interventions ( Masten et al., 2009 ). As laid out in this paper, data collected in our work packages 1–3 together provides an overview of social competence spanning from 20 weeks’ pregnancy (YOUth cohort) to far into adulthood (e.g., RADAR, TRAILS). Consequently, we cover development of social competence completely; that is, we can observe precursors in pregnancy, infancy and toddlerhood as well as its long-term consequences from conception and infancy onwards.

Second, cascade models would benefit from repeated measurements of social competence collected at various overlapping time scales. The choice of a lag is often chosen arbitrarily, while there must be adequate time for the cascading effects of factors leading to social competence to be manifested (e.g., Cole, 2006 ). With ample variation in time lags we can measure effects of time continuously; this allows us to reach a better understanding of how effects manifest themselves over time, as we can disentangle direct from indirect pathways in which the various variables of interest contribute to social competence ( Masten et al., 2006 ). Indeed, we are collecting longitudinal human data indexing social competence ranging from days (WP3: RADAR cohort) to yearly measurements (WP2: l -CID; WP3: Generation R, NTR, RADAR) to three-year intervals (WP1: YOUth cohorts) to generations (WP3: RADAR; TRAILS cohort). The RADAR cohort is of especial interest here as it is one of the few existing cohorts that even combines various lags within their data collection.

Then there are three remaining strands of research that according to Masten and Cicchetti (2010) are also instrumental in informing interventions, but which have not received as much attention in this paper. In all three cases, CID is able to contribute missing information. One line of research should be demonstrating the necessity of testing intervention designs that target mediating processes for change in social competence, which we cover in Work Package 2. A second strand of research should address how the interplay between genes, brain and environment affects social competence, which we address in our multi- method cohorts: YOUth-cohorts, l -CID and NTR all collect genes and multiple indices of environment (YOUth cohorts in WP1: Onland-Moret et al., 2020 twin cohorts L-CID in WP2; cf. Crone et al., 2020 ; NTR cohort in WP3: Boomsma et al., 2006 ; cf. Branje et al., 2020 ). We also have access to rodent models that allow for a level of control at the level of genes, brain, or environment that cannot be achieved in humans (WP4; cf., van der Veen et al., 2020 ). Finally, Masten and Cicchetti (2010) stress the need of well-designed experiments to further bolster our model that go beyond longitudinal cohorts to demonstrate causal directions between variables of interest and the outcome measure of social competence. With the help of animal models (WP4) and neurocognitive testing (WP1, WP2) we can achieve this. For instance, while Bierman et al. (2008 ) suggest that enriched environments foster social competence, we now test its specificity and generalizability of this in rodent models, which allows for hypothesis-testing in more stringent conditions as the contribution of other factors such as socio-emotional and genes are controlled for ( van der Veen et al., 2020 ). All in all, the CID unites various strands of research that together centers on achieving a better understanding of the development of social competence.

6. Conclusions

To conclude, the literature is still missing a unified approach that integrates how a range of underlying skills together shapes the development of social competence in a range of contexts. The cohorts in CID collect information on different indices of social competence as well as on a wide range of underlying skills concerning the same children in a range of contexts repeatedly across various lags. The work packages in CID each provide unique additional information in testing our model. Putting these pieces together, CID aims to provide the evidence required for such theory-building and bridge these gaps in the literature.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Acknowledgements

The Consortium on Individual Development (CID) is funded through the Gravitation program of the Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science, and the Dutch Research Council (NWO) (Grant No. 024.001.003). The authors thank Chantal Kemner and Carlijn van den Boomen for helpful comments on earlier versions of this article, and Lotte Houtepen for assistance. The authors also would like to thank representatives of the cohorts involved in CID for their assistance: Eveline Crone and Bianca van Bulk from the Leiden-CID cohort; Dorret Boomsma and Eveline de Zeeuw from the Netherlands Twin Register; Tineke Oldehinkel from the TRAILS cohort; Manon Hillegers and Elize Koopman Verhoef from the Generation-R cohort; and Juliëtte van der Wal from the YOUth cohorts.

Appendix A Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the online version, at doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100861 .

Appendix A. Supplementary data

The following is Supplementary data to this article:

  • Achenbach T.M. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Achenbach T.M., Rescorla L.A. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; Burlington, VT: 2001. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Adams C., Lockton E., Freed J., Gaile J., Earl G., McBean K., Law J. The Social Communication Intervention Project: a randomized controlled trial of the effectiveness of speech and language therapy for school‐age children who have pragmatic and social communication problems with or without autism spectrum disorder. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012; 47 (3):233–244. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Andreassen C.S. Online social network site addiction: a comprehensive review. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2015; 2 :175–184. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0056-9. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Asher S.R., McDonald K.L. The behavioral basis of acceptance, rejection, and perceived popularity. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. Guilford Press; New York: 2009. pp. 232–248. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bakermans‐Kranenburg M.J., Van IJzendoorn M.H. Research review: Genetic vulnerability or differential susceptibility in child development: The case of attachment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2007; 48 (12):1160–1173. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beauchamp M.H., Anderson V. SOCIAL: an integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychol. Bull. 2010; 136 (1):39–64. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Belsky J., Pasco Fearon R.M., Bell B. Parenting, attention and externalizing problems: testing mediation longitudinally, repeatedly and reciprocally. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2007; 48 (12):1233–1242. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bierman K.L., Domitrovich C.E., Nix R.L., Gest S.D., Welsh J.A., Greenberg M.T., Gill S. Promoting academic and social‐emotional school readiness: the Head Start REDI program. Child Dev. 2008; 79 (6):1802–1817. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bierman K.L., Coie J., Dodge K., Greenberg M., Lochman J., McMohan R., Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group School outcomes of aggressive‐disruptive children: prediction from kindergarten risk factors and impact of the Fast Track prevention program. Aggress. Behav. 2013; 39 (2):114–130. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blakemore S.J. The social brain in adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008; 9 (4):267–277. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blumberg F.C., Deater‐Deckard K., Calvert S.L., Flynn R.M., Green C.S., Arnold D., Brooks P.J. Digital games as a context for children’s cognitive development: research recommendations and policy considerations. Soc. Policy Rep. 2019; 32 (1):1–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boomsma D.I., De Geus E.J., Vink J.M., Stubbe J.H., Distel M.A., Hottenga J.J., Willemsen G. Netherlands Twin Register: from twins to twin families. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2006; 9 (6):849–857. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Branje S.J.T., Meeus W.H.J. 2018. Research on Adolescent Development and Relationships (young cohort). [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Branje S., Geeraerts S., de Zeeuw, Oerlemans A.M., Koopman-Verhoeff M.E., Schulz S., Oldehinkel A.J. Intergenerational transmission: Theoretical and methodological issues and an introduction to four Dutch cohorts. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2020; 100835 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Briggs-Gowan M.J., Carter A.S., Irwin J.R., Wachtel K., Cicchetti D.V. The Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment: screening for social-emotional problems and delays in competence. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2004; 29 (2):143–155. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bronfenbrenner U. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2005. Making Human Beings Human: Bioecological Perspectives on Human Development. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bukowski W.B., Laursen B., Rubin K.H. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2018. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups (2nd Ed) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Caprara G.V., Barbaranelli C., Pastorelli C., Bandura A., Zimbardo P.G. Prosocial foundations of children’s academic achievement. Psychol. Sci. 2000; 11 (4):302–306. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Card N.A., Little T.D. Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2006; 30 (5):466–480. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carlson S.M. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005; 28 (2):595–616. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cicchetti D. How a child builds a brain: insights from normality and psychopathology: child psychology in retrospect and prospect. In: Hartup W.W., Weinberg R.A., editors. Vol. 32. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 2002. pp. 23–71. (Child Psychology in Retrospect and Prospect: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clark E.V. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2003. First Language Acquisition. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen Kadosh K., Johnson M.H., Henson R.N., Dick F., Blakemore S.J. Differential face-network adaptation in children, adolescents and adults. Neuroimage. 2013; 69 :11–20. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cole D.A. Coping with longitudinal data in research on developmental psychopathology. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2006; 30 (1):20–25. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coplan R.J., Weeks M. Shy and soft‐spoken: shyness, pragmatic language, and socio‐emotional adjustment in early childhood. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice. 2009; 18 (3):238–254. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Courage M.L., Howe M.L. To watch or not to watch: infants and toddlers in a brave new electronic world. Dev. Rev. 2010; 30 :101–115. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crick N.R., Dodge K.A. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychol. Bull. 1994; 115 (1):74–101. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crocetti E., Branje S., Rubini M., Koot H.M., Meeus W. Identity processes and parent–child and sibling relationships in adolescence: a five‐wave multi‐informant longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2017; 88 (1):210–228. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crone E.A., Dahl R.E. Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012; 13 (9):636–650. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crone E.A., Achterberg M., Dobbelaar S., Euser S., van den Bulk B., van der Meulen M., van IJzendoorn M.H. Neural and behavioral signatures of social evaluation and adaptation in childhood and adolescence: The Leiden Consortium on Individual Development (L-CID) Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2020; 100805 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Curtis P.R., Frey J.R., Watson C.D., Hampton L.H., Roberts M.Y. Language disorders and problem behaviors: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018; 142 (2) [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis M.H. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983; 44 (1):113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Denham S.A. Social-emotional competence as support for school readiness: What is it and how do we assess it? Early Educ. Dev. 2006; 17 (1):57–89. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Denham S.A., Almeida M.C. Children’s social problem-solving skills, behavioral adjustment, and interventions: a meta- analysis evaluating theory and practice. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 1987; 8 (4):391–409. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Domitrovich C.E., Cortes R.C., Greenberg M.T. Improving young children’s social and emotional competence: a randomized trial of the preschool “PATHS” curriculum. J. Prim. Prev. 2007; 28 (2):67–91. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drago E. The effect of technology on face-to- face communication. Elon Journal of Undergraduate Research in Communications. 2015; 6 (1):13–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durlak J.A., Weissberg R.P., Dymnicki A.B., Taylor R.D., Schellinger K.B. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta‐analysis of school‐based universal interventions. Child Dev. 2011; 82 (1):405–432. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenberg N., Cumberland A., Spinrad T.L., Fabes R.A., Shepard S.A., Reiser M., Guthrie I.K. The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Dev. 2001; 72 (4):1112–1134. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenberg N., Fabes R.A., Spinrad T.L. Prosocial development. In: Eisenberg N., Damon W., Lerner R.M., editors. Handbook of Child Psychology Vol. 3: Social, Emotional and Personality Development. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ: 2006. pp. 646–718. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenberg N., Spinrad T.L., Eggum N.D. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2010; 6 :495–525. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisenberg N., Eggum-Wilkens N.D., Spinrad T.L. The development of prosocial behavior. In: Schroeder D.A., Graziano W.G., editors. The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior. Oxford University Press; New York: 2015. pp. 114–136. [ Google Scholar ]
  • El Mallah S. Conceptualization and measurement of adolescent prosocial behavior: looking back and moving forward. J. Res. Adolesc. 2020; 30 :15–38. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fabes R.A., Gaertner B.M., Popp T.K. Getting along with others: social competence in early childhood. Blackwell handbook of early childhood development. 2006:296–316. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fabes R.A., Martin C.L., Hanish L.D. Children’s behaviors and interactions with peers. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. Guilford Press; New York: 2009. pp. 45–62. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feldman R., Bamberger E., Kanat-Maymon Y. Parent-specific reciprocity from infancy to adolescence shapes children’s social competence and dialogical skills. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2013; 15 (4):407–423. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flannery K.M., Smith R.L. Are peer status, friendship quality, and friendship stability equivalent markers of social competence? Adolesc. Res. Rev. 2017; 2 (4):331–340. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forrest C.L., Gibson J.L., Halligan S.L., St Clair M.C. A longitudinal analysis of early language difficulty and peer problems on later emotional difficulties in adolescence: evidence from the Millennium Cohort Study. Autism Dev. Lang. Impair. 2018; 3 :239. doi: 10.1177/2396941518795392. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Furman W., Buhrmester D. Children’s perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Dev. Psychol. 1985; 21 (6):1016–1024. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1997; 38 :581–586. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2001; 40 (11):1337–1345. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gresham F.M., Elliott S.N. American Guidance Service; 1990. Social Skills Rating System: Manual. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Groh A.M., Fearon R.P., Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., Van IJzendoorn M.H., Steele R.D., Roisman G.I. The significance of attachment security for children’s social competence with peers: a meta-analytic study. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2014; 16 (2):103–136. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halberstadt A.G., Denham S.A., Dunsmore J.C. Affective social competence. Soc. Dev. 2001; 10 (1):79–119. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halle T.G., Darling-Churchill K.E. Review of measures of social and emotional development. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2016; 45 :8–18. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Happé F., Frith U. Annual Research Review: towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014; 55 (6):553–577. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hartup W.W. Critical issues and theoretical viewpoints. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. Guilford Press; New York: 2009. pp. 3–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hawley P.H. Social dominance in childhood and adolescence: why social competence and aggression may go hand in hand. In: Hawley P.H., Little T.D., Rodkin P.C., editors. Aggression and Adaptation: The Bright Side to Bad Behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; Mahwah, NJ: 2007. pp. 1–29. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hay D.F., Payne A., Chadwick A. Peer relations in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2004; 45 (1):84–108. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hay D.F., Caplan M., Nash A. The beginnings of peer relations. In: Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., Rubin K.H., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. 2nd ed. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2018. pp. 200–221. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holmes C.J., Kim-Spoon J., Deater-Deckard K. Linking executive function and peer problems from early childhood through middle adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2016; 44 (1):31–42. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Howes C. Social competence with peers in young children: developmental sequences. Dev. Rev. 1987; 7 (3):252–272. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Howes C. Friendship in early childhood. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 180–194. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hymes D. On communicative competence. The communicative approach to language teaching. 1979; 2 :5–24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Imuta K., Henry J.D., Slaughter V., Selcuk B., Ruffman T. Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: a meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2016; 52 (8):1192–1205. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zondervan- Zwijnenburg M.A.J., Richards J.S., Kevenaar S.T., Becht A.I., Hoijtink H.J.A., Oldehinkel A.J., Boomsma D.I. Robust longitudinal multi-cohort results: The development of self-control during adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2020; 45 :100817. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Johnson M.H., Jones E.J., Gliga T. Brain adaptation and alternative developmental trajectories. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015; 27 (2):425–442. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jones S.M., Yudron M. Social competence in early childhood: challenges in measuring an emergent skill. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2016; 45 :19–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jones E.J., Gliga T., Bedford R., Charman T., Johnson M.H. Developmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014; 39 :1–33. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karmiloff-Smith A. Embrace complexity! Multiple factors contributing to cognitive, social, and communicative development. In: Budwig N., Turiel E., Zelazo P.D., editors. New Perspectives on Human Development. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2017. pp. 386–401. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karmiloff-Smith A., Casey B.J., Massand E., Tomalski P., Thomas M.S. Environmental and genetic influences on neurocognitive development: the importance of multiple methodologies and time-dependent intervention. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2014; 2 (5):628–637. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kochanska G., Murray K.T., Harlan E.T. Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Dev. Psychol. 2000; 36 (2):220–232. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kooijman M.N., Kruithof C.J., van Duijn C.M., Duijts L., Franco O.H., van IJzendoorn M.H., Moll H.A. The Generation R Study: design and cohort update 2017. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2016; 31 (12):1243–1264. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • La Greca A.M., Lopez N. Social anxiety among adolescents: linkages with peer relations and friendships. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1998; 26 (2):83–94. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ladd G.W. Peer relationships and social competence during early and middle childhood. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1999; 50 (1):333–359. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Larson K., Russ S.A., Kahn R.S., Halfon N. Patterns of comorbidity, functioning, and service use for US children with ADHD, 2007. Pediatrics. 2011; 127 (3):462–470. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Laursen B., Pursell G. Conflict in peer relationships. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 267–286. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leppänen J.M., Nelson C.A. Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009; 10 (1):37–47. doi: 10.1038/nrn2554. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Low S., Cook C.R., Smolkowski K., Buntain-Ricklefs J. Promoting social–emotional competence: an evaluation of the elementary version of Second Step® J. Sch. Psychol. 2015; 53 (6):463–477. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luthar S.S. Resilience in development: a synthesis of research across five decades. In: Cicchetti D., Cohen D.J., editors. Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation. 2nd ed. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ: 2006. pp. 739–795. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Malti T., Chaparro M.P., Zuffianò A., Colasante T. School-based interventions to promote empathy-related responding in children and adolescents: a developmental analysis. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2016; 45 (6):718–731. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A.S., Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Dev. Psychopathol. 2010; 22 (3):491–495. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A.S., Coatsworth J.D. Competence, resilience, and psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D., Cohen D.J., editors. Wiley Series on Personality Processes. Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2. Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation. John Wiley & Sons; New York, NY: 1995. pp. 715–752. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A.S., Coatsworth J.D., Neemann J., Gest S.D., Tellegen A., Garmezy N. The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through adolescence. Child Dev. 1995; 66 (6):1635–1659. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A.S., Burt K.B., Coatsworth J.D. Competence and psychopathology in development. In: Cicchetti D., Cohen D.J., editors. 2nd ed. Vol. 3. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ: 2006. pp. 696–738. (Developmental Psychopathology). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A.S., Long J.D., McCormick C.M., Desjardins C.D. Developmental models of strategic intervention. Eur. J. Dev. Sci. 2009; 3 (3):282–291. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Merrill K.L., Smith S.W., Cumming M.M., Daunic A.P. A review of social problem-solving interventions: past findings, current status, and future directions. Rev. Educ. Res. 2017; 87 (1):71–102. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moffitt T.E. Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. 1993; 100 (4):674–701. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Monahan K.C., Steinberg L. Accentuation of individual differences in social competence during the transition to adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 2011; 21 (3):576–585. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morales J.R., Crick N.R. University of Minnesota; Minneapolis: 1998. Self-report Measure of Aggression and Victimization. Unpublished manuscript. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nigg J.T. Annual Research Review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2017; 58 (4):361–383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Onland-Moret, N.C., Buizer- Voskamp, J., Albers, M.E.W.A., Brouwer, R.M., Buimer, E.L., Hessels, R.S., de Heus, R., Huijding, J., Junge, C.M.M., Mandl, R.C.W., Pas, P., Vink, M., van der Wal, J.M., Hulshoff Poll, H.E., & Kemner, C (under review). The YOUth study: Rationale, Design and Study Procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
  • Ormel J., Oldehinkel A.J., Sijtsema J., van Oort F., Raven D., Veenstra R., Verhulst F.C. The TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS): design, current status, and selected findings. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012; 51 (10):1020–1036. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pantic I. Online social networking and mental health. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2014; 17 :652–657. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0070. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parker J.G., Rubin K.H., Erath S.A., Wojslawowicz J.C., Buskirk A.A. Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: a developmental psychopathology perspective. In: Cicchetti D., Cohen D.J., editors. Developmental Psychopathology, Vol 1: Theory and Method. 2nd ed. Wiley; Hoboken: 2006. pp. 419–493. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raver C.C., Zigler E.F. Social competence: an untapped dimension in evaluating Head Start’s success. Early Child. Res. Q. 1997; 12 (4):363–385. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robinson G.E., Fernald R.D., Clayton D.F. Genes and social behavior. Science. 2008; 322 (5903):896–900. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roseberry S., Hirsh‐Pasek K., Golinkoff R.M. Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Dev. 2014; 85 (3):956–970. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rose- Krasnor L., Denham S. Social-emotional competence in early childhood. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. Guilford Press; New York: 2009. pp. 162–179. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rose‐Krasnor L. The nature of social competence: a theoretical review. Soc. Dev. 1997; 6 (1):111–135. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rubin K.H., Bukowski W., Parker J.G. Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In: Eisenberg N., editor. Vol. 3. 1998. pp. 619–700. (Handbook of Child Psychology. III. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B. Guilford Press; New York: 2009. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruff H.A., Rothbart M.K. Oxford University Press; London: 1996. Attention in Early Development: Themes and Variations. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ryan R.M., Deci E.L. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well- being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001; 52 (1):141–166. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salmela-Aro K., Upadyaya K., Hakkarainen K., Lonka K., Alho K. The dark side of internet use: two longitudinal studies of excessive internet use, depressive symptoms, school burnout and engagement among Finnish early and late adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 2017; 46 (2):343–357. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sameroff A. A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev. 2010; 81 (1):6–22. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schoffstall C.L., Cohen R. Cyber aggression: the relation between online offenders and offline social competence. Soc. Dev. 2011; 20 (3):587–604. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Selman R.L. Academic Press; New York: 1980. The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding: Developmental and Clinical Analyses. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Simner M.L. Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Dev. Psychol. 1971; 5 (1):136–150. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spinrad T.L., Gal D.E. Fostering prosocial behavior and empathy in young children. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018; 20 :40–44. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Squires J., Bricker D., Twombly E. Brookes; Baltimore: 2002. Ages & Stages Questionnaires : Social- emotional. [ Google Scholar ]
  • St. Clair M.C., Pickles A., Durkin K., Conti-Ramsden G. A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional, and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI) Journal of Communication Disorder. 2011; 44 :186–199. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trentacosta C.J., Fine S.E. Emotion knowledge, social competence, and behavior problems in childhood and adolescence: a meta‐analytic review. Soc. Dev. 2010; 19 (1):1–29. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Troesch L.M., Keller K., Grob A. Language competence and social preference in childhood: a meta-analysis. Eur. Psychol. 2016; 21 (167):179. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valkenburg P.M., Piotrowski J.T. Yale University Press; 2017. Plugged in: How Media Attract and Affect Youth. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van den Bedem N.P., Dockrell J.E., van Alphen P.M., Kalicharan S.V., Rieffe C. Victimization, bullying, and emotional competence: longitudinal associations in (pre) adolescents with and without developmental language disorder. Journal of speech. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2018; 61 (8):2028–2044. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van den Bedem N.P., Willems D., Dockrell J.E., Van Alphen P.M., Rieffe C. Interrelation between empathy and friendship development during (pre)adolescence and the moderating effect of developmental language disorder: a longitudinal study. Soc. Dev. 2019; 28 :599–619. [ Google Scholar ]
  • van den Eijnden R.J.J.M., Lemmens J.S., Valkenburg P.M. The social media disorder scale. Comput. Human Behav. 2016; 61 :478–487. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.038. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van der Graaff J., Branje S., De Wied M., Hawk S., Van Lier P., Meeus W. Perspective taking and empathic concern in adolescence: gender differences in developmental changes. Dev. Psychol. 2014; 50 (3):881–888. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van der Veen R., Bonapersona V., Joëls M. The relevance of a rodent cohort in the consortium on individual development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2020; 100846 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van der Wilt F., van der Veen C., van Kruistum C., van Oers B. Why can’t I join? Peer rejection in early childhood education and the role of oral communicative competence. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2018; 54 :247–254. [ Google Scholar ]
  • van der Wilt F., van der Veen C., van Kruistum C., van Oers B. Why do children become rejected by their peers? A review of studies into the relationship between oral communicative competence and sociometric status in childhood. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019; 31 (699-):724. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Dijk R., Deković M., Bunte T.L., Schoemaker K., Zondervan-Zwijnenburg M., Espy K.A., Matthys W. Mother-child interactions and externalizing behavior problems in preschoolers over time: inhibitory control as a mediator. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2017; 45 :1503–1517. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Driel I.I., Pouwels J.L., Beyens I., Keijsers L., Valkenburg P.M. 2019. Posten, Scrollen, Appen En Snappen’: Jongeren (14-15 Jaar) En Social Media in 2019. Center for Research on Children, Adolescents, and the Media (CcAM), Universiteit Van Amsterdam. [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Hoorn J., van Dijk E., Meuwese R., Rieffe C., Crone E.A. Peer influence on prosocial behavior in adolescence. J. Res. Adolesc. 2016; 26 (1):90–100. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Roy B., Veenstra M., Clench‐Aas J. Construct validity of the five‐factor Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in pre‐, early, and late adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2008; 49 (12):1304–1312. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Widenfelt B.M., Goedhart A.W., Treffers P.D., Goodman R. Dutch version of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2003; 12 (6):281–289. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Verhulst F.C., Van der Ende J., Koot H.M. 1996. Handleiding Voor De CBCL/4-18. Afdeling Kinder-en Jeugdpsychiatrie, Sophia kinderziekenhuis/academisch Ziekenhuis Rotterdam/Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam, Rotterdam. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vink M., Gladwin T.E., Geeraerts S., Pascal P., Bos D., Hofstee M., Vollebergh W. Towards an integrated account of the development of self-regulation from a neurocognitive perspective: A framework for current and future longitudinal multi-modal investigations. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2020; 100829 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Von Salisch M., Zeman J.L. Pathways to reciprocated friendships: a cross-lagged panel study on young adolescents’ anger regulation towards friends. J. Youth Adolesc. 2018; 47 (3):673–687. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Warden D., Mackinnon S. Prosocial children, bullies and victims: an investigation of their sociometric status, empathy and social problem‐solving strategies. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2003; 21 (3):367–385. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waters E., Sroufe L.A. Social competence as a developmental construct. Dev. Rev. 1983; 3 (1):79–97. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiss R.S. Continuities and transformations in social relationships from childhood to adulthood. In: Hartup W.W., Rubin Z., editors. Relationships and Development. 1986. pp. 95–111. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wellman H.M. The MIT Press; 1992. The Child’s Theory of Mind. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wellman H.M., Fang F., Peterson C.C. Sequential progressions in a theory‐of‐mind scale: longitudinal perspectives. Child Dev. 2011; 82 (3):780–792. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wentzel K.R. Relations between social competence and academic achievement in early adolescence. Child Dev. 1991; 62 (5):1066–1078. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wentzel K.R. Peers and academic functioning at school. In: Rubin K.H., Bukowski W.M., Laursen B., editors. Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. Guilford Press; New York: 2009. pp. 531–547. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Werker J.F., Hensch T.K. Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015; 66 :173–196. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Werner N.E., Crick N.R. Relational aggression and social- psychological adjustment in a college sample. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1999; 108 (4):615–623. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

Case Western Reserve University

  • Clinical Research Office
  • minority accrual committee
  • cultural inclusion and awareness

Cultural Competency and the Research Process

Cultural Competence in Research: Annotated Bibliography. The Harvard Clinical and Translational Science Center (2009 & 2010)

Cultural competence in research

Cultural competency in research has been defined as:

  • The ability of researchers and research staff to provide high quality research that takes into account the culture and diversity of a population when developing research ideas, conducting research, and exploring applicability of research findings.

Cultural competence in research plays a critical role in:

  • Study design and implementation process (the development of research questions and hypotheses)
  • Outreach and recruitment strategies
  • Consent activities
  • Data collection protocols
  • Analysis and interpretation of research findings; drawing conclusions; and presentation and dissemination of  results

Cultural competence is critical for researchers to ensure:

  • Effective communication and interaction between researchers and study participants
  • Adequate analysis and interpretation of results as they relate to the patient/population impact
  • Appropriate engagement in study design and implementation for community/population based research
  • Improved participation of minority populations in research studies ensuring that different subgroups in the population are represented proportionally.
  • Successful recruitment and retention of diverse individuals as study participants
  • Applicability of results and findings to diverse populations
  • Meeting the target population’s social, cultural and linguistic needs

Continuing the Process: What Can You Do?

  • Initiate the conversation! Encourage issues related to culture and research to be discussed in your department and during staff meetings
  • Commit to self-education. This month, read at least 3 of the resources related to cultural competency and research.

Want to learn more? 

View  Cultural Competency in Research , presented Katrice D. Cain, MA and Mary Ellen Lawless, MA, RN from the Center for Reducing Health Disparities (May 2013). This presentation provides an introduction to cultural competency in research and overview of the importance of considering culture throughout the research process.

Viewing the slide presentation and taking an online quiz will allow you to receive 3 Continuing Research Education Credits (CRECs). The slides from the presentation are available at:  https://research.case.edu/Education/CREC_Video.cfm  (Cultural Competency in Research: Katrice Cain and Mary Ellen Lawless’s Powerpoint Handout).

IMAGES

  1. Psychological frameworks for understanding cultural competence

    social and cultural competence in social psychological research

  2. A Simple Guide to Cultural Competence

    social and cultural competence in social psychological research

  3. why is multicultural competence important?

    social and cultural competence in social psychological research

  4. The SAGE model of social psychological research.

    social and cultural competence in social psychological research

  5. A Simple Guide to Cultural Competence

    social and cultural competence in social psychological research

  6. Cultural Competence in Social Work Practice

    social and cultural competence in social psychological research

COMMENTS

  1. In search of cultural competence

    Cultural competence — loosely defined as the ability to understand, appreciate and interact with people from cultures or belief systems different from one's own — has been a key aspect of psychological thinking and practice for some 50 years. It's become such an integral part of the field that it's listed as one of psychology's core ...

  2. Integrating Culture Into Psychological Research

    Put simply, culture includes the unspoken rules of conduct within a group, such as acceptable social standards. This definition intentionally focuses on psychologically relevant elements of "subjective culture," such as social norms, roles, beliefs, and values (Triandis et al., 1980).

  3. Integrating Culture Into Psychological Research

    on psychologically relevant elements of "subjective culture," such as social norms, roles, beliefs, and values (Triandis et al., 1980). Reading the research of Betancourt and López solidified my interest in conducting cultural research because it provided a method for quantifying the nuances often taken for granted in our everyday ...

  4. Cultural Competence and Beyond: Working Across Cultures in Culturally

    The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article. ... Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 107-120. Crossref. Google Scholar ... An anti-opressive approach to cultural competence. Canadian Social Work Review, 24(1), 105-114. doi:10.2307/41669865 ...

  5. The Case for Cultural Competency in Psychotherapeutic Interventions

    It then developed standards for cultural competence in social work practice (Natl. Assoc. Social Workers 2007). In the past two decades, cultural competency has been mandated to reduce mental health disparities; at the very least, cultural competency is recommended by various institutions, governmental bodies, and professional organizations.

  6. Cultural Competence in Research

    According to a report and program of Harvard Catalyst (2017), cultural competence is essential for researchers to ensure 1) effective interactions between researchers and participants, 2) adequate analysis of results, and 3) appropriate engagement in study design and implementation. Researchers need to be culturally aware and sensitive to ...

  7. PDF Cultural Competence in Research

    Cultural competence in research is the ability of researchers and research staff to provide high quality research that takes into account the culture and diversity of a population when developing ... adapted to meet the target population's social, cultural and linguistic needs. Depending on the type of research, cultural competence can be ...

  8. Culturally Competent Social Work Research: Methodological

    To our knowledge, Meleis's framework (1996) is one of the few that has integrated the concept of cultural competence into research methods. The framework has been used to evaluate culturally competent knowledge development (Mendias & Guevara, 2001), culturally specific measurements (Im, Meleis, & Lee, 1999), and the evaluation of the rigor and credibility of research with diverse populations ...

  9. Introduction

    Developing cultural competence is a continuous and continuing process. There is no finality to it. Individuals may be located at various levels of awareness, knowledge, and skills along a continuum. Such a cultural competency continuum was proposed in a monograph by Cross, Bazron, Dennis, and Isaacs originally published in 1989.

  10. PDF HOW YOU CAN BE MORE CULTURALLY COMPETENT

    Fortunately, there are many ways to educate yourself, Sharma says. "Form a multicultural consulting group, or join a book club focused on multiculturalism. Attend diversity summits and conferences. Check in with APA's Div. 45 (Society for the Psychological Study of Culture, Ethnicity and Race)," he suggests.

  11. Full article: Cultural competence for global research and development

    Intercultural competence. Embedded in an interdisciplinary perspective (i.e., international management, sociology, cross-cultural psychology), action research and conflict resolution, Friedman and Berthoin Antal (Citation 2005) propose the construct of intercultural competence that extends current conceptions of cultural competence.Consistent with the notion of intersectionality, discussed in ...

  12. Cultural competency: From philosophy to research and practice

    Journal of Community Psychology is a social psychology journal publishing empirical research into community factors that influence human development and behaviour. Abstract Cultural competency in the delivery of mental health services has gained considerable momentum. This momentum has been accompanied by questions about the meaning, usefulness ...

  13. Cultural humility as a form of social justice: Promising practices for

    Danso argues that those who critique cultural competence fail to consider the intention of its original conceptualization and overlook more nuanced research in this area. Instead, these critics narrowly essentialize the components of knowledge, awareness, and skills in a way that does not reflect the wider literature on cultural competence.

  14. Social and Cultural Psychology

    The aim of social and cultural psychology is to understand the way people behave in social situations, as well as the way they think about and feel about the broader social world, with a focus on uncovering both proximal and distal explanations of such phenomena. Within the areas of social and cultural psychology, Penn faculty have a number of ...

  15. Reconciling evidence-based practice and cultural competence in mental

    He has published more than 50 articles and chapters exploring the cultural psychology of self, identity, personhood, and social relations in Indigenous community settings vis-à-vis the mental health professions, with particular attention to therapeutic interventions such as psychotherapy and traditional healing.

  16. The building blocks of social competence: Contributions of the

    1. Introduction. Social competence can be characterized as the effectiveness of a child to engage in social interactions with peers and adults (Fabes et al., 2006; Rubin et al., 1998).It is the behavioral manifestation of a child's emotional and regulatory competencies while interacting with other people.

  17. Practice-based knowledge perspectives of cultural competence in social work

    Cultural competence in social work practice has been cited as crucial in the pursuit of ethical and professional standards. Still, conceptual, and practical questions remain for defining, imparting, and assessing skills of social work professionals in this key area. Practice-based knowledge has the potential to advance debates regarding the ...

  18. Standards and Indicators for Cultural Competence in Social Work ...

    Standard 1. Ethics and Values. Social workers shall function in accordance with the values, ethics, and standards of the NASW (2008) Code of Ethics. Cultural competence requires self-awareness, cultural humility, and the commitment to understanding and embracing culture as central to effective practice. Standard 2.

  19. Cultural Competency and the Research Process

    Cultural competence is critical for researchers to ensure: Effective communication and interaction between researchers and study participants. Adequate analysis and interpretation of results as they relate to the patient/population impact. Appropriate engagement in study design and implementation for community/population based research.

  20. Understanding Competencies in Research: Social & Cultural

    results of given research, in addition to the application of the acquired results. Social competence, which is defined as the ability to understand various social setting and come up with a strategy in figuring out the next steps with the use of emotional, intellectual ans social skills. In this situation, when the case is understood, the skills listed above come into play as a means of ...

  21. Influence of Collective Consciousness on Individual's Continuous Use

    Building upon the Social Fact theory and Social Impact theory, ... Liu, R. (2020). Research on the Influencing Factors of Government Websites' Willingness to Continue Using. ... Cultural characteristics of individualism-collectivism and its influence on emotional contagion. Chinese Psychological Society. The 23rd Chinese Psychological Society ...

  22. Academic but not social self-esteem mediates the association between

    The current research was designed to extend previous work by bringing a culture-sensitive perspective in the theorizing and empirical investigation of the role of parental psychological control in adolescent development. ... It would be worthwhile to test these roles of parental psychological control in children's development of competence and ...

  23. ERIC

    Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal, v27 n1 p261-281 2024 This paper analyses the literature related to the underrepresentation of women academics in order to identify the factors that influence the underrepresentation of women in higher-education teaching, academic leadership and research.

  24. Understanding Eating Habits With Psychology

    Psychological. "Many people use food as a coping mechanism to deal with such feelings as stress , boredom or anxiety, or even to prolong feelings of joy," says Dr. Albers.