Phil Reed D.Phil.

  • Personality

Self-Presentation in the Digital World

Do traditional personality theories predict digital behaviour.

Posted August 31, 2021 | Reviewed by Chloe Williams

  • What Is Personality?
  • Find a therapist near me
  • Personality theories can help explain real-world differences in self-presentation behaviours but they may not apply to online behaviours.
  • In the real world, women have higher levels of behavioural inhibition tendencies than men and are more likely to avoid displeasing others.
  • Based on this assumption, one would expect women to present themselves less on social media, but women tend to use social media more than men.

Digital technology allows people to construct and vary their self-identity more easily than they can in the real world. This novel digital- personality construction may, or may not, be helpful to that person in the long run, but it is certainly more possible than it is in the real world. Yet how this relates to "personality," as described by traditional personality theories, is not really known. Who will tend to manipulate their personality online, and would traditional personality theories predict these effects? A look at what we do know about gender differences in the real and digital worlds suggests that many aspects of digital behaviour may not conform to the expectations of personality theories developed for the real world.

Half a century ago, Goffman suggested that individuals establish social identities by employing self-presentation tactics and impression management . Self-presentational tactics are techniques for constructing or manipulating others’ impressions of the individual and ultimately help to develop that person’s identity in the eyes of the world. The ways other people react are altered by choosing how to present oneself – that is, self-presentation strategies are used for impression management . Others then uphold, shape, or alter that self-image , depending on how they react to the tactics employed. This implies that self-presentation is a form of social communication, by which people establish, maintain, and alter their social identity.

These self-presentational strategies can be " assertive " or "defensive." 1 Assertive strategies are associated with active control of the person’s self-image; and defensive strategies are associated with protecting a desired identity that is under threat. In the real world, the use of self-presentational tactics has been widely studied and has been found to relate to many behaviours and personalities 2 . Yet, despite the enormous amounts of time spent on social media , the types of self-presentational tactics employed on these platforms have not received a huge amount of study. In fact, social media appears to provide an ideal opportunity for the use of self-presentational tactics, especially assertive strategies aimed at creating an identity in the eyes of others.

Seeking to Experience Different Types of Reward

Social media allows individuals to present themselves in ways that are entirely reliant on their own behaviours – and not on factors largely beyond their ability to instantly control, such as their appearance, gender, etc. That is, the impression that the viewer of the social media post receives is dependent, almost entirely, on how or what another person posts 3,4 . Thus, the digital medium does not present the difficulties for individuals who wish to divorce the newly-presented self from the established self. New personalities or "images" may be difficult to establish in real-world interactions, as others may have known the person beforehand, and their established patterns of interaction. Alternatively, others may not let people get away with "out of character" behaviours, or they may react to their stereotype of the person in front of them, not to their actual behaviours. All of which makes real-life identity construction harder.

Engaging in such impression management may stem from motivations to experience different types of reward 5 . In terms of one personality theory, individuals displaying behavioural approach tendencies (the Behavioural Activation System; BAS) and behavioural inhibition tendencies (the Behavioural Inhibition System; BIS) will differ in terms of self-presentation behaviours. Those with strong BAS seek opportunities to receive or experience reward (approach motivation ); whereas, those with strong BIS attempt to avoid punishment (avoidance motivation). People who need to receive a lot of external praise may actively seek out social interactions and develop a lot of social goals in their lives. Those who are more concerned about not incurring other people’s displeasure may seek to defend against this possibility and tend to withdraw from people. Although this is a well-established view of personality in the real world, it has not received strong attention in terms of digital behaviours.

Real-World Personality Theories May Not Apply Online

One test bed for the application of this theory in the digital domain is predicted gender differences in social media behaviour in relation to self-presentation. Both self-presentation 1 , and BAS and BIS 6 , have been noted to show gender differences. In the real world, women have shown higher levels of BIS than men (at least, to this point in time), although levels of BAS are less clearly differentiated between genders. This view would suggest that, in order to avoid disapproval, women will present themselves less often on social media; and, where they do have a presence, adopt defensive self-presentational strategies.

The first of these hypotheses is demonstrably false – where there are any differences in usage (and there are not that many), women tend to use social media more often than men. What we don’t really know, with any certainty, is how women use social media for self-presentation, and whether this differs from men’s usage. In contrast to the BAS/BIS view of personality, developed for the real world, several studies have suggested that selfie posting can be an assertive, or even aggressive, behaviour for females – used in forming a new personality 3 . In contrast, sometimes selfie posting by males is related to less aggressive, and more defensive, aspects of personality 7 . It may be that women take the opportunity to present very different images of themselves online from their real-world personalities. All of this suggests that theories developed for personality in the real world may not apply online – certainly not in terms of putative gender-related behaviours.

We know that social media allows a new personality to be presented easily, which is not usually seen in real-world interactions, and it may be that real-world gender differences are not repeated in digital contexts. Alternatively, it may suggest that these personality theories are now simply hopelessly anachronistic – based on assumptions that no longer apply. If that were the case, it would certainly rule out any suggestion that such personalities are genetically determined – as we know that structure hasn’t changed dramatically in the last 20 years.

1. Lee, S.J., Quigley, B.M., Nesler, M.S., Corbett, A.B., & Tedeschi, J.T. (1999). Development of a self-presentation tactics scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 26(4), 701-722.

2. Laghi, F., Pallini, S., & Baiocco, R. (2015). Autopresentazione efficace, tattiche difensive e assertive e caratteristiche di personalità in Adolescenza. Rassegna di Psicologia, 32(3), 65-82.

3. Chua, T.H.H., & Chang, L. (2016). Follow me and like my beautiful selfies: Singapore teenage girls’ engagement in self-presentation and peer comparison on social media. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 190-197.

4. Fox, J., & Rooney, M.C. (2015). The Dark Triad and trait self-objectification as predictors of men’s use and self-presentation behaviors on social networking sites. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 161-165.

5. Hermann, A.D., Teutemacher, A.M., & Lehtman, M.J. (2015). Revisiting the unmitigated approach model of narcissism: Replication and extension. Journal of Research in Personality, 55, 41-45.

6. Carver, C.S., & White, T.L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319.

7. Sorokowski, P., Sorokowska, A., Frackowiak, T., Karwowski, M., Rusicka, I., & Oleszkiewicz, A. (2016). Sex differences in online selfie posting behaviors predict histrionic personality scores among men but not women. Computers in Human Behavior, 59, 368-373.

Phil Reed D.Phil.

Phil Reed, Ph.D., is a professor of psychology at Swansea University.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

May 2024 magazine cover

At any moment, someone’s aggravating behavior or our own bad luck can set us off on an emotional spiral that threatens to derail our entire day. Here’s how we can face our triggers with less reactivity so that we can get on with our lives.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Learn more

How it works

Transform your enterprise with the scalable mindsets, skills, & behavior change that drive performance.

Explore how BetterUp connects to your core business systems.

We pair AI with the latest in human-centered coaching to drive powerful, lasting learning and behavior change.

Build leaders that accelerate team performance and engagement.

Unlock performance potential at scale with AI-powered curated growth journeys.

Build resilience, well-being and agility to drive performance across your entire enterprise.

Transform your business, starting with your sales leaders.

Unlock business impact from the top with executive coaching.

Foster a culture of inclusion and belonging.

Accelerate the performance and potential of your agencies and employees.

See how innovative organizations use BetterUp to build a thriving workforce.

Discover how BetterUp measurably impacts key business outcomes for organizations like yours.

A demo is the first step to transforming your business. Meet with us to develop a plan for attaining your goals.

Request a demo

  • What is coaching?

Learn how 1:1 coaching works, who its for, and if it's right for you.

Accelerate your personal and professional growth with the expert guidance of a BetterUp Coach.

Types of Coaching

Navigate career transitions, accelerate your professional growth, and achieve your career goals with expert coaching.

Enhance your communication skills for better personal and professional relationships, with tailored coaching that focuses on your needs.

Find balance, resilience, and well-being in all areas of your life with holistic coaching designed to empower you.

Discover your perfect match : Take our 5-minute assessment and let us pair you with one of our top Coaches tailored just for you.

Find your Coach

Research, expert insights, and resources to develop courageous leaders within your organization.

Best practices, research, and tools to fuel individual and business growth.

View on-demand BetterUp events and learn about upcoming live discussions.

The latest insights and ideas for building a high-performing workplace.

  • BetterUp Briefing

The online magazine that helps you understand tomorrow's workforce trends, today.

Innovative research featured in peer-reviewed journals, press, and more.

Founded in 2022 to deepen the understanding of the intersection of well-being, purpose, and performance

We're on a mission to help everyone live with clarity, purpose, and passion.

Join us and create impactful change.

Read the buzz about BetterUp.

Meet the leadership that's passionate about empowering your workforce.

Find your Coach

For Business

For Individuals

The self presentation theory and how to present your best self

Find my Coach

Jump to section

What does self presentation mean?

What are self presentation goals, individual differences and self presentation.

How can you make the most of the self presentation theory at work?  

We all want others to see us as confident, competent, and likeable — even if we don’t necessarily feel that way all the time. In fact, we make dozens of decisions every day — whether consciously or unconsciously — to get people to see us as we want to be seen. But is this kind of self presentation dishonest? Shouldn’t we just be ourselves?

Success requires interacting with other people. We can’t control the other side of those interactions. But we can think about how the other person might see us and make choices about what we want to convey. 

Self presentation is any behavior or action made with the intention to influence or change how other people see you. Anytime we're trying to get people to think of us a certain way, it's an act of self presentation. Generally speaking, we work to present ourselves as favorably as possible. What that means can vary depending on the situation and the other person.

Although at first glance this may seem disingenuous, we all engage in self-presentation. We want to make sure that we show up in a way that not only makes us look good, but makes us feel good about ourselves.

Early research on self presentation focused on narcissism and sociopathy, and how people might use the impression others have of them to manipulate others for their benefit. However, self presentation and manipulation are distinct. After all, managing the way others see us works for their benefit as well as ours.

Imagine, for example, a friend was complaining to you about   a tough time they were having at work . You may want to show up as a compassionate person. However, it also benefits your friend — they feel heard and able to express what is bothering them when you appear to be present, attentive, and considerate of their feelings. In this case, you’d be conscious of projecting a caring image, even if your mind was elsewhere, because you value the relationship and your friend’s experience.

To some extent, every aspect of our lives depends on successful self-presentation. We want our families to feel that we are worthy of attention and love. We present ourselves as studious and responsible to our teachers. We want to seem fun and interesting at a party, and confident at networking events. Even landing a job depends on you convincing the interviewer that you are the best person for the role.

There are three main reasons why people engage in self presentation:

Tangible or social benefits:

In order to achieve the results we want, it often requires that we behave a certain way. In other words, certain behaviors are desirable in certain situations. Matching our behavior to the circumstances can help us connect to others,   develop a sense of belonging , and attune to the needs and feelings of others.

Example:   Michelle is   a new manager . At her first leadership meeting, someone makes a joke that she doesn’t quite get. When everyone else laughs, she smiles, even though she’s not sure why.

By laughing along with the joke, Michelle is trying to fit in and appear “in the know.” Perhaps more importantly, she avoids feeling (or at least appearing) left out, humorless, or revealing that she didn’t get it — which may hurt her confidence and how she interacts with the group in the future.

To facilitate social interaction:

As mentioned, certain circumstances and roles call for certain behaviors. Imagine a defense attorney. Do you think of them a certain way? Do you have expectations for what they do — or don’t — do? If you saw them frantically searching for their car keys, would you feel confident with them defending your case?

If the answer is no, then you have a good idea of why self presentation is critical to social functioning. We’re surprised when people don’t present themselves in a way that we feel is consistent with the demands of their role. Having an understanding of what is expected of you — whether at home, work, or in relationships — may help you succeed by inspiring confidence in others.

Example:   Christopher has always been called a “know-it-all.” He reads frequently and across a variety of topics, but gets nervous and tends to talk over people. When attending a networking event, he is uncharacteristically quiet. Even though he would love to speak up, he’s afraid of being seen as someone who “dominates” the conversation. 

Identity Construction:

It’s not enough for us to declare who we are or what we want to be — we have to take actions consistent with that identity. In many cases, we also have to get others to buy into this image of ourselves as well. Whether it’s a personality trait or a promotion, it can be said that we’re not who   we   think we are, but who others see.

Example:   Jordan is interested in moving to a client-facing role. However, in their last performance review, their manager commented that Jordan seemed “more comfortable working independently.” 

Declaring themselves a “people person” won’t make Jordan’s manager see them any differently. In order to gain their manager’s confidence, Jordan will have to show up as someone who can comfortably engage with clients and thrive in their new role.

We may also use self presentation to reinforce a desired identity for ourselves. If we want to accomplish something, make a change, or   learn a new skill , making it public is a powerful strategy. There's a reason why people who share their goals are more likely to be successful. The positive pressure can help us stay accountable to our commitments in a way that would be hard to accomplish alone.

Example:   Fatima wants to run a 5K. She’s signed up for a couple before, but her perfectionist tendencies lead her to skip race day because she feels she hasn’t trained enough. However, when her friend asks her to run a 5K with her, she shows up without a second thought.

In Fatima’s case, the positive pressure — along with the desire to serve a more important value (friendship) — makes showing up easy.

Because we spend so much time with other people (and our success largely depends on what they think of us), we all curate our appearance in one way or another. However, we don’t all desire to have people see us in the same way or to achieve the same goals. Our experiences and outcomes may vary based on a variety of factors.

One important factor is our level of self-monitoring when we interact with others. Some people are particularly concerned about creating a good impression, while others are uninterested. This can vary not only in individuals, but by circumstances.   A person may feel very confident at work , but nervous about making a good impression on a first date.

Another factor is self-consciousness — that is, how aware people are of themselves in a given circumstance. People that score high on scales of public self-consciousness are aware of how they come across socially. This tends to make it easier for them to align their behavior with the perception that they want others to have of them.

Finally, it's not enough to simply want other people to see you differently. In order to successfully change how other people perceive you, need to have three main skills: 

1. Perception and empathy

Successful self-presentation depends on being able to correctly perceive   how people are feeling , what's important to them, and which traits you need to project in order to achieve your intended outcomes.

2. Motivation

If we don’t have a compelling reason to change the perception that others have of us, we are not likely to try to change our behavior. Your desire for a particular outcome, whether it's social or material, creates a sense of urgency.

3.  A matching skill set

You’ve got to be able to walk the talk. Your actions will convince others more than anything you say. In other words, you have to provide evidence that you are the person you say you are. You may run into challenges if you're trying to portray yourself as skilled in an area where you actually lack experience.

How can you make the most of the self presentation theory at work?

At its heart, self presentation requires a high-level of self awareness and empathy. In order to make sure that we're showing up as our best in every circumstance — and with each person — we have to be aware of our own motivation as well as what would make the biggest difference to the person in front of us.

Here are 6 strategies to learn to make the most of the self-presentation theory in your career:

1. Get feedback from people around you

Ask a trusted friend or mentor to share what you can improve. Asking for feedback about specific experiences, like a recent project or presentation, will make their suggestions more relevant and easier to implement.

2. Study people who have been successful in your role

Look at how they interact with other people. How do you perceive them? Have they had to cultivate particular skills or ways of interacting with others that may not have come easily to them?

3. Be yourself

Look for areas where you naturally excel and stand out. If you feel comfortable, confident, and happy, you’ll have an easier time projecting that to others. It’s much harder to present yourself as confident when you’re uncomfortable.

4. Be aware that you may mess up

As you work to master new skills and ways of interacting with others,   keep asking for feedback . Talk to your manager, team, or a trusted friend about how you came across. If you sense that you’ve missed the mark, address it candidly. People will understand, and you’ll learn more quickly.

Try saying, “I hope that didn’t come across as _______. I want you to know that…”

5. Work with a coach

Coaches are skilled in interpersonal communication and committed to your success. Roleplay conversations to see how they land, and practice what you’ll say and do in upcoming encounters. Over time, a coach will also begin to know you well enough to notice patterns and suggest areas for improvement.

6. The identity is in the details

Don’t forget about the other aspects of your presentation. Take a moment to visualize yourself being the way that you want to be seen. Are there certain details that would make you feel more like that person? Getting organized, refreshing your wardrobe, rewriting your resume, and even cleaning your home office can all serve as powerful affirmations of your next-level self.

Self presentation is defined as the way we try to control how others see us, but it’s just as much about how we see ourselves. It is a skill to achieve a level of comfort with who we are   and   feel confident to choose how we self-present. Consciously working to make sure others get to see the very best of you is a wonderful way to develop into the person you want to be.

Transform your life

Make meaningful changes and become the best version of yourself. BetterUp's professional Coaches are here to support your personal growth journey.

Allaya Cooks-Campbell

With over 15 years of content experience, Allaya Cooks Campbell has written for outlets such as ScaryMommy, HRzone, and HuffPost. She holds a B.A. in Psychology and is a certified yoga instructor as well as a certified Integrative Wellness & Life Coach. Allaya is passionate about whole-person wellness, yoga, and mental health.

Impression management: Developing your self-presentation skills

How to make a presentation interactive and exciting, 6 presentation skills and how to improve them, how to give a good presentation that captivates any audience, what is self-preservation 5 skills for achieving it, how self-knowledge builds success: self-awareness in the workplace, 8 clever hooks for presentations (with tips), self-management skills for a messy world, developing psychological flexibility, similar articles, how self-compassion strengthens resilience, what is self-efficacy definition, examples, and 7 ways to improve it, what is self-awareness and how to develop it, how to not be nervous for a presentation — 13 tips that work (really), what i didn't know before working with a coach: the power of reflection, self-advocacy: improve your life by speaking up, building resilience part 6: what is self-efficacy, why learning from failure is your key to success, stay connected with betterup, get our newsletter, event invites, plus product insights and research..

3100 E 5th Street, Suite 350 Austin, TX 78702

  • Platform Overview
  • Integrations
  • Powered by AI
  • BetterUp Lead™
  • BetterUp Manage™
  • BetterUp Care®
  • Sales Performance
  • Diversity & Inclusion
  • Case Studies
  • Why BetterUp?
  • About Coaching
  • Find your Coach
  • Career Coaching
  • Communication Coaching
  • Life Coaching
  • News and Press
  • Leadership Team
  • Become a BetterUp Coach
  • BetterUp Labs
  • Center for Purpose & Performance
  • Leadership Training
  • Business Coaching
  • Contact Support
  • Contact Sales
  • Privacy Policy
  • Acceptable Use Policy
  • Trust & Security
  • Cookie Preferences

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Online self-presentation strategies and fulfillment of psychological needs of chinese sojourners in the united states.

Tian Yang

  • 1 School of Overseas Education, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
  • 2 Business School, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China

This study statistically analyzed survey data to examine the relationship between fulfillment of psychological needs of 223 Chinese sojourners in the United States and their online self-presentation strategies on Chinese and American social media. The results showed that the combined use of proactive and defensive self-presentation strategies on Chinese social media instead of American social media were more effective to fulfill the sojourners’ need for autonomy. Moreover, presentation strategies that helped to meet the sojourners’ need for relatedness were significantly different between Chinese and American social media. Specifically, a proactive strategy was more effective to meet sojourners’ need for relatedness on Chinese social media, while a defensive strategy was more effective to fulfill their need for relatedness on American social media.

Introduction

Self-presentation is the core concept of American sociologist Irving Goffman’s Dramaturgy . As an individual’s role-playing behavior of self-expression in interpersonal interaction, self-presentation provides an impetus for self-promotion in real life ( Goffman, 1959 ). Western social psychologists have tested and revised the Goffman’s theory ( Jones and Nisbett, 1971 ), and the impression management theory (IMT) has been developed, which suggests that people apply a series of strategies (such as modification, concealment, and decoration) to control others’ perception of themselves as impression decoration or self-presentation.

With social media widely involved in people’s daily lives, there have appeared an increasing number of studies that are based on the theories of Dramaturgy and the self-determination theory (SDT), analyzing the relationship between online self-presentation behavior and the fulfillment of psychological needs. Online self-presentation is an important part of online social interaction and is influenced by multiple factors such as individual psychology, social context, and social culture. For instance, self-enhancers will selectively choose only positive life events and favorable personal information to share with their social network friends, but other people may entail presenting both positive and negative aspects of the self on social media to reveal their true feelings ( Lee-Won et al., 2014 ; Bareket-Bojmel et al., 2016 ).

In terms of self-presentation and need for relatedness, for example, Deters and Mehl (2013) pointed out that the active self-presentation on Facebook can reduce loneliness; Pittman and Reich (2016) found that compared with text-based platforms, social media users’ presentation on image-based platforms significantly reduced loneliness due to their enhanced intimacy with others. In terms of self-presentation and the need for autonomy, since a more multidimensional space for self-determined behaviors is provided in social media ( Reinecke et al., 2014 ), people can freely present their true selves without being affected by the outside world, therefore meeting their needs for autonomy ( Chen, 2019 ). For immigrants or sojourners, studies have found that they are more inclined to fulfill their autonomy needs through self-presentation on ethnic social media ( Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007 ; Lim and Pham, 2016 ; Pang, 2018 ; Hofhuis et al., 2019 ). Additionally, proactive self-presentation strategies were found to be positively related to the maintenance of psychological well-being ( Swickert et al., 2002 ; Kim and Lee, 2011 ; Ellison et al., 2014 ; Stieger, 2019 ), and in order to obtain more social support, people need to keep a balance between the use of selective and authentic presentational strategies ( Bayer et al., 2020 ).

The psychological effect of online self-presentation has attracted more and more academic attention. However, these studies still remain inconclusive as how people fulfill their psychological needs by means of online self-presentation behavior in intercultural contexts. Specifically, most studies of sojourners are conducted in unitary contexts, either in sojourners’ ethnic social media environments or the social media of the host country, ignoring sojourners’ co-performance in dual-cultural contexts. Moreover, with the growth of the scale of Chinese sojourners, an increasing number of studies have been aimed at them, yet most have focused more on acculturation problems than online self-presentation behaviors. However, online self-presentation has gradually become an important behavior mechanism for Chinese sojourners’ acculturation and communication under the increasing influence of social media. Therefore, it is necessary to fill in the gaps in current research has left and to investigate the logical relationship between the online self-presentation and fulfillment of psychological need of Chinese sojourners in China and America’s dual-cultural contexts.

As important members of intercultural communication groups, Chinese sojourners in the United States are in the dual-cultural contexts of Chinese and American social media, thus they are ideal research participants. In view of this, this study focuses on the following questions:

RQ 1: Do Chinese sojourners mainly use Chinese or American social media to fulfill their psychological needs?

RQ 2: What kinds of presentation strategies are more effective in fulfilling Chinese sojourners’ psychological needs in dual-cultural contexts?

The purpose of this research is to study the logical relationship between online self-presentation strategies and the fulfillment of psychological needs (for autonomy and relatedness) of Chinese sojourners in the context of American and Chinese cultures and to further understand the characteristics of the psychological effects of Chinese sojourners’ online self-presentation behavior in intercultural contexts, so as to provide a new and resourceful way of thinking about maintaining Chinese sojourners’ mental health, as well as helping them to acculturate and communicate more effectively.

Participants and Procedure

This study focused on Chinese sojourners, who are mainly distributed on the east and west coasts of the United States. However, due to factors, such as the uniqueness of sojourners’ identity and their mobility, it is not possible to verify the official statistics on the population data. Therefore, the sampling method used in this study was a nonrandom sampling, and we were utilizing snowball sampling approach to recruit participants.

To be specific, our study initially chose Chinese overseas students, visiting scholars (college teachers and Confucius Institute teachers), and Chinese with a working visa in Washington state in the northwest of the United States as the main sample groups. We applied “Wenjuanxing” (wjx.cn), the most commonly used online questionnaire platform, to send out our questionnaires to people we knew in these three sample groups. We asked them to fill out the questionnaires and distributed the questionnaire link to their interpersonal social networks, including the WeChat groups of Chinese students studying in the United States and visiting scholars in American Colleges and universities, as well as online communities of local American Chinese. Following these procedures, we collected a snowballing sample of 300 questionnaires with responses.

In order to further reduce the error, the study carefully checked the responses to the 300 questionnaires; 29 questionnaires that did not indicate the use of both Chinese and American social media were excluded from the total sample, leaving 223 questionnaires as statistically valid. According to the data analysis of the demographic characteristics of the sample (see Table 1 ), a total of 135 female and 88 male sojourners participated in the survey. In terms of age, they ranged from 17 to 60 years of age, and the number of people aged between 21 and 30 was the biggest (120 people); there were 211 sojourners who had lived in the United States for 1 year or more.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 . Descriptive statistics for demographic characteristics of participants.

Finally, based on the data collected, this study performed a descriptive statistical analysis of self-presentation strategies and psychological needs on Chinese and American social media followed by a regression analysis of the two main variables.

Self-presentation strategies and the fulfillment of psychological needs were two major variables in our questionnaire, and both of them were measured with multiple items that were modified from established scales ( Lee et al., 1999 ; Partala, 2011 ; Chen, 2019 ).

Self-Presentation Strategies

Although there were differences in the classification of self-presentation strategies in the field of psychology at the microlevel, the self-presentation strategies could still be divided into two categories: proactive strategies and defensive strategies ( Goffman, 1959 ; Arkin et al., 1980 ; Tedeschi and Melburg, 1984 ; Fiske and Taylor, 1991 ). Based on this dichotomy and the self-presentation tactic scale developed by Lee et al. (1999) , as well as our empirical observation of Chinese sojourners’ online self-presentation behavior in the United States, this paper specified six presentational tactics, namely “posting selected photos,” “expressing humorous and close content,” and “displaying discipline” for proactive strategies, aimed at actively shaping and maintaining an ideal image and, “expressing controlled feelings,” “self-taunting,” and “reporting only good news” for defensive strategies, aimed at preventing others from depreciating or belittling one’s image. These tactics were measured with six statements; responses were captured on a 5-point Likert scale from “1-never use” to “5-use almost every time.”

Psychological Need Fulfillment

Our measure of the fulfillment of need for autonomy was based on scale for the satisfaction of psychological needs on social networking sites developed by Partala (2011) and was specified with the statements “I feel that my choices express my ‘true self’” and “I have a say in what happens and can voice my opinion.” To measure the fulfillment of need for relatedness, we adapted existing measures of need satisfaction ( La Guardia et al., 2000 ; Ryan et al., 2006 ; Partala, 2011 ) to the intercultural context on social media. Specifically, sojourners mainly maintained and developed three types of relationships in the intercultural context: the relationship with relatives and friends in their home country, the relationship with co-nationals or immigrants of the same cultural background, and the relationship with the locals in the host country ( Lim and Pham, 2016 ; Hofhuis et al., 2019 ; Liu and Kramer, 2019 ). Based on the existing research, this study divided the needs for relatedness of Chinese sojourners in the United States into three categories: first, relational need with domestic relatives and friends, which was stated as “I feel close and connected with my domestic relatives and friends”; second, relational need with Chinese Americans, which was stated as “I feel a sense of contact with Chinese Americans”; third, relational need with Americans, which was stated as “I feel a sense of contact with Americans.” Responses were captured on a 5-point Likert scale from “1-totally disagree” to “5-totally agree.”

In order to understand the basic identity characteristics of Chinese sojourners, this study designed demographic characteristics variables, including “gender,” “age,” “marital status,” “education level,” “time to the United States,” “daily social media use time.” On this basis, this study designed a set of scale to evaluate the online self-presentation behavior of Chinese sojourners in the United States from the overall level. The scale consists of three parts: demographic information, self-presentation strategy, and psychological need fulfillment. Responses were captured with 5-point Likert scales, except for demographic characteristics. Since WeChat and Facebook were the two social media that are most frequently used according to our preliminary study on Chinese sojourners’ general use of social media, this paper chose WeChat and Facebook as the main platforms to observe and analyze the self-presentation behavior of the sojourners. On the basis of quantitative research, this study conducted interviews with 18 Chinese sojourners from all the respondents to understand the logical relationship between self-presentation strategies and fulfillment of psychological needs on Chinese and American social media.

RQ 1: Do Chinese Sojourners Mainly Use Chinese or American Social Media to Fulfill Their Psychological Needs?

In order to answer this question, this study conducted a descriptive statistical analysis of the questionnaire data, and the results are shown in Table 2 . We first calculated the average score of the respondents’ psychological needs on social media in China and the United States and then used a t -test to compare the difference of the average scores between Chinese and American social media. As for “the fulfillment of the need for autonomy,” the results showed that the average score of Chinese social media was significantly higher than that of American social media at the level of 1%, indicating that the self-presentation behavior of Chinese social media was more effective for the fulfillment of Chinese sojourners’ need for autonomy.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 . Fulfillment of the needs for autonomy and relatedness in American and Chinese social media.

In terms of “the fulfillment of the need for relatedness,” the average score of American social media was significantly higher than 3 (a score of 3 represents neutrality), indicating that the development of a relationship with Americans through online self-presentation was significant. In Chinese social media, the average score of “maintaining the relationship with domestic relatives and friends” was significantly higher than 3 at the level of 1%, indicating that Chinese social media had a significant impact on the relationship with family and friends back in China. As for maintaining a relationship with American Chinese, the average score of Chinese social media was significantly higher than that of American social media at the level of 1%, suggesting that the Chinese social media could promote the relationship between sojourners and American Chinese more effectively than American social media.

RQ 2: What Kinds of Presentation Strategies Are More Effective to Fulfill Chinese Sojourners’ Psychological Needs in the Dual-Cultural Contexts?

In order to test the relationship between online self-presentation strategies and the fulfillment of psychological needs, this study further applied a regression analysis after controlling the demographic characteristics of sojourners such as gender, age, marital status, education level, years in the United States, and time spent on social media. The specific regression model was as follows:

Among them, the dependent variable Effect represented the fulfillment of psychological needs (autonomy and relatedness) brought by the online self-presentation behaviors of the Chinese sojourners, and the independent variable Strategy represented the self-presentation strategies including “posting selected photos,” “expressing humorous and close content,” “displaying discipline,” “reporting only good news,” “expressing controlled feelings,” and “self-taunting.” The control variables included the sojourners’ gender ( Gender ), age ( Age ), marital status ( Marriage ), education level ( Education ), length of stay in America ( Years ), and hours spent on social media daily ( Time ). Table 1 illustrates the descriptive statistics for the above demographic characteristics of participants in our regression.

We have found in Table 2 that Chinese sojourners’ self-presentation behavior on Chinese social media is more effective in fulfilling their need for autonomy. Therefore, we conducted a regression analysis on the relationship between the presentation strategies adopted by the sojourners on Chinese social media and their need for autonomy (see Table 3 for the research results). It was found that all six presentation strategies can significantly promote the fulfillment of the sojourners’ need for autonomy but that there are differences in the effectiveness of these strategies. Specifically, for the autonomy dimension of “expressing one’s true self,” the strategy with the most obvious effect was the proactive strategy “expressing humorous and close content,” while for the autonomy dimension of “voicing one’s opinion,” the strategy with the most obvious effect was the defensive strategy “expressing one’s controlled feelings.” It could be seen that in the context of social media in China, the combination of proactive and defensive strategies played a more positive role in meeting the need for autonomy. Through offline interviews, the results of quantitative analysis were further supported. Interviewees have said that the presentation strategy of “expressing humorous and close content” played an important role in arousing emotional resonance and expressing one’s true self; while for important events in personal or social life, using the defensive strategy of “expressing one’s controlled feelings” was more helpful for sojourners to voice his or her opinion in an objective stand and build an intercultural image with the ability of reflection.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 . The effect of different self-presentation strategies on the fulfillment of the need for autonomy in Chinese social media.

The empirical analysis in this paper had shown that the self-presentation behavior was effective in fulfilling sojourners’ need for relatedness in both Chinese and American social media. In order to investigate the differences between presentation strategies used in Chinese and American social media, this paper then conducted a regression analysis of the two platforms’ presentation strategies and fulfillment of sojourners’ needs for relatedness.

For Chinese social media, panel A in Table 4 shows that except “reporting only good news,” the other five presentation strategies have positive effects on maintaining the relationship between sojourners and their domestic relatives and friends. However, there were differences in the effectiveness of these strategies in fulfilling such a need, specifically, the proactive strategies of “expressing humorous and close content” and “displaying discipline” were comparatively more effective in fulfilling sojourners’ need to maintain domestic relationships. Similarly, the results in panel B shows that only the two proactive strategies of “displaying discipline” and “expressing humorous and close content” played an active role in maintaining the relationship between sojourners and Chinese Americans. It could be seen that the self-presentation on Chinese social media, whether to meet the relational needs with domestic relatives and friends or with Chinese Americans, was more effective by adopting proactive presentation strategies. The results of offline interviews further supported the quantitative research results. Interviewees said that “expressing humorous and close content” played an important role in maintaining the relationship with domestic relatives and friends, and this strategy could help them to narrow down the emotional distance with their relatives and friends back in China. At the same time, interviewees often expressed humorous and close content in the WeChat group of “Fellow Countrymen Association,” so as to promote the emotional connection with Chinese Americans. Also, interviewees considered as it necessary to present their “principled” side on Chinese social media and pointed out that “forwarding + commenting” was the most effective way to show the principle. Interviewees said that the strategy of “displaying principle” could help them to shape their self-image of self-discipline, self-reliance, and maintenance of their own cultural identity, thus strengthening the connection with their domestic relatives, friends, and Chinese Americans.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 . The effect of different self-presentation strategies on the fulfillment of the need for relatedness in Chinese social media.

For American social media, the statistical results of Table 5 shows that four presentation strategies played effective roles in developing the relationship between sojourners and Americans, but that there were differences in their degree of effectiveness. According to a ranking of their effect, the top three presentation strategies included two defensive ones, which were “reporting only good news” and “expressing controlled feelings,” and “reporting only good news” served as the most effective strategy to fulfill sojourners’ need for intercultural relatedness. This result was different from the situation on Chinese social media. That was, on Chinese social media, sojourners mainly adopted a proactive strategy to fulfill their need for relatedness with domestic relatives, friends, and Chinese Americans, while on American social media, sojourners preferred to use a defensive strategy to promote the fulfillment of their needs for relatedness. In the offline interview, the interviewees said that the strategy of “reporting only good news” could build a positive impression, activate dialog more quickly, and protect personal privacy. Such strategy conformed to the communication code of conduct on American social media, thus laying a good foundation for the establishment and maintenance of the interpersonal relations between Chinese sojourners and Americans. Additionally, the cultural context of American social media is obviously different from that of Chinese social media. In order to avoid possible cultural misunderstanding or even conflict, the interviewees said that they would control the limit of emotional expression on American social media. The results of interview analysis supported the quantitative research.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 . The effect of different self-presentation strategies on the fulfillment of the need for relatedness in American social media.

Our study recruited 223 Chinese sojourners in the United States as research participants, investigated, and analyzed the relationship between their self-presentation behavior and the fulfillment of their psychological needs (autonomy and relatedness) on Chinese and American social media.

The study shows that, compared with American social media, the self-presentation behavior on Chinese social media can more significantly promote the fulfillment of sojourners’ need for autonomy. This paper holds that the main reason for this difference may be cultural context, that is, Chinese social media are more conducive to the realization of the sojourners’ autonomy. After all, there are cultural values and relational networks that the sojourners are familiar and identified with. The higher the degree of identification and integration with the cultural context, the higher the degree of autonomy of individual actions ( Chirkov et al., 2003 ). In contrast, the cultural context of social media in the United States is relatively unfamiliar and features more heterogeneity. According to SDT, heterogeneity is a reverse force that hinders the realization of autonomy ( Deci and Ryan, 1985 , 2000 ); therefore, compared with the heterogeneous American social media, self-presentation behavior on Chinese social media is more active in promoting the satisfaction of the need for autonomy. Additionally, the results show that Chinese social media play a more active role in maintaining the relationship between sojourners and Chinese Americans than American social media. This result shows that the relatively homogeneous cultural context of Chinese social media provides sufficient emotional and spiritual exchange opportunities, as well as mutual social assistance space for sojourners and Chinese Americans, which is more recognized and adapted by both sides, thus helping to meet the fulfillment of their need for relatedness in the common cultural context ( Lim and Pham, 2016 ; Xiao et al., 2018 ).

This study found that on Chinese social media, the comprehensive use of proactive and defensive presentation strategies helps to meet sojourners’ need for autonomy, which to a certain extent reflects the expediency of Chinese self-presentation behavior ( Zhai, 2017 , p. 56). That is, even when “expressing one’s true self,” sojourners still pay attention to what to say and what not to say, what kind of emotion needs to be expressed and what need not be, which generally reflects that sojourners are striking a balance between sense and sensibility on Chinese social media. At the same time, the sojourners not only distribute and adjust their presentation content but also pay attention to “voicing one’s opinion” through different forms of media, and Chinese social media is technically providing the sojourners with different kinds of effective ways to present ideal self-images and realize autonomous expression.

There are significant differences between Chinese and American social media in the use of self-presentation strategies that help to fulfill sojourners’ need for relatedness. On Chinese social media, a proactive strategy is more effective in meeting sojourners’ need for relatedness, while on American social media, sojourners tend to use a defensive strategy to promote the fulfillment of their need for relatedness. This paper argues that the differences in the connotation of the relationship between Chinese and American cultures affect sojourners’ tendencies when choosing presentation strategies. In the Chinese context, relationship ( guanxi ) is “a kind of social force exerted by family chain and social structure prior to individual existence” ( Zhai, 2011 , p. 187). Individuals must actively maintain important relationships for settling down and gain identification from the social environment at the same time. For Chinese sojourners, their intercultural identity and experiences more intangibly promoting them to adopt proactive presentation strategies on Chinese social media to meet their need for relatedness, because on the one hand, they can help them to consolidate different domestic relationships, and on the other hand, the maintenance of domestic relationships can provide them emotional attachment and a sense of belonging, which help them to alleviate various negative emotions caused by cultural maladjustment.

Compared with the guanxi in China, interpersonal relationships in the American context are clear “role relationships” and have a distinct public-private boundary ( Chu, 1979 ). In the classic social interaction mode with an American-style interpersonal relationship at the core, the means of maintaining and developing the relationship presents very obvious characteristics of instrumental rationality ( Altman and Taylor, 1973 ). Most of the Chinese sojourners who participated in this study came to the United States between 1 and 2 years prior. With the purpose of achieving their specific goals of sojourning in the United States, they needed to develop intercultural interpersonal relationships with local Americans as much as possible; on the other hand, the context of American social media is full of strangeness, heterogeneity, and uncertainty, which made the sojourners more cautious and more aware of all kinds of intercultural communication barriers. Therefore, based on the identification and understanding of the characteristics of relationships in an American context, Chinese sojourners are more likely to adopt a defensive strategy as the main and proactive strategy as the auxiliary to achieve the purpose of fulfilling their need for intercultural relatedness on American social media.

Unlike most previous studies that mainly analyzed the relationship between self-presentation strategies and psychological need fulfillment in a single cultural context, this paper provides empirical evidence for the first time on how self-presentation strategies affect fulfillment of psychological needs in the contexts of dual culture (host and home culture), which provides new inspiration for the study of online self-presentation behavior of sojourners, an important intercultural communication group.

Future Directions

Future research might include empirical research on the relationship between online self-presentation strategies and the satisfaction of Chinese sojourners’ need for competence ( Deci and Ryan, 2000 ) in the United States. In addition, future research might examine how the psychological effects of Chinese sojourners’ online self-presentation behavior affect their offline intercultural adaptation and communication, as well as the acquisition of social capital; such research should be strictly followed by an intercultural analysis of the causes of the general impact. On the basis of empirical research, future research might discuss ways to positively promote the intercultural adaptation and communication of international sojourners, and help sojourners to maintain their psychological well-being in host countries over the long run.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for this study on human participants, which was in accordance with local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TY contributed to research design, theoretical discussion, and manuscript writing. QY contributed to data processing and empirical analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

This work was supported by the Social Sciences General Project of China’s Sichuan Province (SC19B067), the research fund from Sichuan University (2018hhs-24, SCU-SOE-ZY-202008, SKSYL201822, and SCU-BS-PY-202003), and the Youth Fund Project for the Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China (18YJC790204).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Sichuan University and all the funding resources that helped us in the completion of this research.

Altman, I., and Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Google Scholar

Arkin, R. M., Appleman, A. J., and Burger, J. M. (1980). Social anxiety, self-presentation, and the self-serving bias in causal attribution. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 38, 23–25. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.38.1.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bareket-Bojmel, L., Moran, S., and Shahar, G. (2016). Strategic self-presentation on Facebook: personal motives and audience response to online behavior. Comput. Hum. Behav. 55, 788–795. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayer, J. B., Triệu, P., and Ellison, N. B. (2020). Social media elements, ecologies, and effects. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 71, 471–497. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050944

Chen, A. (2019). From attachment to addiction: the mediating role of need satisfaction on social networking sites. Comput. Hum. Behav. 98, 80–92. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.03.034.

Chirkov, V., Ryan, R. M., Kim, Y., and Kaplan, U. (2003). Differentiating autonomy from individualism and independence: a self-determination theory perspective on internalization of cultural orientations and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 97–110. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.97

Chu, G. (1979). “Communication and cultural change in China: a conceptual framework” in Moving a mountain: Cultural change in China. eds. G. Chu and F. L. K. Hsu (Honolulu: The University Press of Hawaii).

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum.

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “What” and “Why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01.

Deters, F. G., and Mehl, M. R. (2013). Does posting Facebook status updates increase or decrease loneliness? An online social networking experiment. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 4, 579–586. doi: 10.1177/1948550612469233

Ellison, N. B., Gray, R., Lampe, C., and Fiore, A. T. (2014). Social capital and resource requests on Facebook. New Media Soc. 16, 1104–1121. doi: 10.1177/1461444814543998

Fiske, S. T., and Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition: From brains to culture. Sage Publishing.

Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: A Division of Random House, Inc.

Hofhuis, J., Hanke, K., and Rutten, T. (2019). Social network sites and acculturation of international sojourners in the Netherlands: the mediating role of psychological alienation and online social support. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 69:120. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2019.02.002

Jones, E. E., and Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning Press.

Kim, J., and Lee, J. -E. R. (2011). The Facebook paths to happiness: effects of the number of Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 359–364. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0374

La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Within-person variation in security of attachment: a self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfillment, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 367–384. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.367

Lee, S., Quigley, B. M., Nesler, M. S., Corbett, A. B., and Tedeschi, J. T. (1999). Development of a self-presentation tactics scale. Pers. Individ. Differ. 26, 701–722. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00178-0

Lee-Won, R. J., Shim, M., Joo, Y. K., and Park, S. G. (2014). Who puts the best “face” forward on Facebook?: positive self-presentation in online social networking and the role of self-consciousness, actual-to-total Friends ratio, and culture. Comput. Hum. Behav. 39, 413–423. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.08.007

Lim, S. S., and Pham, B. (2016). “If you are a foreigner in a foreign country, you stick together”: technologically mediated communication and acculturation of migrant students. New Media Soc. 18, 2171–2188. doi: 10.1177/1461444816655612

Liu, Y., and Kramer, E. (2019). Cultural value discrepancies, strategic positioning and integrated identity: American migrants’ experiences of being the other in mainland China. J. Intercult. Commun. Res. doi: 10.1080/17513057.2019.1679226

Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York: Guilford Press.

Pang, H. (2018). Exploring the beneficial effects of social networking site use on Chinese students’ perceptions of social capital and psychological well-being in Germany. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 67, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2018.08.002

Partala, T. (2011). Psychological needs and virtual worlds: case second life. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 69, 787–800. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2011.07.004

Pittman, M., and Reich, B. (2016). Social media and loneliness. Comput. Hum. Behav. 3, 155–167. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.084.

Reinecke, L., Vorderer, P., and Knop, K. (2014). Entertainment 2.0? The role of intrinsic and extrinsic need satisfaction for the enjoyment of Facebook use. J. Commun. 64, 417–438. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12099.

Ryan, R., Rigby, C., and Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: a self-determination theory approach. Motiv. Emot. 30, 344–360. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8.

Stieger, S. (2019). Facebook usage and life satisfaction. Front. Psychol. 10:2711. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02711

Swickert, R. J., et al. (2002). Relationships among Internet use, personality, and social support. Comput. Hum. Behav. 18, 437–451. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(01)00054-1.

Tedeschi, J. T., and Melburg, V. (1984). “Impression management and influence in the organization” in Research in the sociology of organizations. Vol. 3. eds. S. B. Bacharach and E. J. Lawler (Greenwich, CT: JAI), 31–58.

Xiao, Y., Cauberghe, V., and Hudders, L. (2018). Humor as a double ‐ edged sword in response to crises versus rumors: the effectiveness of humorously framed crisis response messages on social media. J. Conting. Crisis Manag. 26, 247–260. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.12188

Zhai, X. (2011). The principles of Chinese guanxi: Time-space order, life desire and their changes. Peking University Press.

Zhai, X. (2017). The behavioral logic of Chinese. Beijing: Joint Publishing.

Keywords: self-presentation strategies, fulfillment of need for autonomy, fulfillment of need for relatedness, social media, Chinese sojourners

Citation: Yang T and Ying Q (2021) Online Self-Presentation Strategies and Fulfillment of Psychological Needs of Chinese Sojourners in the United States. Front. Psychol . 11:586204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586204

Received: 09 October 2020; Accepted: 29 December 2020; Published: 29 January 2021.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2021 Yang and Ying. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qianwei Ying, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

The Presentation of Self Online

Introduction.

Web users are engaging in computer-mediated self-expression in varying ways. The technology that enables this is developing fast, and how it does so is influenced by many more factors than just the needs of the people it touches. This chapter explores literature about the impact of computer-mediated self-expression on:

  • people's everyday lives.
  • individual self-expression and exploration.
  • interactions and relations with others.
  • interactions with and expectations of society and community.

We ground our discussion in established literature about non-digital self-expression and identity from the social sciences. This raises the key theme of individuals desiring control over how others see us, yet wanting to behave in a way that is authentic, or consistent with their internal identities. There is also emphasis on the collaborative and collective nature of identity formation; that is, our self-presentation fluctuates depending on the people we're with, the situation we're in, and norms of the society we're part of. The focus on face-to-face interactions and embodiment leads us to draw contrasts between online and offline experiences, and to look at the substitutes for the body in digital spaces.

The extent to which online and offline identities interact and overlap is hotly debated. Is creating an online identity a chance to reset, to reshape yourself as an ideal? Or are you simply using it to convey true information about what is happening in your daily offline life? Is it a shallow, picture of you, or a forum for deep self-exploration? How does the way one portrays oneself in digital spaces feedback to ones offline self-presentation? We explore these questions in section 3 .

Section 4 examines social media and blog use, including how one's imagined audience affects self-presentation in public, and how context collapse might occur when the actual audience is different to expected. There are several examples of techniques for managing who sees which 'version' of oneself, and the types of 'versions' of self that are commonly seen to be constructed on social media, and with what degree of transparency they are linked together. Most of the longitudinal studies in this space are of teenagers and young people, who have never known a world without social media, and who may incorporate it naturally and seamlessly into their daily practices, thus making it a core part of their identity during formative years. I draw a contrast between the relationship-driven architecture of contemporary social networking sites, and the more personal, customisable blogging platforms which preceded them. Studies of bloggers and blogging communities reveal some different priorities and habits than what is common practice today, and offer insight into how online self-presentation is evolving.

Throughout literature from both social and computer sciences, privacy is a common concern. In section 5 we look further at how tensions between users and the privacy settings of systems they use impact on personal information disclosure. Does self-censorship affect identity formation? How do people weigh up the risks and benefits of exposing themselves online? This is particularly pertinent for future systems development, as more and more people become aware of state surveillance, for-profit data collection, and their diminished rights over their personal data.

Finally we introduce the relatively new Web Science concept of Social Machines in section 6 in order to recapture the circular interdependencies between humans, technologies, and communities. We propose to build on current work of describing and classifying social machines to better account for the individual perspectives of participants.

Ultimately we posit that online is simultaneously a reflection, a distortion, an enhancement, and a diminishment of the offline world. They impact each other in complex ways, particularly with regards to self-presentation and identity formation. The various theories and studies described in this chapter form the basis for which we conduct the investigative and technical work in the remainder of this thesis.

My perspective on this review

I'd like to take a moment to note that whilst reading various studies about young peoples' reactions to and interactions with rapidly evolving digital technologies from the 2000s, it occurred to me that the subject of these studies is in fact my own age group. Some of the results are instinctively familiar to me; I was there, I experienced these things. Some are ridiculous. I don't know how my first-hand experience of growing up with technology (I was born in the same year as the Web, and my parents were early adopters) affects my reading of these studies, or my ability to study others' use of technology, but it is something I ponder.

Performing the self

The obvious place to start when embarking on a discussion about self-presentation is Goffman [ goffman1959 ]. In The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life , Goffman posits several, now well-established, theories using drama as a metaphor:

  • Everyone is performing. The front-stage of our performance is what we create for others - the audience - to see, so that they may evaluate and interact appropriately with us.
  • We also have a back-stage; how we act when there is no audience, or an audience of our team . Our team participate alongside and collude with us on the front-stage.
  • Our performances have both conscious and unconscious aspects. That is, we consciously give information about ourselves to others in order to manage their impression of us, but we also unconsciously give off information that others may pick up on and take into account when deciding how to interact with us.
  • Both actors and audiences are complicit in maintaining the cohesion of a situation. Performances break down if actors break character, deliberately or accidentally, or if there is a mismatch between parties' definition of the situation.

These theories emphasise the collaborative or social nature of self-presentation, and apply to face-to-face interaction.

Whilst Goffman's dramaturgy refers mostly to body language, a related theory is Brunswik's lens model [ brunswick56 , lens01 ], part of which suggests that individuals infer things about others based on "generated artifacts", or things left behind. In [ bedrooms02 ] this model is used to study how personal spaces (offices and bedrooms) affect observers' assessments of the characteristics of the owner of the space. This study links individuals to their environments by:

  • self-directed identity claims (eg. purposeful decorations like posters or use of colour);
  • other-directed identity claims (eg. decorations which communicate shared values that others would recognise);
  • interior behavioural residue (ie. "physical traces of activities conducted within an environment");
  • exterior behavioural residue (ie. traces of activities conducted outside of the immediate environment which nonetheless provide some cues as to the personality of the environment occupant).

Self-presentation is largely unconscious in the physical realm and comes naturally to most people. People may also use in-crowd markers (like a shirt with a band logo on) consciously to send certain messages to people who will recognise them, whilst not drawing any attention from people who won't [ boydfacid ]. Later in this chapter I look at how our presence in digital spaces fail and succeed to take the place of the physical body when it comes to interactions and identity formation.

https://thesocietypages.org/cyborgology/2013/01/10/context-collapse-a-literature-review/

Social psychologists argue that we come to know ourselves by seeing what we do and how others react to us, and that through interaction, actors seek to maintain the identity meanings associated with each role (Burke and Stets 2009; Cooley 1902; Mead 1934). Indeed, Mead (1934) contends that for each role the actor plays, there is a separate Generalized Other, or larger moral understanding of who the person is and how the person is expected to be in the world, and that social actors manage their roles by adhering to disparate expectations as is situationally necessary. Similarly, Goffman (1959) demonstrates the skillful ways in which social actors reveal and conceal aspects of themselves for varying audiences, maintaining separate faces within distinct social arenas, while Leary (1995) discusses playing to each audience, their values, and their perceived positive opinion of the actor.

The self in context

By reflexively adjusting one’s perception of self in reaction to society, people construct their individual identity. [ boydfacid ]

Development of personal identity is not only something that happens internally. We are strongly influenced by feedback (conscious and unconscious) from others around us, as well as the particular setting and culture in which we find ourselves. How we react to things outside of our control in part determines our identity construction, and some people adjust their behaviour in response to feedback more than others [ snyder74 ]. Thus identity is socially constructed, and often is dynamically adjusted according to context [ boydfacid ].

(self-awareness is identity situated as in society.) A person's understanding of the context in which they are performing impacts their performance [boydfacid]. boyd argues that online, people find it much more difficult to evaluate this context, and thus run increased risk of performing inappropriately, or experience context collapse when multiple audiences are inadvertently combined. This is explored further in section 4 .

…. political, cultural, racial. stuff about web dissolving these [Turkle], but not really. [Kenny] study about people using identity language (with a corpus of terms that corresponds to culture/society), but in content not wrt themselves or in their profiles.

The project of the self

Giddens [ giddens84 ] looks at the relationship between macro and micro views of the world, acknowledging that broader effects of society impact individual behaviour, and vice versa, with neither one being the primary driving force. This suits well my ideas about online self-presentation, confirming the complex interplay between technological affordances, individual actions, and the place of both in a cultural and social context.

Giddens argues that self-identity is an aggregation of a person's experiences, an ongoing account, and a continuous integration of events. In contrast to Goffman's dramaturgy, Giddens downplays the role of an audience, and in contrast to Brunswik's lens theory, he downplays what we can learn from the traces someone leaves behind. Giddens argues that self-identity cannot be uncovered from a moment, but something which is ongoing, over time. Modern society, according to Giddens, affords us more freedom to create our own narratives to determine our self-identity. In the past, rigid social expectations dictated our roles for us. However, increased choices about what to do with ourselves may also increase stress and prove problematic. Awareness of the body is central to awareness of the self, as the body is directly involved in moments we experience in daily life. As we are now explicitly constructing a narrative about our identities, rather than having one ascribed to us by society, the self is an ongoing project which takes work to maintain [ giddens91 ].

The focus on explicit actions and decision making about self-presentation is pertinent when it comes to digital representations of identity.

Extending the self

Early to mid 20th century philosophers and social scientists complicate notions of the 'self' by combining it and extending it with our physical surroundings, and this view emerged long before the Web. Heidegger expresses technology as coming into being through use by a human; when tools are used the tool and its user do not exist as independent entities, but as the experience of the task at hand (using the example of a carpenter hammering, unaware of himself or his hammer) [ manhammer ]. McLuhan discusses media, literate and electronic, from the printing press and electric light to radio, TV and telephone, and its impact on how we communicate. He places communication technologies as simultaneously extensions of and amputations of our bodies and senses, which continuously and fundamentally re-shape the way we (humans) see and place ourselves the world [ mcluhan ]. More recently, Clark's Extended Mind Theory uses the example of a notebook as a means of externally processing information that would otherwise be carried out by the brain, drawing the external world in as party to our cognitive processes [ clarkmind ].

The next logical step is to consider how the modern digital technologies of Web and social networking can also be considered extensions of the self, and this is addressed in part by Luppicini's notion of Technoself [ technoself ]. Technoself incorporates (amongst other things) extension of the self through physical technology embedded in the body (cyborgology); in our changing understanding of what it is to be , as life is extended and augmented through advancing healthcare; but also in our relationships with our virtual selves. This is not a topic into which I will dive deeply from a philosophical standpoint, but the idea of the Web and online social networks as extensions to the self rather than as separate entities or concepts is worth bearing in mind as this thesis proceeds to explore the complexities of intertwined digital and offline identities.

maybe something to do with hyperreality, Baudrillard, Simulacra and Simulation

Offline to online… and back again

When people use digital technologies to communicate, they are passing a version of themselves through the filter of the platform they use. In this section I discuss the relationships between online and offline selves.

One might assume that the disembodied nature of the interactions lets people have more control over how they are "seen" by others; Turkle claims that it is "easy" to create and tweak a perfect self online [ Turkle2015 ]. In reality this control is affected by a great number of factors. Turkle writes that computer-mediated communication is predictable even in the ways it is unpredictable, and that people seek out this predictability in preference to face-to-face interactions, and in preference to facing emotional vulnerability. Turkle's argument assumes that we have full understanding and control of the digital systems we use and the audiences we are reaching through them. It presumes we know exactly how and where and when the data we input will be output in the short and long term, and how others will interpret it. given off by users as a side effect of scrolling, typing, tapping and clicking in their browser and mobile apps which is collected in the background by a plethora of third-parties. This generates a view of the individual that they may not even realise is being compiled, and over which they certainly have no control. Further, the affordances of the technical platform may dictate how a user is able to actively express themselves, or influence what they want to share on a subconscious level. We will discuss both of these issues in more detail later in this chapter. -->

Turkle's argument neglects that at every crossroad in these 'predictable' systems are stationed unpredictable humans, perhaps with conflicting interests and motivations, from the conception of a social system, to its realisation and use. Turkle has been studying for decades how people explore, experiment and find themselves through technology, and her overriding narrative is of a desire to express an idealised version of the self; one that is not subject to any interpretation other than what the expresser desires. However, as discussed further in the next section numerous studies of social media users find a variety of other types of motivation for participation.

Keen [ digitalvertigo ] on the other hand emphasises the risks that individuals become trapped by technology of which they have neither understanding nor control. Being swept up in cultural technology trends, social media users may unwittingly become "prisoners" of a carefully curated digital "hyperreality", the importance of which supplants their offline lives. Similarly, [ inreallife ] suggests that "fantasy gets in the way of real progress" when it comes to self improvement, but studies only examples of "catfishing", where individuals create exaggerated online profiles in order to deliberately mislead potential romantic interests. The idea that the online self replaces rather than supplements the offline self also misses the nuances of how and why people use social media in the first place. Nonetheless, I agree that online worlds are certainly not just a mirror of the offline.

On the contrary, not only are online identities some form of reflection of offline identities, but the inverse can be true as well. In ethnographic studies of teenagers' use of mobile apps for socialising, it is reported that crafted online personas both reflect and feed back into teenagers' true sense of self [ appgen ]. Numerous studies from the field of psychology, reported in [ infinitereality ], demonstrate various ways in which playing a role online or in a virtual world reflects back and directly changes people's offline behaviour. Avatars can be used to build confidence and reveal suppressed personality traits. Relatedly, study participants who were asked to interact from behind avatars which conveyed different appearances of age, race, and body type expressed affinity with these previously unfamiliar experiences, and responded differently in personality evaluations before and after. A longitudinal study of teenage girls roleplaying online, a process through which they developed their identity through narrative, revealed positive impacts on their confidence, through new friendships [ youth ].

There is a long history of assessments of online interactions which proclaim that the self-centered nature of social media makes people narcissistic, that competition for reputation isolates us [ digitalvertigo ], and that the construction of an ideal self or facade is damaging, especially to young people. However, [ appgen ] argues that narcissism is not created by the Web, but enabled by it, as an existing need for validation is more readily satisfied.

Several studies [ insta17 , Rousseau17 , haferkamp10 ] find that people's self confidence or body image drops after viewing the online profiles of people who appear to be more attractive or more successful than them. [ wellbeing17 ] explains that passive use of social media is what appears to have a negative impact on people, whereas active use has a positive effect on well-being. This is shown in several studies, including [ toma10 ], which explores the beneficial effects of browsing one's own Facebook profile; [ nemer15 ] which discusses how selfies can empower marginalised communities; and [ noland06 ] which finds a positive impact on self-esteem of teenaged girls who engage in "auto-photography".

It is also worth bearing in mind that experience in the virtual world can cause physical reactions - laughter, tears - and the virtual and the physical blur together in the subject stream of experiences, adding to an identity which is made of virtual and physical events blended together [ youth ].

Authenticity and integrity

In an interview, Facebook founder Zuckerberg said that "having two identities for yourself is an example of lack of integrity" [ fbeffect ]. This received public backlash at the time, and on several more occasions as Facebook and other social networking sites imposed real name policies, sometimes linked to an official ID [ boyd2012politics ]. This removes a level of identity control which many people take for granted. A particularly clear example can be seen in the reaction of a community of drag queens who were used to being able to interact online using chosen names. Real name requirements removes a vital aspect of fluidity from their interactions, glossing over the seams which provide poignant performance material and a route towards a group identity [ lingel2015face , barmann14drag ]. Additionally the requirement for real names and the ability to report 'fake names' became a mechanism whereby a marginalised group could be harassed and silenced [ hotmess2015selfies ]. In general, an attitude that people should be happy to connect all of their identities together under a single legal name is an expression of social privilege: a result of having no features or proclivities which are socially censored.

Sandberg, whilst COO of Facebook in 2012, commented that profiles as detailed self portraits is a "shift towards authenticity" [ digitalvertigo ], but her organisation's notion of 'integrity' as a single complete version of oneself that is the same no matter to whom one is presenting is somewhat at odds with authenticity. Neither are people "intraviduals", caught between competing identities as claimed by Conley in 2009 [ conleyintra ], but expressing aspects of themselves appropriately and according to context. This is behaviour which we have already established via Goffman as ordinary offline, and so shouldn't be considered unusual online. One's 'authentic' self-presentation may be partial or moderated and no less genuine for that. Indeed, some individuals find they are more able to express their authentic selves online than they are offline due to oppression or social expectations which are disjoint from their core values.

However, in highly commercialised or competitive online environments, 'authenticity' is a quality to strive for, to maintain an audience. A Web search for 'authenticity on social media' will reveal a plethora of guides on how to craft an 'authentic' persona, how to maintain personal-but-not-too-personal ties with one's audience so that they see that you're just like them. For individuals who set out to explore and express their identity online, this can be a tricky world to navigate [ duffy15b ]. True authenticity in online communities is seen as disjoint with self-promotion and celebrity; popularity implies a reduction in authenticity, perhaps linked to 'selling out' or 'pandering' to an audience [ donathboyd04 , ellison07 ]. Whereas authenticity is often seen from an outside perspective as always something manufactured, an idealized reality [ Marwick2010 ].

The idea that online spaces are under control of their owners suggests others may be suspicious of their authenticity [ tong08 ] but reinforcement of social identity from others can counteract this. Warranting theory describes how information that appears to be outside of the subject's control—for example, a message posted publicly by a friend on someone's profile—can reinforce the trustworthiness of the other profile data to an outside observer [ warrant , warrant1 , warrant2 ].

"strive to be honest and honerable" [inreallife]

Dishonesty and deception

"Some argue that distinct contexts are unnecessary and only encourage people to be deceptive. This is the crux of the belief that only those with something to hide need privacy." - [ boydnp10 ]

Most people like to consider themselves to be quite honest in their communications with friends, family and acquaintances. However, even honest people routinely modulate what they share, omitting and sometimes falsifying information in order to reduce social friction, avoid confrontation, defuse awkward situations, or to save face [ buller1996interpersonal , burgoon1989maintaining ]. Hancock et. al. introduced the term butler lies to refer to a common use of simple lies to manage communications, such as smoothly exiting from an unwanted conversation [ hancock2009butler ]. Online, the notion of who our 'friends' are has become increasingly blurred and difficult to define. In such settings, people commonly navigate different social spaces, projecting and varying self-presentation according to the ways they want to be perceived by each [ marwick2010TweetHonestly ].

Whilst part of tailoring one's presentation to an audience is the ability to carry out some level of deception , with personal communications, there is an implicit expectation of authenticity [ aoki2005Ambiguity ]. However, online, the need to navigate multiple and uncertain audiences means that we may constantly vary our self-presentation. Authenticity becomes a social construct derived from the social context and how we wish to be perceived by a given audience [ boyd2002faceted ]. We may be deceiving, at least to some extent, nearly constantly without even being conscious of it.

Deception has long been studied, both within and outwith the HCI community. Traditionally, deception has been cast in a negative light [ bok1978lying ], to be used only if no other option is available. In the 1980s, however, communications researchers began to investigate the positive aspects of lying, in particular white lies - socially acceptable lies which cause little or no harm to the recipient [ camden1984WhiteLies ].

In 1992, McCornack cast deception as an understandable response to complexity: "[r]esearchers studying deception recently have begun to argue that deceptiveness is a message property that reflects a kind of functional adaptation to the demands of complex communication situations" [ mccornack1009InformationManipulation ]. People then manipulate the information which they share as a necessary part of participation in society. This has led to recent work on the positive aspects of deception in human computer interaction, in particular how butler lies are used to ease social situations [ hancock2009butler ], and how systems can deceive their users for beneficial reasons [ adar2013Benevolent ].

Several different taxonomies of lying and deceptive behaviours have been proposed [ camden1984WhiteLies , depaulo1996lying , lindskold1983categories ]; Anolli et al. examined a family of deceptive miscommunications, including self-deception and white lies [ anolli2001DeceptiveMiscommunication ]. They look at omission of relevant information, concealment using diversionary information, falsifaction and masking with alternative, false information. Of particular interest is their claim that "a deceptive miscommunication theory should be included in a general framework capable of explaining the default communication", that is that deception should not be seen as a psychologically different activity than 'normal' communication. This tallies with the earlier approach of McCornack [ mccornack1009InformationManipulation ] who situates deceptive messages within the spectrum of information manipulation . This, combined with the lens of Gricean conversational maxims, allows for an explanation of deceptions where some of the truth is told, but information which the speaker knows is relevant to the listener is omitted or obscured [ grice1970logic ].

Motivations for lying have also been extensively studied in social psychology. Turner et al.'s taxonomy included saving face ; guiding social interaction; avoiding tension or conflict; affecting interpersonal relationships; and achieving interpersonal power [ turner1975information ]. Camden et. al. [ camden1984WhiteLies ] develop a detailed categorisation of lies to do with basic needs, managing affiliation with others, self-esteem and miscellaneous practices such as humour and exaggeration.

Many malicious or undesirable behaviours are facilitated by the ability to create and alter identities. Astroturfing [ cho2011astroturfing ] has become common online [ zhang2014astroturfing ], with corporations and governments employing sophisticated identity management software to carry out large scale operations. Possibly the most famous of this is the 50 Cent Party , hired by the government of the People's Republic of China to post favourable comments towards party policy [ fiftycent ]. On a smaller scale, sock-puppets — multiple accounts controlled by a single person — are used to skew ideas of consensus and distort discussion in online societies, leading to attempts to automatically identify such accounts [ bu2013sock , solorio2013case ]. Personas can be constructed for the purpose of trolling , whether it is overtly offensive in order to cause outrage or more subtle manipulation to trick people into wasting effort or taking caricatured positions, and correlations have been shown between enjoyment of trolling and everyday sadism [ buckels2014trolls ].

Many of these activities are a form of obfuscation , in some way hiding the truth, polluting the data pool and diminishing trust. The ethical issues here are complex and contextual, with the viewpoints of different actors having considerable divergence [ brunton2011vernacular ].

Another strand of research borrows from information warfare, to look at the possibilities for disinformation. Disinformation tactics are most useful when a channel of information cannot be completely closed, but can be rendered useless by being filled with incorrect, but plausible, assertions in order to lower its overall signal-to-noise ratio [ wiki:disinformation ]. The intended target of the lie may not be the official recipient of the message: lies can be directed at those who are eavesdropping on the communications channel or surveilling the participants [ alexander2010Disinformation ]. Techniques used include redaction to remove parts of the message, airbrushing to blur parts of the message and blending to make the message similar to other plausible messages, as well as other forms of information distortion [ alexander2010Disinformation ].

In chapter 3 I carry out two studies which aim to bring together these general theories of deceptive behaviour with a closer look at how and why people might engage in them online.

Networked publics

Social media technologies blur the boundaries between private and public, and this affects identity performance. Ten years ago, boyd hoped that educators and technologists would succeed in easing the cultural transition for young people into the networked era [ boyd-sns07 ]. She describes social networking sites as a type of "networked publics", technologically-mediated spaces where people can virtually go to interact with their friends, and where they may be subject to observation or interjection by passers-by. Differently to offline public spaces, online publics may be persistent, scalable, searchable, replicable, and/or have invisible audiences. These features of networked publics affect how people express themselves and interact, however they do not directly dictate participants' behaviour [ boydnp10 ]. Networked publics are not only spaces, but collections of people or "imagined community"; different publics can serve different purposes, but can also intersect with each other [ boyd2014s ].

"Networked publics must be understood in terms of “publics,” a contested and messy term with multiple meanings that is used across different disciplines to signal different concepts ... In short, I contend that networked publics are publics that are restructured by networked technologies; they are simultaneously a space and a collection of people ... My approach accepts the messiness and, instead, focuses on the ways in which networked technologies extend and complicate publics in all of their forms ... publics that have been transformed by networked media" [boydnp10]

Over subsequent years, boyd and many others proceed to explore the effects of these differences on those who engage with online social media to different degrees. In this section I recount some of these studies and findings.

A benefit of participation in networked publics is that a wider variety of communities are accessible than offline. Niche identities don't have to be set aside to fit in [ appgen ]. Online interactions are "not simply a dialogue between two interlocutors, but a performance of social connection before a broader audience" [ boyd2014s ]. boyd looks specifically at teenagers in networked publics, who she says have sought online spaces in recent years as they are not allowed to 'hang out' any more in physical spaces like malls [ boyd2014s , marwickdrama14 ].

cues by social partners affect observers' impressions of the profile owner. Profiels offer interactive and static features and are complicated by input of others. Less control by initiator.

"Comments are not simply a dialogue between two interlocutors, but a performance of social connection before a broader audience. " [boydnp10]

[modern American] teens create their own publics. Digital spaces because they're not allowed in physical ones (like malls). Being themselves, to see and be seen. "What makes a particular site more or less public is... how it is situated within the broader social ecosystem." [boyd2014s]

performance in public. 'drama' as a defense mechanism for teens. Saving face, get attention, manipulate perceptions of others. Larger audience than school halls. distinguish from adult-defined practices like bullying, so for autonomy and control. elicit support. [marwickdrama14]

reconceptualise audiences as co-present and participatory. misalignment potential increases with SNS. [baym12]

change from bi to unidirectional connects as norm with Twitter. asymmetrical disclosure. reinforces weak ties, lowers barrier to communication. [ellison13]

First I reflect on the digital substitutions for the physical body in online social interactions. Then, in comparing and contrasting 'old school' style blogging with contemporary (circa 2013-2017) social networking sites I look deeper into how differences in technological affordances impact peoples' interactions and self-expression.

Audiences for identity performance as well as the context in which the performance takes place are critical, but online both of these may be unknown or dynamic, or both. I'll introduce work around imagined audience and context collapse, both of which pioneer our understanding of identity behaviours in networked publics. When audiences and contexts are known, we can examine how people connect with others and form communities; in the final section I look at trust, social reinforcement of identity, and studies of what social media participants choose to disclose or conceal.

Unfortunately, networked publics appear to reproduce many of the biases that exist in other publics-social inequalities, including social stratification around race, gender, sexuality, and age, are reproduced online (Chen and Wellman, 2005; Hargittai, 2008). Political divisions are also reproduced (Adamic and Glance, 2005)" [boydnp10]

humans are reduced into packages of information... digital is separate from true humanness [digitalvertigo]

Profiles and embodiment

In Faceted Id/entity [ boydfacid ], boyd highlights several differences between self-representation offline compared to online. Embodiment is a key factor in self-presentation and she claims that there is considerable difference between performing one's identity through appearance, eg. fashion and body language, when walking into a room, compared to explicitly describing oneself by entering attributes and other personal information into an online form to create a profile. The disembodied nature of online interactions means that people must find new ways to express themselves, and manage the impressions other people have of them, or "a new type of body" [ youth ]. A lack of control over one's online self presentation is compounded by the inability to visualise - or perhaps even be aware of - the data that is collected by the systems we use. Online activities are logged over time to an extent that most individuals are not aware of; these activities, an individual's expressions given off (Goffman), are used, largely unknowingly, for the commercial benefits of third parties; this constitutes a kind of implicit or unconscious profile. boyd suggests that visualising all of one's personal information that is available online, as well as visualising one's 'audience' or social network connections, would provide an individual with better awareness of, and so better control over, their online image. In boyd's prototype interfaces, users are explicitly asked for personal data in order to build a profile of themselves, and boyd does point out the problematic nature of this, compared with the unconscious or implicit identity performance one conducts in offline social settings.

Counts in [ counts09 ] explores the impact of profile attribute selection on self presentation, and finds that upon completing the values for 10 attributes, participants converge on their "ideal" representation of themselves. This study also finds that free-form attributes are better than ones with preset choices for participants' satisfaction with how they have portrayed themselves. This study does not take into account that most online profiles are created in a particular context, with a particular purpose in mind. Asking participants to express their ideal self-presentation 'in general' vastly oversimplifies reality. Participants are not told who the consumers of the profile they are creating are expected to be, or how it is to be presented; nor are participants given an opportunity to indicate who their expected audience is or what they think the profile is for.

Since boyd's prototypes were designed, social media gained widespread popularity. Most, if not all, mainstream systems request input of explicit personal data to build an initial profile, despite the discord of this activity compared with offline identity expression. However, unlike in boyd's prototypes, it doesn't stop there. Such systems encourage ongoing engagement through adding and messaging contacts with various degrees of publicness, creating status updates to broadcast a current situation, production of creative media content, and feedback on content and updates created by others. As we will see in chapter 3 , contemporary social media builds one's profile from various combinations of these online activities, and typically use far more than the explicit data entered by the user to generate a representation of a person. This increases the likelihood that individuals may not have an accurate impression of what this representation looks like to others.

Expression through avatars, design, visuals - pull from blogging and sns studies

Recent studies confirm that visuals are a key part of expressing identity online. Many focus on selfies as a modern substitute for the body [ vanhouse11 , lasen15 , senft15 , frosch15 ], but [ bunnies17 ] examines self-presentation through other kinds of photos. Examples include humorous images from popular culture or photographs of other things with an overlayed caption, coupled with a tag (eg. #currentstate) that indicates the poster relates to this concept; as well as photos of items that people carry with them day to day. In [ papatwit12 ], self-expression is performed through use of Twitter hashtags, and [ food15 ] suggests that food photography is a means of self-presentation.

Pointing at something and saying that one has chosen it as self-representative makes the assemblage of tags, text, and image a culturally intelligible self-representation [ bunnies17 ].

A lack of embodiment can also have a distinct advantage. In [ stendal12 ] several studies of people with disabilities who use online social systems are reviewed, and reveal findings about increased control over disclosure of disability (which may not be possible offline) and reduced isolation when people are able to interact online.

Beyond simply visualising all of one's personal information, and the connections between social network contacts to show how personal data may flow through a network, boyd suggests that these visualisations can be used to actively segregate one's contacts along parameters which correspond to one's personal information, permitting an individual to perform identity more appropriately in digital spaces. However in practice, designing user interfaces for this proves to be challenging. For one, people aren't used to explicitly thinking about audience and context, so dividing their contacts up this way is not natural.

Transparency of the collection of any of this data, and control over how it is distributed are key to boyd's thesis of how to empower individuals online. Sadly, the situation here has not improved in the intervening years. Indeed, private corporations are collecting more and more personal data through free social networking sites, and granting the subjects zero rights with regard to access or distribution. Later in this chapter we elaborate on how privacy concerns impact online disclosure and profile creation behaviours; in chapter 3 we build on the existing literature with an empirical study of a community of hackers intent on decentralised and personally-owned data storage as a means to greater control.

Next we look more closely at the behaviour of users of Social Network Sites, of which "profiles" are a key feature [ boydsns07 ].

Social Network Sites

In 2007 boyd and Ellison defined Social Network Sites (SNS) to be Web-based, bounded, public or semi-public, and afford creating and viewing connections with or between other users [ boydsns07 ]. They note that users of these systems tend to connect with others with whom they already have a 'real life' social relationship, and present a fairly thorough history of SNS from 1997 onwards, which I won't recount here. This definition is pertinent to this thesis due to its emphasis on profiles, implying self-presentation, as a core feature of SNS. In 2013 they updated their definition to incorporate different types of content and data into profiles; to de-emphasise the traversal of connections (as this became more important to machines than humans); and to emphasise participants' interaction with streams of user-generated content [ ellison13 ]. I will proceed to focus on case studies and experiments which were carried out since these definitions, and due to the rapid pace of change in this area, prioritise those from the last four years.

SNS have: profiles, friends lists, public communication, stream-based updates." [boydnp10]

Many studies of identity formation on social media focus on young people and teenagers. One reason is because this is a crucial point in life for understanding oneself and asserting a personal identity. Other studies approach teenagers as somewhat alien "digital natives", born into a world of social technology which is expected to fundamentally change how they interact with the world compared with older generations, who don't or can't distinguish between online and offline [ borndigi ]. I will relate the results of these studies, but note that I disagree with the notion of a "digital native" because being born in a particular year or even raised around modern technology does not automatically give one a natural instinct for identity expression in digital spaces, and not even necessarily more opportunity to experiment and reflect than older SNS users [ boyd2014s ].

Digital communication technologies can help or hinder identity formation. The App Generation [ appgen ] provides a balanced argument between the pros and cons of teenagers socialising through mobile applications. They find that some applications provide a "prepackaged identity" for users to adopt rather than encouraging experimentation. The affordances of applications shape the forms of expression that are available, and so identity formation is in a way controlled by the application designer. Born Digital [ borndigi ] suggests that teenagers experiment with identity online, but aren't fully aware, or don't care about, the traces that are left behind when they do so.

In some cases, for example fan communities, self-presentation shifts between a more playful fictional identity performance, and an identity which is closer to 'real life' [ baker09 ]. In others, such as professional self-presentation, individuals lean on automatically generated metrics by the system they use to convey a positive image, with gamification or commodification of the self becoming commonplace [ academicqs16 ]. When SNS provide a platform for professionalising passions such as content creation, [ duffy15 ] notes that participants may be even more vulnerable to the consequences of performing and maintaining one's self-presentation in an exposed online space, as well as the "labour of visibility" that goes into it.

Most people occupy multiple roles offline, find ways to establish and maintain boundaries between them, and continue to do so to different degrees when taking representations of these roles to online spaces. SNS increase the permeability of boundaries, but users employ various tactics to manage their identity when a one-identity-per-person model is imposed on them [ quinn15 ].

[ singh15 ] describes how Twitter users subvert features of the system to express themselves in new ways, as well as reflecting on how changes to the functionality of they system affect how people use it. This supports [ papatwit12 ] which, through content analysis of trending hashtags, also describes how people work around technical constraints of Twitter to meet their self-presentation needs.

Even in the early days of SNS (specifically Facebook) beginning to rise to popularity when use was overflowing into the workplace, the access control settings offered by Facebook were considered too complicated to enable most people to realistically manage connections with both professional and personal contacts from a single profile, despite the potential advantages of connecting with colleagues through the platform [ DiMicco07 ]. More recently, we see that SNS users manage tensions between their multiple roles and the affordances of systems by segregating their audience across multiple platforms. The interview study in [ Zhao16 ] found that sharing decisions across multiple sites are made primarily based on the known audiences of the different sites, and the content being shared. This study also recounts previous work on motivations for using different SNS, including to connect with old friends, and share pictures, which feed into decisions taking regarding content sharing. A similar study found that family was a crucial audience to whom more private sharing was desired [ Farnham11 ], and findings in [ Vitak14 ] indicate that Facebook users desire to re-asssert their offline boundaries when online, and concurs that managing this through the tools that Facebook provides is cost-intensive. Facebook itself compounds this issue by using identity information as a "social lubricant" which encourages people to make new connections [ ellison11fb ].

In Alone Together, she says people are reduced to profiles

is definition from [ellison13] still good?

A social network site is a networked communication platform in which participants 1) have uniquely identifiable profiles that consist of user-supplied content, content provided by other users, and/or system-provided data; 2) can publicly articulate connections that can be viewed and traversed by others; and 3) can consume, produce, and/or interact with streams of user-generated content provided by their connections on the site.

Blogging and personal homepages

Personal homepages and blogs have been around for considerably longer than SNS, though remain a comparatively specialist practice. It is widely accepted that blog or website owners have more control over their online space than do users of SNS, [ intlblog , markus06 ], including freedom to innovate with the site's appearance and thus explore more individualistic aspects of the online self [ alist05 ]. Relatedly, communities of bloggers are not owned or controlled by a single entity [ dennen09 ].

Through observations of over 200 blogs within a particular community and semi-structured interviews with 40 bloggers, [ dennen09 ] identifies five aspects which affect how bloggers build their identity: name and blog title; descriptive attributes; post content; voice; affiliations; and visual design. All of these are subject to change over time, and sometimes major offline transitions can cause a shift to a new pseudonym or blog altogether; often the audience is invited along however. Blogs are often designed to reinforce community norms, to enhance a sense of belonging; as a result, the community develops and evolves its own identity, which in turn influences how newcomers choose to present themselves. Bloggers' contributions are fragmented across different domains, and where their writing style and topics constitute a part of their identity, so it fragments their identity.

Blogging communities are traditionally more accepting of pseudonyms but [ dennen09 ] notes that distrust is not of other community members, but rather of personally-known community outsiders who may accidentally stumble across blog entries.

"homepages became an important site of identification, as early web users posted biographies, photos, and entertaining links for viewers who surfed to their page (Döring, 2002) [ellison13]"

Earlier in this chapter I mentioned Brunswik's lens model which describes a way in which identity can be constituted through physical traces left behind. This model has also been used to understand how observers make personality judgments about people based on the traces left in their digital space , ie. personal homepages [ gosling08 , markus06 , vazire04 , papacharissi02 ].

The importance of themes and designs of blogs and homepages is emphasised by [ dennen09 ], who mentions that whilst some blog consumers use a feed reader to receive new content from the blogs they are interested in, they often click through to the original post to view it in the context of the author's own space.

On the other hand, [ blogdesign ] takes a snapshot of a random sample of blogs in 2003 and maps the state of the blogosphere through analysing visual elements in depth and tracking commonalities. The conclusion is that significant customisation of blog templates was in fact relatively rare, with most people only slightly tweaking colours or adding custom images. A likely explanation for this is that bloggers lacked the technical expertise to do so.

Studies of blogging communities outside of the US demonstrate that blogging is not a uniform practice that can be understood as a whole [ intlblog ]. Certain communities (in this case, Muslim ones) which are seen by outsiders as homogeneous use blogging to highlight their uniqueness and individuality. Others (for example in China) emphasise their ethnicity and culture as a key part of their identity. Blogs from the Paris Banlieues in fact had a direct impact on how the mainstream media portrayed their plight; an example of how personal identity expression in networked publics was able to affect a broader social understanding of that identity. The overriding message from these studies is cultural taboos and offline societal context affect narrative about identity, and this is reflected online.

[papa04blog] is a content analysis of blogs, and suggests that self expression through blogging is spontaneous

Imagined audience

The audience to whom one performs is critical in forming the context in which one is performing [ boydnp10 ]. On SNS, people are often expressing themselves to multiple audiences simultaneously. When people are aware of this, they take different strategies when it comes to navigating what they share; individuals with many followers on Twitter practiced self-censorship (only posting things they are happy for the worst-case audience to read) and practicing coded communication (strategically targeting some posts at some audience members, and others at others, to maintain overall interest) [ Marwick2010 ].

However, given the many possible ways in which Tweets can be discovered and consumed ([ Marwick2010 ] questioned people who post publicly) it is virtually impossible to determine the actual audience for one's content. Thus, people imagine who their audience is likely to be, and express themselves accordingly. Obviously these imaginings, which may stem from understanding of the affordances of a particular platform, or a particular community or topic of discussion, impact how people express themselves online.

[ litt12 ] theorises about how the imagined audience is synthesised, and draws in Giddens' structurational framework, noting a combination of macro- (social roles, technical affordances) and micro-level (individual motivations, technical skills) factors. [ litt12 ] ultimately concludes that asking people about their imagined audience is prone to errors or misinterpretations, as imagined audience is a concept which is both difficult to measure and difficult to express.

Relatedly, as people perform in networked publics, they must contend with a "networked audience," who are not connected only with the performer, but also with each other [ Marwick2010 ].

Context collapse

I have so far discussed how people attempt to map boundaries from their offline lives into their online interaction spaces, and the notion of imagined audience. Context collapse occurs when boundaries come down and personas intended for different audiences are merged [ Marwick2010 ]. The consequences of this may range from slight social awkwardness, to direct breaches of privacy and potential danger, and have been examined in a variety of different circumstances, such as [ daviscontext , contextwesch , duguay14 , duguay13 ].

Thanks to the properties of networked publics such as searchability and persistence, contexts may also collapse when information is consumed later, or through a different systems, whereby it may be interpreted differently by the consumer than how it was originally intended [ boydnp10 ].

As we look forward to how SNS and online self-presentation in general will evolve, we must consider how the lines people have drawn around their contexts are tethered to particular (versions of) systems. What happens when these systems change, merge, or disappear? As designers of new systems, we must be cognisant of the role technical affordances play in creating, enabling, and destroying social boundaries.

Everybody knows I’m a dog

"While once viewed as a set of technologies built in resistance to the ugliness of the dot-com era, social media is now intertwined with neoliberal capitalism and data surveillance" - [ boyd15 ]

We are rapidly moving into a world where information about nearly every aspect of our lives is becoming sensed, recorded, captured and made available in digital form. Data is captured and shared voluntarily, as tools invite ever more intimate participatory surveillance [ albrechtslund2008Participatory ]. While the abundance of information traces has unlocked a wide range of new kinds of applications (eg. [ Akker:2014:TRP:2684563.2684638 , Consolvo:2008:ASW:1357054.1357335 ]), the creation and potential for disclosure poses new threats to individual privacy and autonomy. The overall lack of transparency by manufacturers regarding how they are capturing and handling personal information has created a heightened sense of unease among many, in addition to the potential threats dealing with their unintentional disclosure or misuse [ fife2012privacy , metzger2004privacy , featherman2010reducing ].

Various data and surveillance scandals involving private companies and governments [ dcent , crit12 ] that gained media attention mean that awareness of surveillance and personal data collection is growing amongst the general public. There are many studies examining peoples' awareness of and attitudes towards privacy and surveillance on SNS, but I will not detail them here. During studies in the 1990s, Westin defined three categories to describe how ordinary people feel about privacy: "pragmatists", "fundamentalists" and "unconcerned." Privacy pragmatists accept that there may be tradeoffs between benefits to information sharing and the intrusiveness of requests for information. Fundamentalists distrust organisations which request personal data. The unconcerned are comfortable with sharing personal data with organisations in exchange for services [ krane2002privacy ]. Westin found, prior to the Web becoming mainstream, that approximately half of the general public are pragmatists; just over half of the remainder are fundamentalists, and a minority are unconcerned. Suffice it to say that people do care about privacy, and are just finding new ways to manage it [ boydnp10 ], contrary to what certain tech executives might claim [ zuckpriv , googpriv ]. My main concern in terms of this thesis is how privacy infringement might impact presentation of self. We have already seen that online identity performance may alter a general understanding of oneself, which reflects in the offline world. So I must ask: when people self-censor online due to privacy concerns, how does this stifle self-expression, and in turn impact internal identity construction?

Furthermore, implications of our online sharing decisions affect more than just ourselves; "interpreted selves" are created by recognising patterns across millions of people [ boydpriv12 ].

There is often an asymmetry about the collection and use of data. To take a relatively prosaic example, Facebook introduced 'read receipts' on messaging, which indicate when a user has seen a message. This feature has been shown to cause anxiety when present in email systems, as users seek to maintain their responsiveness image , the impression which they project to others about how they respond to input and partition their attention [ tyler2003can ]. Once ambiguity about attention has been removed, a whole class of white lies - 'The internet was bad, I couldn't check my messages' - are no longer possible, and people develop alternative strategies, such as not opening messages until they feel prepared to respond. The key difference in the context of SNS is that the user does not have the same degree of control over the channel - email receipts can be switched off, but SNS offer different levels of control.

The social aspects of privacy relate to what DeCew terms expressive privacy - a freedom from peer pressure and an ability to express one's own identity [ decew1997pursuit ]. Nissenbaum's contextual integrity [ nissenbaum2004privacy , nissenbaum2009privacy ] seeks to understand "appropriate sharing", looking at the ways in which flows of information are governed by norms, which may be easily violated as technological systems repurpose and share data.

"I argue that privacy is simply in a state of transition as people try to make sense of how to negotiate the structural transformations resulting from networked media. ... just because people are adopting tools that radically reshape their relationship to privacy does not mean they are interested in giving up their privacy. " [boydnp10]

From licloud paper background:

Deception in the age of pervasive surveillance

We are rapidly moving into a world where information about nearly every aspect of our lives is becoming sensed, recorded, captured and made available in digital form. Increasingly, every app on our smartphones, wirelessly connected devices in our homes, and on-body worn sensing devices are capturing what we do in unprecedented detail. Dodge and Kitchin \cite{dodge2005codes} introduced the term \emph{capta} to cover the data about us which are selected and captured as we go about our lives. In addition, vast amounts of data are captured and shared voluntarily, as tools invite ever more intimate \emph{participatory surveillance} \cite{albrechtslund2008Participatory}. While the abundance of information traces has unlocked a wide range of new kinds of applications~(eg. \cite{Akker:2014:TRP:2684563.2684638} \cite{Consolvo:2008:ASW:1357054.1357335}), the creation and potential for disclosure poses new threats to individual privacy and autonomy. The overall lack of transparency by manufacturers regarding how they are capturing and handling personal information has created a heightened sense of unease among many, in addition to the potential threats dealing with their unintentional disclosure or misuse\cite{fife2012privacy, metzger2004privacy, featherman2010reducing}.

There is often an asysmmetry about the collection and use of data. To take a relatively prosaic example, Facebook introduced `read receipts' on messaging, which indicate when a user has seen a message. This feature has been shown to cause anxiety when present in email systems, as users seek to maintain their \emph{responsiveness image}, the impression which they project to others about how they respond to input and partition their attention~\cite{tyler2003can}. Once ambiguity about attention has been removed, a whole class of white lies---`The internet was bad, I couldn't check my messages'---are no longer possible, and people develop alternative strategies, such as not opening messages until they feel prepared to respond.

The key difference in the context of social networks is that the user does not have the same degree of control over the channel---email receipts can be switched off, while social networks offer different levels of control. In response, a spectrum of tools and practises have emerged to re-introduce attentional privacy in messaging systems (see Section \ref{sec:existing-tools}).

Beyond sensing and storing this information, these apps and devices often disclose it, such as to the app or product's manufacturer's cloud data services, or to third-party companies and services for marketing and other purposes.

For those wanting to not be tracked, the two primary options available: try to suppress the amount of information being shared, or discontinue their use altogether. In practice, however, neither option is feasible or effective; first, the ever-increasing and varied ways people are now being tracked means that people are hardly aware of how they are being tracked to begin with. For example, most end-users have little awareness of the information being captured about them by their smartphone apps~\cite{mylonas2013delegate, wetherall2011privacy}. Second, many of the tools that track people are so vital to their everyday work or social activities that discontinuing their use is simply not practical~\cite{} . Finally, suppressing or falsifying information given to services only works insofar as that information is both manually supplied and inessential to delivering the provided services. Since people's greatest concerns surround information automatically sensed and captured, such as a person's physical location or activities, suppression may simply be impossible. With the promise of further Internet-connected `smart' sensors becoming increasingly invisible, the problem of even knowing where embedded in the environment these devices are may simply become impossible.

In contrast, a different strategy to suppressing information is the use of \emph{disinformation}, in which false information is strategically added to an information channel to discredit and draw attention away from true information that could not be suppressed~\cite{}. Such strategies have been used heavily in wartime by various counterintelligence agencies in order to reduce the damage of leaked sensitive information~\cite{alexander2010Disinformation} \ToCite{CVDazzle? Similar stuff?}. While such measures may seem extreme, they are, the concept of disinformation is quite simple. In this paper, we hypothesise that that, when applied selectively and appropriately in contexts, the use of disinformation, like other forms of deception, can be made to benefit end-users, system designers, and even those who are disinformation targets.

We are rapidly moving into a world where information about nearly every aspect of our lives is becoming sensed, recorded, captured and made available in digital form. Data is captured and shared voluntarily, as tools invite ever more intimate participatory surveillance \cite{albrechtslund2008Participatory}. While the abundance of information traces has unlocked a wide range of new kinds of applications~(eg. \cite{Akker:2014:TRP:2684563.2684638} \cite{Consolvo:2008:ASW:1357054.1357335}), the creation and potential for disclosure poses new threats to individual privacy and autonomy. The overall lack of transparency by manufacturers regarding how they are capturing and handling personal information has created a heightened sense of unease among many, in addition to the potential threats dealing with their unintentional disclosure or misuse~\cite{fife2012privacy, metzger2004privacy, featherman2010reducing}.

Many tools have been dedicated to helping people carry out various kinds of digital deception for the purpose of protecting their privacy. Without aspirations of comprehensiveness, we mention some here. Tools for masking identity are currently available for all levels of the software stack, from tools like \emph{tor} for masking the origin and destination at the network level~\cite{dingledine2004tor}, to privacy-enhancing features at the browser level. Such browser features include \emph{Do Not Track}~\cite{tene2012track}, user-agent spoofing, and tracker and cookie-blocking capabilities~\cite{eckersley2010unique}. At the application level, anonymous e-mail re-mailers~\cite{gulcu1996mixing}, anonymous e-Cash and cryptocurrencies~\cite{casale2015cryptocurrencies}, and anonymous secure file sharing systems~\cite{scarlata2001responder} have started to support certain activities offering guarantees of privacy under specified conditions.

The commoditised self

Social systems which involve content creation (like YouTube) or knowledge generation (like Wikipedia) are commonly seen as cooperative communities, whose participants generate value both for each other and also for the organisation behind the system. In [ vand09 ], van Dijck et. al. contest several uncritical manifestos for the business and communal interests of revolutionary Web 2.0 peer-production. They point out that seemingly open co-creation platforms are still profit-driven commercial entities. These entities do not provide tools out of benevolence, but in order to harvest metadata about their users, which they can process and resell. The balance of power between individuals and corporations is not swinging back towards the individual, as proponents of user-generated content sites claim, but the illusion is created that it is.

Users of systems often have little understanding of how their activities are being exploited - or nudged [ vand09b ]. Even as users are empowered by technology to create media, products, or services they desire, [ vand09b ] calls into question their agency when participants are being used and manipulated by commercial entities under the guise of community formation or participatory culture. [ dbeer08 ] describes SNS profiles as "commodities, both produced and consumed." In more recent years, awareness of this fact has spread. A popular refrain from advocates of less commercial alternatives is that "if you're not paying for it, you're the product." 1 , 2 , 3

In a similar vein to the previous section, we must also wonder about the impact of external commercial and economic forces which shape the tools and systems people are using to express themselves online. As a contrast, in the second half of this thesis, I focus on decentralised systems, which are potentially much less likely to exploit user metadata for profit.

The ghost in the (social) machine

Social Machines are systems for which the human and computational aspects are equally critical. In most cases, humans do the creative work whilst machines do the administrative tasks [ Timbl1999 ]. Up to now, systems have not been designed to be Social Machines; rather, the concept and definition of a Social Machine is derived from observations of existing (usually Web-based) systems. Many are products of the contemporary social web, on many different scales and in many different domains, often evolving, responding to technological and social developments, and interacting with each other [ Hendler2010 , ByrneEvans2013 , DeRoure2013 , Strohmaier2013 ]. Social Machines can be identified within and across social media networks, within and across online communities, and within and across technological spaces.

Social Machines are pertinent to our work here as they provide a lens through which we can examine sociotechnical phenomena which emphasises the interdependence between humans and technology. In studies of social networks discussed previously in this chapter, humans are considered as users of systems, and discussions focus around how people react to technology, how people behave in the context of particular technical or social constraints, or how people's lives are changed in response to their interactions with and through digital environments. It is important to also reflect upon the ways in which technology evolves or is reconceptualised as a result of passive (mis)use and active (mis)appropriation by humans. We similarly must recognise technical systems in the wider context of society, and include in our dialogue the developers who design and build technical systems, the organisations and legal entities which finance and drive them, and the cultural and economic climate in which they are situated.

Social Machines which have been studied so far have been described and categorised in terms of purpose [ DeRoure2013 ], motivations and incentives, technology used, goals and processes, quality assessment of outputs, and user participation and interaction [ Smart2014 ]. The emphasis in this work is on discussing Social Machines in collective terms; that is, 'a' Social Machine - wherever its boundaries happen to have been drawn (so far these boundaries are typically drawn around the edge of a "service" [ Smart2014 ]) - is considered as a coherent whole. The circumstances of the individual human participants not been given extensive consideration. For any given Social Machine, individual participants are diverse and participate in different ways, with varying goals, motivations and outcomes. As we have seen through studies of social media mentioned previously, they manipulate their online presence(s) so that they may behave in different ways according to different contexts, or may work together to construct a single image controlled by multiple people [ Dalton2013 ]. Such behaviour impacts our understanding of roles, autonomy and awareness, incentives and attribution, and accountability and trustworthiness of participants. Overlooking unique individual perspectives when observing a Social Machine as a whole can cause incorrect assumptions, for example: believing that participants who lie about who they are have negative intentions in a Social Machine whose overall "purpose" is to strengthen social ties. We must also bear this in mind when designing systems, so that a system may grow in response to unexpected actions of participants rather than hampering their explorations.

I argue that due to the complex nature of online identity, understanding nuanced individual behaviours of participants in a more granular way is crucial for Social Machine observation. I advance this argument in the next chapter through an empirical study of a Social Machine centered around creative media production.

Conclusions

Present day social media has dramatically increased participation in publishing and sharing online content. Easy-to-use services lower the barrier to entry for connecting with and pushing thoughts out to an audience. Identities expressed through social media are inherently collaborative; every interaction is pushed to a network, and part of a dynamic cycle of consumption and creation feedback. Commenting on someone else's post automatically links the post to your own profile, and often it appears there as well, accessible from two different contexts. Yet SNS permit little customisation, providing preset options for content or reaction templates, and consistent inflexible designs for profile pages. This tips the balance away from the individual aspect of identity construction performed by bloggers in the earlier years of the Web. Yet blogs and personal homepages are left wanting for a dynamically constructed and low barrier to entry network, which impedes the collaborative aspects of identity construction.

People manage shortcomings with the affordances of both blogging platforms and SNS in different ways—contending with invisible audiences and collapsed contexts, as well as reduced expectations of privacy—through carefully crafted personas, strategically omitting or amending the information they post online, or simply using different platforms for different purposes. We see that there are a multitude of factors which affect people's presentation of self online, which vary according to broader cultural or technological contexts, as well as personal motivations and abilities.

Along with their updated definition of SNS mentioned earlier, [ ellison13 ] calls for social media scholars who are studying individuals or communities online to systematically describe the technology in which their participants are situated, and the practices of the users. Technologies are changing rapidly still, so studies which are a snapshot in time can be linked to a broader discourse or overview, and remain relevant as time progresses, if they situate themselves appropriately. One way of accessing this bigger picture is through an organising framework that helps to record the background state of the environment being studied, and so surfaces connections between work that is otherwise perhaps not directly comparable [ vand09b ]. In chapter 3 I use several studies of my own on diverse identity behaviours across various social network sites in order to propose such a framework.

  • Search Menu
  • Sign in through your institution
  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Urban Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Culture
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Politics
  • Law and Society
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Neuroanaesthesia
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Medical Ethics
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Ethics
  • Business Strategy
  • Business History
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and Government
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic History
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • International Political Economy
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Theory
  • Politics and Law
  • Politics of Development
  • Public Policy
  • Public Administration
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

The Oxford Handbook of Cyberpsychology

  • < Previous chapter
  • Next chapter >

2 The Online Self

Alison Attrill-Smith, Cyberpsychology Research, University of Wolverhampton

  • Published: 07 November 2018
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

This chapter considers how people create their online selves. It outlines how an online “self” could be an extension or replication of the offline self, but equally how an individual may create different versions of self online. It considers the online world as a heterogeneous environment that requires the involuntary creation of an online self in order to achieve functional goals online (e.g. online banking) while enabling flexible, voluntary self-creation that allows people to explore and manipulate their online self based on behavioral goals. In doing so, it explores how people make use of available technology, images, video, and text to create cues that are used offline to perceive and interpret others’ behavior to allow online interactions that mimic offline interpersonal relationships. The chapter presents a number of theories that have been pivotal in shaping understanding the online self, as well as the main personal features of online self-creation. The last section of this chapter considers why online self-creation may not always be functional or healthy.

Introduction

The “self” is a curious creature. It is you, it is all of the psychological and physiological features that come together to give you a personal identity. It is how you think and feel, and how you interpret, react to, and behave in diverse situations. It is the person you are talking about when you use the pronouns “I” and “we.” This list is not exclusive. Many aspects of your daily life will influence how you see, interpret, and present yourself, both offline and online. As this book is about online behavior, the rest of this chapter will refer to the online self—the person you are when perusing the Internet, interacting with others online, or simply observing others in their online activities. That is not to say that we can ignore the offline self entirely. Of course, your offline “you” shapes and influences who you are in your online activities, and vice versa, but there are crossovers, changes, and distinctions between the two. It would be extremely rare for a person to be able to draw a thick black line between who they are online and who they are offline.

Throughout this book, you will read about a whole array of online behaviors and activities, ranging from gaming to dating, from banking and shopping to diverse crimes. One thread that carries through all of these actions is that people need to create online selves to be able to partake in any of the behaviors considered. The sheer fact that you need to enter your details on a website to use it, means that you are sharing information about yourself that enables others to create an image or profile of you. You are putting your “self” online. How people construct their self online is the focus of this chapter, which considers the voluntary online self (information you choose to share) and involuntary online self (information you need to share in order to complete a goal or task online). It also deliberates some of the advantages and disadvantages to the creation of diverse selves online. There may be differences in how we create ourselves online versus offline based on the time we are afforded to explore how we want to portray ourselves to others online—an advantage we rarely have offline. We can create, edit, and re-edit who we are before we share information about ourselves with others online (Attrill, 2015 ). There is no need for spontaneous communication; with asynchronous interactions we are in full control of who we want to be online. We might thus suppose that our online selves are reincarnations of the desired or ideal self, or a self that fits the activity in hand. Alternatively, we might consider these online selves to be extensions of already formed offline selves. These notions are explored in more detail as we progress through our consideration of the online self. Chris Fullwood then picks up on some of the concepts and explores them in terms of self-presentation and impression management online in the following chapter.

Multiple Selves

To set the scene for this exploration, we need to briefly consider the different “selves” in terms of psychological conceptualizations, as these will re-emerge throughout many chapters of this book. You will come across many terms about the self and how this consists of multiple selves. When we use the term “self-concept”, it refers to “the individual’s belief about himself or herself, including the person’s attributes and who and what the self is” (Baumeister, 1999 ). Higgins ( 1987 ) defined one of the most commonly referred to conceptualizations of self: the actual, ideal , and ought selves. The latter two guide how we interpret and process information about our selves, while the actual self (also called the real, true , or core self in later theoretical conceptualizations) portrays who we really are in any given situation at a given point in time. The ideal self is the person we really want to be and who we strive to be, whereas the ought self is the person we believe others want us to be. For example, your parents might have wanted you to study psychology; thus, you are a psychology student currently reading this text. You see your ideal self as someone achieving a good grade as a psychology student, and therefore work hard to make that happen. Your actual self might however be more inclined to think that you can wing an important assignment and therefore choose to spend time in the university bar rather than striving to achieve your ideal version of self. These selves do not need to be vastly distinct from one another, and indeed, too much distance between the selves can cause psychological tension and discomfort (Festinger, 1954 ) to the point of illness. If you have these different selves, you might now be wondering how they reconcile to provide you with a self-identity, a notion of who you are, regardless of whether you are online or offline. When we talk of the core self, we assume this is the part of your self-identity that remains unaffected by temporal and situational factors. It guides and influences your behavior but can sometimes be led astray by your ideal or ought selves. Higgins’ discrepancy theory will be discussed in more detail later, but it is worth mentioning that Higgins’ theory spurned a host of similar distinctions of different types of self that are beyond the scope of this chapter. If you would like to know more about this, you can find overviews in almost any basic social psychology textbook (e.g. Hogg & Vaughan, 2017 ).

Two notes of distinction before progressing. The first relates to distinguishing the online and offline worlds in which the self is presented. In the early days of the Internet, researchers and lay persons alike often referred to an online world and a “real” world, and the latter has been replaced with the term “offline” world. The self is the self, regardless of whether this is online or offline. Continuing to use the terms real and online worlds negates the effects that online behavior can have offline, and vice versa. You only need to think of the impact that cyberbullying might have on a teenager’s offline social life to realize the reality of online behavior.

The second concerns a distinction between human computer interaction (HCI) from this exploration of online self. In HCI, the focus is on the design and usability of technology, and it refers to the interface between people and technology. This chapter instead focuses on how you create your online self, how you act as yourself online, and how you can selectively manipulate and re-edit that self to suit both your and the task’s needs and/or demands, as well as those of other users with whom you are interacting (see Attrill, 2015 ), e.g. people, shops, banks, agencies, discussion groups, review groups, and many more versions of “other” with whom you interact online.

Who Am I Online?

Let us now begin by considering who you think you are online. Who you are online will depend on the theoretical stance you adhere to, diverse research findings, the type of activity you are engaged in, and with whom you are interacting online. In order to complete any online task, you need to create an online self-identity, a cyberself, or a digital self. For the purposes of this chapter, these self terms will be used interchangeably. In some instances, you will be required to share more or less information about yourself (self information). This self will be flexible enough to suit the situational and temporal norms of behavior related to the task in hand, just as it does offline. Offline, you fulfill a number of social roles. As you read this text, you might be a student, a curious wife or husband, a son or daughter, a brother or sister, or you might be a law enforcement officer wanting to understand more about online crime, or a counsellor wanting to explore ways of helping people through online modes of communication. Social roles are not mutually exclusive, with some overlapping elements of your personality, but in any given situation you choose which self to present. If you are a student, for example, you might behave very differently on a Friday night out with your student friends to how you would behave sitting around a restaurant table with your parents and grandparents. Different offline situations create different social norms. Your online self is no different in this respect. If you are an online dater, you might be more brash in dismissing potential dates than you would if you had met them face to face. Your core self might be principally against being rude, but online you can let a bolder you shine through the suspected anonymity of your monitor. You can become a keyboard warrior without revealing your real true self. But what would happen if you continued to do that online? Consider for a moment TV soap operas. Some actors have been in a show for so long that if you were to encounter them out of character, it might be difficult to distinguish between their on-screen character and their “real” self. That is because if we act out a role repeatedly over time, we take on the characteristics of that role. The self is thus malleable and adaptable, and you can, over time change your core self. Thus, whichever “self” you are presenting online can, and does, have both positive and negative consequences offline.

One useful place to begin with understanding who you are online, and how you act out these social roles online, is to play the “I am … … ” game. Writing twenty “I am” statements about your offline you, you will most definitely write a number of descriptive statements, e.g. I am female, I am British, I am a lecturer. If, however, you were to write down twenty statements beginning with the words “I am” in relation to your online behavior and activities, what would that list look like? I am an online shopper, I am a blogger, I am an online banker, I am on Facebook, I am on Twitter, I am an online gamer, etc. Next, try to group those statements into functional categories. You will likely end up with various categories, for example, that revolve around your functional existence (e.g. financial activities such as shopping or banking) and social activities (e.g. social media and gaming). But how many of your statements relate to activities that you need to do online? For instance, did you write any statement that requires you to tax your vehicle, apply for child tax credits, renew your passport, or check your child’s school homework and performance online? In order to execute all of these activities, you need to create versions of your self online. The former are more voluntary and the latter, borne from necessity, are your involuntary online selves. Note, I am talking of selves here, intentionally. These are, however, not separate or distinct from one another, but more fluid and responsive to situational norms. An alternative distinction of these selves is to think of those activities in which you freely engage online as those for which you manage your online self-presentation, whereas involuntary selves are those which engage with the Internet as a tool for completing a desired function or goal. But what does this distinction actually mean?

Leary ( 1995 , p. 2) describes self-presentation as “the process of controlling how one is perceived by other people.” When you manipulate how you present your self online, you are also trying to control how others see you in that situation. You then interpret their reactions to your presented self, which, in turn, influences how you present your (possibly edited or changed) self in the future. Alongside this process, some theoretical considerations are necessary to highlight that there is a core self, as already described above, underlying both the voluntary and involuntary online self. This self influences not only how you manipulate and self-present across different online activities, but also how you interpret and respond to others’ behavior online. Imagine, for example, that someone posts what they believe to be an innocuous statement on social media. A person who has a negative core self might read that statement as implying harmful intent, whereas a more positive core self could put an entirely different spin on the statement. Both are not as the poster intended! Nonetheless, how you interpret that neutral post will subsequently guide your response to it. Thus, your sense of core self, and the cognitive processes associated with the self, may be difficult to change in the long term, but they will guide those aspects and features of self which you can change across different online activities. The process of online self-creation is therefore just as ongoing and in constant flux as is your offline self with all of its different social roles. This overall process of self-presentation, interpretation, and manipulation comes together to form your sense of online self. While the process of self-presentation is also considered to be one of impression management, you are trying to present a version of your self, with or without conscious awareness, that influences how others see you. The terms impression management and self-presentation have become somewhat synonymous with one another and are consequently used interchangeably in this chapter. However, it could be argued that self-presentation is the act of managing your impression of self to others.

There are a number of reasons as to why we engage in online self-presentation, from having the freedom to explore different selves online (Turkle, 1995 ) to needing to present a version of self to achieve a certain goal (e.g. Leary, 1995 ). The former might involve, for example, exploring a more outgoing and expressive version of self online than the offline self, whereas the latter might require a certain style of self-presentation to acquire a new job via a platform such as LinkedIn ( http://www.linkedin.com ). Another factor of Internet behavior discussed in more detail in other chapters is that the Internet is considered a heterogeneous, ever-changing landscape. In your offline world, you behave differently in diverse situations with various goals. Your online behavior is no different. Offline, you present a different self and behave differently during a first date to how you would behave when at work. Online, you are no different. The self presented on a dating website, for example, will likely be very different to the self you reveal on a professional networking website such as LinkedIn or researchgate ( http://www.researchgate.org ). Where your goal is one of providing self information that needs to endure for a longer time, or that creates a more positive impression of your self, you may be more truthful and honest in that online self creation than when your goal or online interaction is a brief or short-lived interaction or encounter. Obviously, there will be individual and personal factors that impinge upon how a person presents themselves online. Although online impression management and the role of individual factors is touched upon here, interested readers are referred the next chapter of this volume (see Fullwood , this volume) for a more detailed discussion.

Theories of Self

In order to understand online self-presentation, it is useful to take a look at some of the theories of self that have been used to explain online impression management. In doing so, it is acknowledged that many people will consciously believe that they do not manipulate who they are online, but are firm in their notion of self being consistent offline and online, regardless of what they are doing and with whom they are interacting. In other words, they believe that they are always presenting a true or real self, regardless of situational or temporal factors. From a psychological perspective, the Internet offers users the opportunity to step outside of these selves, and even to create an entirely fictitious or fantasy self that simply cannot exist offline. Imagine for example, the avatars that people use when gaming online. Although they may don a costume to act out a character when visiting a ComicCon Convention ( http://www.mcmcomiccon.com/birmingham/ ), or take part in offline costume play of an online game, they cannot sensibly exist in that offline environment continuously or for any length of time. A prime example hereof is the game World of Warcraft, a video game that first came to attention in 2004, drew large crowds when acted out offline. Recently, players donned costumes and escaped to a forest in the Czech Republic to partake in an offline construction of a World of Warcraft battle ( https://www.news18.com/news/tech/in-pics-real-life-world-of-warcraft-takes-place-in-czech-forest-1734865.html ). While this is possible for a brief period of time, imagine turning up to work on a Monday morning wearing the costume of a video game character! Online, however, you can return to this character on a daily basis. It is less sporadic and could potentially become a more integrated part of your daily self. Of course, there are risks around this integration of fantasy selves into one’s notion of self that could potentially impinge on daily activities and routines, but its inclusion does illustrate that we cannot assume that the online self will always be identical to the offline self. Sometimes, online selves are used for escapism much in the same way as they are offline; online people may be able to escape for longer periods of time more frequently. But we must ask why people manipulate their self-presentation in this way. Borrowing from long established theories and coupling these with more recently emerging ideas may help answer this question.

Goffman’s Theory of Self-Presentation

Over fifty years ago, Goffman ( 1959 ) presented a theory of self-presentation that has remained eminent throughout the study of offline selves, and has survived technological advances and adaptations to be even more applicable now than it was of the time of its creation. Goffman suggested that the world is nothing other than a stage on which humans are actors. We are therefore nothing other than actors playing out our roles of everyday life, and it is the arenas in which we stage those roles that are constantly changing. This echoes the earlier outline of the different social roles we embody in our daily lives. When online, we are thus manipulating our self-presentations, working alone or with others, to create, shape, and present impressions of both the self and others in a way that portrays a certain self-image we want others to see. Indeed, Goffman was ahead of his time in stating that the self portrayed via the telephone or in written communication (i.e. letters) was inferior to that presented to another person in face-to-face communication. Online communication in text form via email, instant messenger, or even via the emerging rudimentary social networking sites, often lacked the conveyance of social cues, emotion, and other body language cues of communication. Consider, for example, early bulletin board systems which required single user asynchronous contributions (only one person at a time could contribute to the text-based communications), Internet Relay Chat, which offered one of the first forms of instant messaging, or online forums. All of these text-based tools helped shape and develop online communication as we now know it, illustrating how capable humans are of adapting, changing, and developing available tools to suit their wants and needs.

With these text-based interactions, which lacked social cues, humans began to develop avatars, abbreviations (LOL, BRB, ROFL), and emoticons. Social cues that could convey the appearance of self as well as momentary feelings and emotions were replaced online with expressions presented in symbols and letters (e.g. [ ;) ], [ :( ], [ <3 ], etc.). These have been replaced by fully formed emoticons and miniscule images of almost every conceivable category of items nowadays—e.g. animals, buildings, flags, food types, holidays, etc. If you so desired, you could convey an entire message and emotional self through small images alone. While Goffman’s notion of the world as a stage could not be more accurate when considering the ways in which we present ourselves online, many features of his outlined self appeared to stem from futuristic notions or science fiction. For the reader who remembers the 1980’s television program Dick Tracy, Knight Rider , and David Hasselhoff’s character Michael talking to a wrist watch, consider other science fiction predictions that are now a reality—the Apple watch into which you can now talk to communicate rather than tapping on it. We might not yet have a speaking car, but many of us do already speak orders to our dashboards to carry out in transit activities (e.g. make a phone call or find a street). We digress! Humans have forever managed to convey themselves in one form or another, whether it be via face-to-face communication, in text, in images, and in a world filled with technology that is reminiscent of science fiction, people will always find a way to portray the self that they want others to see and experience, and where better to do so than online. The online world offers myriad avenues for self-exploration and self-presentation, be that of an ideal, actual, fictional, ought, or fusion self online.

At this point, you might be asking yourself why people feel inclined to manipulate their online self-presentation. There are many theoretical reasons as to why this might be, but before these are discussed, take a moment to consider your own actions online. Do you ever see images of friends, family, or acquaintances enjoying the perfect lifestyle, with perfect bodies, the perfect house, perfect holidays, and perfect car, etc.? Of course, in order for the Internet to exist, people need to use it and share information, images included, but do you ever take a moment to ask yourself if these portrayals are an actual reflection of life? Also, do they then influence your thinking of how you should be living your life? If this is the case, then rightly or wrongly, others’ self-portrayals are influencing how you present your self to others. The next time you look at someone’s perfect life as depicted online, think of Turkle’s ( 1995 ) suggestion of the Internet being a playground, where people can be who they want to be, where they can explore their self identities, and, to some extent, create a completely fake persona if they wish. These ideal life portrayals are possible online due to software and technological advances, e.g. the filters and adaptations available to post perfect images of one’s self to Instagram. Recent work in our Cyberpsychology group has highlighted that people tend to manipulate images of themselves when posting to Instagram, but not images of other people. The expectation was that if people scored high on measures that indicate they want to present themselves in a positive light, they might also be inclined to want to make other people look less favorable when posting images of them, but that did not emerge in our data (Attrill-Smith & Shaban, n.d. ). Altering self-images in this way raises social pressure for other posters to equally comply, leading to even the most technically challenged of individuals being able to completely alter images of themselves to post online. According to Goffman’s theory, people once used written text to attract attention from others. The filtering of Instagram images may reflect the modern equivalent, in that people seek to attract attention by the use of image manipulation to present a perfect image of self to others. After all, Instagram “success” is measured in the amount of likes an image receives and the number of followers a poster has. Although Goffman’s theory can help us understand what people do when putting themselves online, and illustrates the different selves one might portray across different types of Internet arena, it doesn’t offer a great insight into the strategies they might use in doing so.

Arkin’s Self-Presentation Strategies

Arkin ( 1981 ) suggested two strategies that people use to present the self to achieve the performance outlined by Goffman: acquisitive self-presentation (used to gain others’ approval of the depicted self) and protective self-presentation (used to avoid others’ disapproval of the self). On an image-sharing platform as Instagram, people may look for approval from others. In online support groups, they may instead be seeking health advice. Whereas the former may promote acquisitive self-presentation, the latter may involve protective self-presentation. Arkin outlines the use of these two steps of self-presentation, which serve to both enhance acceptance of the depicted self, but equally to ensure that a poster can protect themselves against negative feedback or criticism of others. However, both Goffman’s and Arkin’s theories existed a good few decades before the spread of the Internet as it exists today. Again, there are many factors that influence how people create their online self, not least individual factors, but also what the aims and goals are in portraying their online self. Instagram is a very different online arena to many other forms of online communication. From discussion groups to talking to an online expert via a chatbot, and from online dating to being a member of an online gaming community, it is likely that anyone using the Internet carefully and selectively presents themselves across a diverse and varied Internet landscape in such a way that promotes both a sense of positive self while also protecting the self from others’ disapproval.

Having read this chapter thus far, you are now probably wondering how such a fragmented self, consisting of different selves, can exist in psychological harmony online. Firstly, the portrayal here is not of a fragmented self, but of an overall self-concept that consists of many different features of self. It is these features that determine different types of behaviors in different situations and times. The self-concept is actually somewhat stable over time and not as fragmented as this outline would suggest. Secondly, there are countless existing and emerging theories of online self-creation. Reviewing every single theory is beyond the scope of this volume. Thus, the chapter next provides a brief overview of some of the theories that may elucidate why we don’t have a fragmented self, and which will hopefully help spark an interest for further reading around this topic.

Self-Discrepancy Theory (SDT)

Built on the notion of people having the three selves outlined above (actual, ideal, and ought selves), Higgins ( 1987 ) suggested in his Self-Discrepancy Theory (SDT) that how we perceive ourselves in both our own and others’ eyes is pivotal to how we present our self to the world. Depending on the value and weight we give to others’ views and social norms, a person will strive to present one of the three selves. If, however, that presentation strays too far from the actual or core self, you would be trying to be someone who you are not. Thus, the cleft between an actual and ideal self, or an actual and ought self, cannot be too great or it will cause psychological tension. This tension can be detrimental and harmful to a person’s perceptions of their self. People may engage in some level of comparison with others to gauge how to behave in any given situation (see Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988 and their self-evaluation maintenance model of upward and downward comparisons), or to enhance their sense of belonging socially (see Festinger’s 1954 social comparison theory), but essentially any self presented other than the core or actual self is a step away from a core or real self; psychological frustrations may result if the ideal or ought self is too far removed from reality. In other words, if you set realistic and achievable goals related to your actual or ought self, there is less likelihood of psychological tension and upset. However, if those ideal or ought selves are so far removed from reality that they cannot be achieved, frustration and psychological discomfort would increase. Hopefully, you are alread the most ideal version of you that you can and want to be, and thus, your ideal and actual selves are in sync—something many endlessly strive for, but few ever achieve. Being at one with your self in this manner is often referred to as having self-concept clarity (Campbell et al., 1996 ).

Before wondering why very few people achieve self-concept clarity, bear in mind that of importance is your perception of how much your actual self is living up to your expectations of your ideal and ought selves. Your interpretation of what those selves should be is critical in creating or reducing discrepancies between your selves. These perceptions, expectations, and interpretations will, of course, be influenced by your previous experiences of similar situations, or hearsay of what to expect of any given situation. Ultimately, presenting an actual self where possible will help reduce psychological discomfort in a situation where you strive for an ideal or actual self. Moreover, not all selves are based on instant gratification or instant goal attainment. Some behavior goals require a longer time period to achieve than others. For example, you might currently be working on an undergraduate or postgraduate qualification to achieve an ideal version of your self as an accomplished student. It is healthy to have these realistic goals of achieving an ideal self. And in terms of online behavior, as much as offline behavior is governed by social norms, rules, regulations, and laws, any self we present needs to remain within those behavioral boundaries unless we wish to invite unsavory or even illegal consequences of our behavior. When considering online selves, however, it might be the case that the distance between the selves is less prominent than it may be offline, because people don’t always have the social reinforcement of the social norms and boundaries that exist offline. Alternatively, we could adopt the view of early reports of the self online, which suggested that online self-presentation as more of an exploration for developing and presenting an ideal self, rather than actual self-presentation (e.g. Turkle, 1995 ). One of the advantages of this online self-exploration is that versions of self can be acted online that could potentially be transferred to the offline world. For example, in their 2013 study, DeHaan and colleagues reported on 32 lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender individuals who were able to explore aspects of their sexuality online, but who often would maintain a low profile in their offline worlds for fear of negative judgment (Mustanski, Newcomb, & Garofalo, 2011 ). The underlying notion of this line of reasoning is that if an individual pretends to be someone long enough they might eventually “become” that person, along the lines of Arkin’s ( 1981 ) aforementioned acquisitive self-presentation, and akin to the example of soap opera actors becoming their characters both in and out of role.

Consider for a moment your own online behavior. Are there certain things you do online where you feel you are more you than other areas of online behavior? Or, do you filter all of your Instagram photos to present the best possible version of yourself that you believe you could be (ideal self), or, do you filter and amend photos of your self to present someone who you believe others want you to be (ought self)? If you have ever been active on an online dating website, and you like the pictures of a potential dater, do you ever ask yourself if that is really what they look like, only to be hugely disappointed when they turn up to an offline date and it is clear that their images had been altered to present a much better-looking version of them to attract a date? This is SDT in practice online. Now, imagine that when you meet this person offline, you cannot hide your sheer disappoint that s/he does not look as promised by the online photos. Surely, that person returns home from their date with a huge dent to their ego, even more so if they feel that the images they are using reflect their real self. If, however, they admit to themselves that they have altered the images to present a false ideal self, then the reality of the situation and disappointment will have less psychological impact on them than if they believe themselves to be portraying their real self to others online. One feature of this type of online self-creation is that it is flexible, malleable, and constantly changing. It is the individual who chooses which of these selves to use in any given online arena, but this benefit of choice does not translate so readily offline. In the offline world the individual is usually in the moment, and there is no time to edit or re-create the self as easily as can be done online. For example, if you tell a bad joke and experience a tumbleweed silence in a pub, the awkwardness can last for quite a while. In an online environment, however, you probably wouldn’t experience the same level of embarrassment as you can suggest that the meaning or pun of the joke was lost in translation. It is worth mentioning though that Schlegel, Vess, and Arndt ( 2012 ) proposed that, even online, this level of self-re-creation is not sustainable. They argue that if you have a core or actual self that is well-developed, it will eventually surface, regardless of where you are presenting yourself (offline or online). Specifically, it is the core self that is stable and consistent across time and situations. An individual cannot be in a constant state of self-flux, as it would cause continuous discrepancy and disharmony to the notion of self. Some level of awareness of the core self would guide a person to certain areas of online behavior in the first place. Once there, the individual may decide to slightly or considerably alter the self to comply to the situational norms, presenting an idealized or ought self. The next section considers what role social cues play in online self creation.

Social Information Processing Theory (SIP)

If you meet a person offline for the first time, what cues do you use to form your impression of them? You will likely judge them by their dress sense and level of perceived personal hygiene, as well as by their facial and general body language. All of these factors, and more, are considered to be social cues, i.e. little bits of information gleaned from a person or situation. These cues are then used to interpret and respond in any given situation. Offline these facial expressions and body language convey a wide range of information; someone crossing their arms unwelcomingly can be interpreted as hostility, them throwing open their arms as welcoming, and tears can be shed for both sadness and joy. These emotions, as well as an array of others in between, are instantaneous signals that enable us to interpret and respond to others’ behavior in our offline worlds, and which have historically been less available in written forms of online communications. During the infancy of online communication, people needed to create a version of themselves based entirely in written text. However, humans are continuously adapting and using tools to our advantage, and how we use the Internet is no different. According to SIP theory (Walther, 1992 ), we simply compensate for absent social cues through other online means.

When engaging in online communication, have you ever read an online description of someone and created an image in your own head of what you think they look like? Or has it informed preconceptions of how they might behave? Has that then shaped the way in which you respond to them? This is exactly the premise of SIP theory: your self-identity is shaped and presented based on the world around you. According to McCarthey and Moje ( 2002 ), this process of absorbing information around us helps us make sense of the world in which we exist. Offline cues are now increasingly re-created online through means other than the basic emoticons already outlined. Popular on social media sites such as Facebook and Instagram, graphic interchange formats (GIFs) are bitesized, repetitive, bitmap videos that are used to convey an emotion or a reaction to something posted or shared in text format. Use of GIFs, emoticons, and other online substitutes for offline cues could be interpreted as suggesting that humans are adequately able to convey social cues online. However, the assumption here is that people are willing to compensate situational cues in this manner, as well as that people interpret the cues as intended by the original poster thereof, which is not necessarily the case. Information can become lost in translation, and receivers or viewers of that information often misinterpret the intended emotional conveyance. This misinterpretation then shapes the way in which that person presents their self in this situation, through written communication, GIF use, or otherwise. Interpretations of these online compensations are less open to clarification in the first instance and could inadvertently cause an instant unintended behavioral response. We have all done this—have you ever thought about a meeting or phone call you need to make? Before doing so, to organize your own thoughts, you create a script in your own mind of exactly what you will say and imagine the other person’s response(s) to your communication. You might, for instance, tell someone that you are breaking up with them romantically. You know exactly how you are going to put it to them and have played it out in your own mind a hundred times. When the time comes, however, they might throw you completely off track with a totally unexpected response. The phone call may not go according to your anticipated plan, and your interpretation of the other person’s behavior in that moment will shape how you then proceed with the conversation. Although online behavior is no different, depending upon the mode of communication you use, the instant interpretation and response might be vastly different.

Unlike all other forms of online communication, video communication is the only truly synchronous interaction currently available online. People often use videoconferencing apps such as Skype and FaceTime to stay in touch with people the world over at affordable prices. While it is gaining in popularity, some recent research suggests that people are less comfortable with using synchronous video communication, especially with people outside of immediate family circles (Rai & Attrill, 2014 ). If we consider the advantage of online communication as enabling individuals to carefully create a version of their self they feel comfortable with online, it stands to reason that video communication would be less preferred, simply because it does not offer the advantage of time lag to carefully creating a self to present to others. Imagine the following three versions of a job interview: face to face, telephone, and videoconferencing. Which do you think would afford you the most flexibility in being able to present the best possible version of you? Different people will accept/reject these modes of interview based on their own individual characteristics. They all have advantages and disadvantages in terms of social information processing. There is evidence to show that people are judged less favorably in video interviews than face to face (e.g. Chapman & Webster, 2001 ), and less favorably generally via video than face to face (e.g. Fullwood, 2007 ). In a face-to-face interview, you can use your body language instantly to more or less confidently respond to your interviewer’s body language. A brief sideways glance from an interview panel member can convey a lot of information for the interviewee. This is a cue that would be completely absent in a telephone call, where voice intonation and length of pause between questions and answers likely play a role. Subtle cues might not be so easily noticed on a live video interview. Even though video calls are synchronous, it takes a lot of practice to be able to confidently look at the camera while keeping an eye on the little box somewhere on your monitor that shows what’s happening at the other end of a conversation. It would appear that although humans have adapted their communication skills in line with technological advances, thus far they have not mastered an entire replication of our offline interactions online. And until they do so, a person’s online self may be different or similar to their offline self depending upon the mode of communication and Internet arena in use. These considerations are central to Walther’s ( 1996 ) initial conceptualizations of his hyperpersonal communication model.

Hyperpersonal Communication Model (HPCM)

Walther put forward his HPCM in 1996, when online communication for the masses was in its infancy, and people were very much restricted to text-based communications. Although his theory has had some modifications over the years, of interest for this chapter is Walther’s notion that people use different types of computer-mediated communication (CMC) to create their online self. In particular, asynchronous modes of communication enable people to compensate for the absence of social cues because they have time to think about the self that they are sharing or presenting. Text can be used to embellish and enrich a depiction of self to the online world, creating thoughtful constructions of self that portray the ideal version of self in that moment, or with a particular goal in mind.

Let’s return to the example of a job interview. Before getting to the interview stage, you might have to submit a job application via email. Of course, you want your best possible self to shine through in that email, and therefore you would therefore unlikely open the email with “Hey dude … ”. The recipient will use any cues or information available to them to form an opinion of you; in fact, this begins with your email address, not the email content. Have you ever been embarrassed to share your email address with someone? Online interactions are thus akin to a circular process of self-presentation and image formation; any information conveyed via written text or other modes of communication can, and does, influence how people build an image of a person. It will then also guide their communications and interactions with that person. This response, in turn, influences how that person responds, etc.

Notably, this circular process is not reserved for asynchronous text-based communications. There are some differences between modes of synchronous (real-time) communications that convey more or fewer social cues. Instant messaging, for example, may convey less accurate information about a person than does video communication. Indeed, instant messaging is not really a synchronous communication in this respect. Rather, the respondent can take as little or as long as they like to reply to a message. This length of time, in and of itself, can influence how a message sender will react to the message once received. Many instant messenger services or apps indicate at what time a message was received, and whether it has been read. Moreover, many apps will show that a message recipient is actively typing a response, usually in the form of three dots bouncing up and down. If the reply to you takes quite a while to compose, you might expect a lengthy reply, but it may be only one or two words—maybe just a “yes” or “no”. The difference between the send and receive time and the length of the received message is only one of many small factors that come together to aid your impression formation and judgment of another person. Equally, these factors also influence which features of your own self come to the fore to interact in that situation. This is self-creation online in real time. It is you creating you as you interact and communicate with others via the Internet. Even though we may experience an absence of the more traditional offline social cues in these text-based communications, there are ample other cues on which we draw that influence our perceptions and interpretations of others’ behavior, which in turn influence how we create our self and our own behavior.

The cues considered thus far will, of course, differ to those employed in synchronous video communication. When Skyping or FaceTiming someone you have instant cues, much in the same way as when you are chatting to them offline—or do you? Do you change your behavior and facial expressions when talking to someone in this manner? There may be subtle differences when video-chatting online to how you would behave offline. You might, for instance, play up or play down certain facets of your body language or behavior in attempts to control others’ impressions of you. When doing so, you have an advantage you don’t have offline—you can instantly see yourself as others see you because you have that information directly in front of you. This instant self-feedback allows you to amend your self instantly. However, this line of reasoning needs to consider the notion of behavioral intent. Many of the theories outlined thus far suppose that people display intent in their online behaviors, and that any recipient of an online communication is suitably able to detect that intent and respond accordingly. All of these theories assume that 1) we are carefully considering how we present our selves to others, and 2) that we are creating whichever version of the self will best achieve a desired goal. That goal might be to secure a job interview through a very well-written email, or it might be to land a date with a desired other, or it might simply be to make a friend of someone you have interacted with online. Regardless of the goal, it is extremely rare that human beings behave in a non-goal-directed manner. Creating a temporal goal-directed version of your self online is no different.

Uses and Gratifications Theory

When presenting your self online, you may or may not be consciously aware of the goal of that behavior. Goals come in many shapes and forms, and can be overtly behavioral, psychological, cognitive, physical, or emotional, or any combination of these, along with many more facets of human nature. Whatever the goal of your online activity, you will select and present aspects of your self that can be tailored to meet that goal. Katz, Blumler and Gurevitch’s ( 1973 ) Uses and Gratifications Theory is in line with many other theories that posit all human behavior to be goal-driven. For example, Deci and Ryan ( 2000 ) suggest human behavior to be driven by an underlying need for human belonging, while Davis and Kraus ( 1989 ) suggest human interaction comes from a desire to fulfill social needs. Which self you are presenting online will not only depend upon the goal you are trying to achieve, but also on which further theory you choose to adhere to. For example, according to McKenna, Green, and Gleason ( 2002 ), a person would be more likely to shape their self and engage in online interactions in a way that compensates for their inhibitions in offline social interactions. Whereas this notion presents the use of a distinct self online, a further proposal can be made for people presenting an extended offline self online. Accordingly, if aligning to Valkenburg, Schouten, and Peter’s ( 2005 ) notion of social enhancement, it could be assumed that one’s online self is an extension of their offline self, and that the same crafting and creation of self is present regardless of whether the person is interacting online or offline. Regardless of which of these two proposals (and there are many others available) are explored (or not) beyond this chapter, the inclusion of Uses and Gratifications theory suggests that any form of Internet behavior is likely goal driven, in a way that either consciously or non-consciously aims to fulfill basic human needs and desires. In doing so, online behavior requires a flexible self that can be shaped according to any goals. Our behaviors are, however, not only guided by these internal and external factors. There are many more facets of human behavior that serve to constrain and regulate our online selves. The final section of this chapter investigates two more aspects of human behavior that are often overlooked in considering online selves: moral and social norms, and the unhealthy online self.

Morals and Social Norms

Morals are messages about human behavior that have usually developed culturally over a long period of time. They are different to social norms, which are also culturally developed over time. Whereas social norms are usually constructed based on human social interaction and reflect the boundaries of acceptable social behavior, morals are more a judgment of right or wrong, based on standards of behavior that have been conveyed through fables and myths, and that are often aligned to religious beliefs. A person’s core self will likely be developed and shaped through childhood and into adulthood by the moral beliefs and principles to which their families adhere, but the social norms which curtail and constrain behavior are set by the society in which they live. Both morals and social norms shape who a person is in any given situation, regardless of whether this is online or offline. Years ago, with developing technology affording the UK only three TV channels, two of which did not broadcast all day, morality and social norms guided a very different presentation of the self to the masses. As late as the 1970s and 1980s, television, film, and radio were very different to the overflow of modern mass media. Where once a Playboy magazine was half hidden on the newsagent’s top shelf, nowadays the Internet can provide similar content, as well as almost anything else the heart desires. For example, Bettie Mae Page was one of the world’s first pin up models of the 1950s; she was socially and morally vilified for posing naked. Compare her photos to the abundance of pornography (legal or otherwise) available nowadays at the click of a mouse. In addition to sexual content, racist, homophobic, nationalist, extremist, and many other types of content are available instantaneously, and influence peoples’ construction of both their core and other selves. This availability has led some authors to suggest that the western world is losing its sense of morality. Human beings are not just blank canvasses, but are sponges that soak up their surroundings, aspects of the people they interact with, and the nuances of the communications in which they engage. Just think about how your own mood might change in a split second because of something someone says to you online. Selves are constantly shaped through every minute of every day. Seibt and Nørskov ( 2012 ) raise questions about the moral self online that have hitherto been largely neglected academically, with most work focusing on social norms. They suggest that an absence of physical and face-to-face interaction online coupled with anonymity leads to individuals presenting multiple different selves online. Offline, most of our moral judgments and behaviors are constrained by social norms of acceptable behavior. Online, these social norms can theoretically be easily avoided through supposedly anonymous online self-presentation to more easily present a judgment that may not be morally reconcilable with a person’s core self. Many will remember Peter Steiner’s ( 1993 ) cartoon from The New Yorker of dogs using the Internet, with the caption “On the Internet, nobody knows you’re a dog.” Absolute anonymity is no longer real online, but, if people perceive themselves to be interacting anonymously online they may dissociate their core self from that behavior to avoid any psychological tension that might arise from the difference in their presented self and core self. In other words, it may be easier to present a different self online—one that is not aligned to the moral and societal norm constraints of offline behavior. There are instances of when this perceived anonymity might be of an advantage to the online self, for example, the exploration of different possible selves outlined by Turkle ( 1995 ). It might, however, also be at the root of behaviors online that can be linked to more detrimental constructions of the self online.

The Unhealthy Self

The final section of this chapter examines the less positive side of online self creation that is unfortunately also gathering pace in light of the supposed reduction in morality and societal norms influencing online behavior. One of the advantages of the Internet is that it opens up a wider playground than people may experience offline in finding like-minded others. As we know from decades of social psychology research, people are drawn to like-minded others (Zajonc, 1968 ). If that like-mindedness is grounded in positive individual characteristics or group spirit, it is likely to pose little to no threat of harm or negative intent to an individual. If, however, the coming together of people online is motivated by nefarious intent, it can have severely detrimental effects to both the online and offline selves of the user. There exists an array of online spaces that are designed to draw people into a promotion of their negative self, and that reaffirm their skewed negative judgments of their self. Websites that promote eating disorders, suicidal ideations, and acts of self-harm are aimed at reeling in vulnerable individuals. Similarly, online groups and forums that promote idealized political or religious views, and which are aimed at destruction and devastation through terrorist ideations, are all too readily available online to anyone who is seeking a sense of belonging. While people may initially be drawn into such arenas based on ideas or behaviors that cause them to seek out like minded others, others may simply be curious. However, once connected to those others, the continuous positive reinforcement of their ideas and skewed self-perceptions may make it difficult to subsequently extricate themselves from that space. For example, if a person who is extremely underweight finds themselves drawn to a pro-anorexia website, they may see images of other similarly underweight individuals who have received praise and adoration for their look. This will incur in them a sense of acceptability for that body style and reaffirm their positive view of self. They may also interact with other members of the group, who tell them that they look good and provide reinforcement and encouragement that they would unlikely receive offline for their eating behavior. This works both ways, as there are websites available that cater for overeaters, and the notion of big being beautiful. Of importance here is that once a person feels accepted and positively reinforced within such an online environment, the people with whom they interact can become hyperpersonal to the individual (Walther, 1996 ). That is, the individual may start to neglect offline (and possibly more realistic) relationships that offer healthier interactions and opinions to focus on the online relationships. If someone is being told what they want to hear, they are unlikely to want to remove themselves from that situation and will shun any situation which does not offer that positive reinforcement of their self, or which questions their self. This unhealthy self may then come to over-ride any other selves the individual has, online or offline, which could eventually lead to a whole host of negative consequences.

Conclusions

This very brief outline of a few carefully chosen theories shows that many factors come into play in how we construct the self online. From our current behavioral goals to the mode of communication used, from the ways in which any available social cues are interpreted to their re-creation in some types of online interaction, each shapes both how we perceive and interpret those others, as well as how we subsequently craft and create a self in response. Through words, emoticons, or GIFs, as well as the circular interaction with a single other or multiple groups of others, these are but a few of the facets of human behavior that feed into our online self-creations which have been conceptualized theoretically. In his chapter in this volume, Dr. Chris Fullwood picks up where we leave off and explores further exactly how people manipulate their online self-presentations.

Arkin, R. M. ( 1981 ). Self-presentational styles. In J. T. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression management theory and social psychological research (pp. 311–333). New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Google Preview

Attrill, A. ( 2015 ). Cyberpsychology . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Attrill-Smith, A. , & Shaban, M. ( n.d. ). Who filters on Instagram? Self-presentation, perception control and self-esteem predictors of Instagram filtering.

Baumeister, R. F. ( 1999 ). The nature and structure of the self: An overview. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), The self in social psychology (pp. 1–20). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

Campbell, J. , Trapnell, P. , Heine, S. J. , Katz, I. M. , Lavallee, L. R. , & Lehman, D. R. ( 1996 ). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70, 141–156.

Chapman, D. S. , & Webster, J. ( 2001 ). Rater correction processes in applicant selection using videoconference technology: The role of attributions.   Journal of Applied Social Psychology 31(12), 2518–2537.

Davis, M. H. , & Kraus, L. A. ( 1989 ). Social contact, loneliness, and mass media use: A test of two hypotheses.   Journal of Applied Social Psychology 19, 1100–1124.

Deci, E. L. , & Ryan, R. M. ( 2000 ). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behaviour. Psychological Inquiry 11(4), 227–268.

DeHaan, S. , Kuper, L. E. , Magee, J. C. , Bigelow, L. , & Mustanski, B. S. ( 2013 ). The interplay between online and offline explorations of identity, relationships, and sex: A mixed-methods study with LGBT youth.   The Journal of Sex Research 50(5), 421–434.

Festinger, L. ( 1954 ). A theory of social comparison processes.   Human Relations 7(2), 117–140.

Fullwood, C. ( 2007 ). The effect of mediation on impression formation: A comparison of face-to-face & video-mediated conditions.   Applied Ergonomics 38, 267–273.

Goffman, E. ( 1959 ). The presentation of self in everyday life . Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Higgins, E. T. ( 1987 ). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect.   Psychological Review 94, 319–340.

Hogg, M. A. , & Vaughan, G. M. ( 2017 ). Social Psychology . Pearson. UK.

Katz, E. , Blumler, J. G. , & Gurevitch, M. ( 1973 ). Uses and gratifications research.   Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(4), 509–523.

Leary, M. R. ( 1995 ). Self-presentation, impression management and interpersonal behaviour . Madison, WI: Brown and Benchmark.

McCarthey, S. J. , & Moje, E. B. ( 2002 ). Identity matters.   Reading Research Quarterly 37(2), 228–238.

McKenna, K. Y. A. , & Green, A. S. , & Gleason, M. E. J. ( 2002 ). Relationship formation on the Internet: What’s the big attraction?   Journal of Social Issues 58(1), 9–31.

Mustanski, B. , Newcomb, M. , & Garofalo, R. ( 2011 ). Mental health of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth: A developmental resiliency perspective.   Journal of Gay and Lesbian Social Services 23, 204–225.

Rai, R. , & Attrill, A. (2014). The effects of synchronous and asynchronous internet communication, personality, and representations of the self on the uptake of online video communication. Poster presented at the 16th International Conference on Human Computer Interaction. 22–27 June, Heraklion, Crete, Greece.

Schlegel, R. J. , Vess, M. , & Arndt, J. ( 2012 ). To discover or to create: Metaphors and the true self.   Journal of Personality 80, 969–993.

Seibt, J. , & Nørskov, M. ( 2012 ). “Embodying” the Internet: Towards the moral self via communication robots? Philosophy and Technology 25, 285–307.

Steiner, P. (1993, July 5). On the Internet, nobody knows you’re a dog . Cartoon published in The New Yorker .

Tesser, A. , Millar, M. , & Moore, J. ( 1988 ). Some affective consequences of social comparison and reflection processes: The pain and pleasure of being close.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54(1), 49–61.

Turkle, S. ( 1995 ). Life on the screen: Identity in the age of the Internet . New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

Valkenburg, P. M. , Schouten, A. P. , & Peter, J. ( 2005 ). Adolescents’ identity experiments on the Internet.   New Media and Society 7(3), 383–402.

Walther, J. B. ( 1992 ). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational perspective.   Communication Research 19, 52–90.

Walther, J. B. ( 1996 ). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction.   Communication Research 23, 3–43.

Zajonc, R. B. ( 1968 ). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.   Journal of Personality & Social Psychology Monograph Supplement 9, 1–27.

  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Institutional account management
  • Rights and permissions
  • Get help with access
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection

Logo of phenaturepg

How different online self-presentations relate to life satisfaction differently in college students: The role of positive online feedback and self-esteem

2 Jinan University, Guangzhou, China

Ruonan Zhai

1 School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan, China

Pengyan Dai

Jieling cui, associated data.

All data generated or analysed in this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Not applicable.

In the Internet age, some online factors, such as online self-presentation, related to life satisfaction have received much attention. However, it is unclear whether and how different strategies of online self-presentation are linked to an individual’s life satisfaction differently. Accordingly, the present study examined the possible different relationships between different online self-presentations and life satisfaction with a sample of 460 Chinese college students. Using a series of questionnaires, a moderated mediation model was built in which positive online feedback was a mediator and self-esteem was a moderator. The results indicated that: (1) positive self-presentation was negatively associated with college students’ life satisfaction, whereas honest self-presentation was positively related to it; (2) positive online feedback was a significant mediator in such relationships; (3) the mediation process was moderated by self-esteem. Specifically, positive self-presentation was negatively related to positive online feedback only for high self-esteem college students, but negatively associated with life satisfaction only for low self-esteem ones. By contrast, honest self-presentation was positively associated with positive online feedback despite the level of self-esteem, but positively linked with life satisfaction only for those with low self-esteem. The findings suggest that honest rather than positive online self-presentation should be conducive to college students’ life satisfaction, particularly for those with low self-esteem. The implications were discussed.

Introduction

Life satisfaction, as a key indicator of well-being, refers to how an individual overall assesses and feels his or her lives during most of the time or a certain period of time (Diener et al., 2002 ; Maddux, 2018 ). It has been shown to be positively related to many personal psychological, behavioral, interpersonal, and social outcomes (Proctor et al., 2009 ). It can mediate the association of adverse life events with suicidal ideation as well (Yang et al., 2020 ), and improving individuals’ life satisfaction helps reduce the risks of mental disorder (Chen et al., 2017 ). Therefore, identifying its contributing factors has long been concerned by scholars.

Traditionally, when an individual has a high quality of social network and perceives much social support from this network, he or she will have a high level of life satisfaction (Lebacq et al., 2019 ) because good interpersonal communication produces positive emotion and affect (Diener et al., 1991 ). In the Internet age, however, online social networking sites (SNSs) have been indispensable mediums for individuals to present themselves and communicate with others (Pew Research Center, 2018 ). They remain an effective way of online socialization for individuals who are capable of maintaining personal relationships with friends from near and far (Brailovskaia et al., 2020 ). Therefore, some online factors related to life satisfaction have attracted much attention. This topic is especially important in certain periods, such as a special time of the COVID-19 pandemic when individuals have fewer face-to-face social contacts and turn to SNSs for happiness.

On SNSs, individuals can post photos and videos, likes, comments, and share their personal stories with others (Aljasir et al., 2017 ; Kuss & Griffiths, 2017 ). When they present this personal information, some individuals may selectively show information that are beneficial to the self in order to actively make themselves look positive in public (Wright et al., 2018 ); In contrast, others would rather express themselves in a real and sincere way, disclosing their personal information deeply. Numerous studies have indicated that the quantity of online self-disclosure is positively linked to subject well-being (SWB) (Bij de Vaate et al., 2019 ; Chan, 2021 ; Jang et al., 2018 ; Kim & Lee, 2011 ; Tyler et al., 2018 ) or life satisfaction (Kereste & Tulhofer, 2019 ; Pang, 2018 ; Wang, 2013 ). However, it is not well answered whether and how different self-presentation strategies are associated with them differently. Accordingly, the current study explored the possible different relationships between different self-presentations on SNSs and life satisfaction and a mediating role of positive online feedback in these associations as well as a moderation role of self-esteem in the mediating process.

Online self-presentation and life satisfaction

In terms of relationship management, the strategies of online self-presentation can be divided into two contrasting categories (Kim & Lee, 2011 ): positive self-presentation and honest self-presentation. The former refers to selectively revealing or highlighting one’s positive aspects in order to create a good impression on SNSs. In contrast, the latter is more strongly associated with one’s honest self that represents one’s real characteristics, reflecting the way users authentically disclose their feelings, thoughts as well as life events on SNSs.

According to the self-discrepancy theory posited by Higgins ( 1987 ), people often copmare their own actual self with the ideal self, while a larger discrepancy between the two will lead to more negative psychological outcomes, such as disappointment and anxiety. Actually, Facebook users usually disclose more positive emotions rather than negative ones on Facebook (Ziegele & Reinecke, 2017 ). This positivity bias on Facebook seems likely to attenuate the willingness to present real, but negative information (e.g., distress). It will lead to a larger difference between the real self and the virtual self, and then produce negative emotions (Grieve et al., 2020 ). In this sense, inauthentic self-presentation on SNSs can be related to many psychological problems of maladjustment (Grieve & Watkinson, 2016 ), such as high social anxiety (Duan et al., 2020 ; Twomey & O'Reilly, 2017 ), low self-esteem (Manago, 2015 ), poor psychosocial well-being (Michikyan et al., 2014 ), and problematic social networks use (Li et al., 2018 ). In other words, concealing one’s self behind positive self-presentation may result in negative emotions and adverse thoughts (D’agata & Holden, 2018 ; Jackson & Luchner, 2017 ).

In contrast, individuals can have higher self-concept clarity regarding social anxiety (Orr & Moscovitch, 2015 ) and improve subjective happiness in honest self-presentation (Jang et al., 2018 ). Sharing honest personal information, thoughts, and feelings enables people to express themselves, buffer negative feelings, and provide psychological benefits (Kim & Dindia, 2011 ). Research has found that people who present their real self on SNSs have more positive affect, less negative affect (Reinecke & Trepte, 2014 ), greater happiness particularly for those high self-esteem individuals (Jang et al., 2018 ), and higher levels of SWB (Lee & Borah, 2020 ).

Social penetration theory (SPT; Taylor, 1968 ) can also explain the association of honest self-presentation with life satisfaction from the aspect of interpersonal relationships. SPT proposes that information disclosed to others has different types and layers, and that the development of relationships depends on how individuals reveal their personal information, such as their attitudes, feelings, and likes, to each other (Taylor & Altman, 1987 ). Honest self-presentation on SNSs is a special way for individuals to present their true self to their friends (Nadkarni & Hofmann, 2012 ), which can enhance interpersonal trust and the intimate relationship between friends (Lin & Utz, 2017 ), and help individuals accumulate social capital and obtain social support (Sosik & Bazarova, 2014 ). Specifically, honest self-presentation on SNSs helps to construct and maintain good social ties (Lee et al., 2011 ), and contributes to relief of individual depression and loneliness, and to improvement of individual life satisfaction (Chai et al., 2018 ; Grieve & Watkinson, 2016 ). Meanwhile, for all tested demographic groups, interpersonal relationships have been found to be an obvious contributor to life satisfaction (Bermack, 2014 ).

Despite accumulating evidence supporting different relationships between different self-presentations and life satisfaction, empirical evidence of comparing them directly in a same study is very scarce. Thus, by incorporating previous literature, we aimed to fill this gap and hypothesized that positive self-presentation would be negatively linked with life satisfaction, whereas honest self-presentation would be positively related to it (H1).

Positive online feedback as a mediator

When presenting ourselves, we generally imagine and speculate how audience respond to us, and typically search for such feedback from others to evaluate ourselves (Goffman, 1959 ). On SNSs, a unique feature is that audience feedback is available, immediate and quantifiable (Schlosser, 2020 ). As a result, users can often obtain timely feedback after online self-presentation (Metzler & Scheithauer, 2018 ). Among them, positive online feedback is an important type of social support, mainly in the form of positive and timely evaluations during online interactions (Liu & Brown, 2014 ). Specifically, it refers to the supportive responses that individuals receive after they post or update personal information on SNSs, such as likes (Metzler & Scheithauer, 2017 ) and positive comments (Bazarova et al., 2015 ). Previous research has indicated that adults’ different areas of self-presentation on Facebook are related to positive feedback from the online audience (Liu & Brown, 2014 ; Yang & Brown, 2016 ). This perceived positive online feedback also can lead to positive social consequences (Brailovskaia & Margraf, 2019 ). Positive feedback indicates being accepted, concerned, and socially supported, implying that the responder has positive attitude to the individual, and solidifying their relationship (Lee et al., 2014 ; Liu & Brown, 2014 ). Accordingly, positive online feedback should be a key mediator in the relationship between online self-presentation and life satisfaction.

In terms of the relationship between different self-presentation and positive online feedback, positive self-presentation can not contribute to people’s mental health or relationship if people are unable to trust in others on SNSs (Kim & Baek, 2014 ), whereas those who present themselves in a “courageous” and even self-deprecating way acquire much social support (Bareket-Bojmel et al., 2016 ). Although we usually regard self-derogation as a problematic behavior which may cause to adverse social outcomes (e.g., social reject) (Ollier-Malaterre et al., 2013 ), some studies support that self-derogation is not always unfavorable while self-enhancement does not always lead to positive outcomes. For example, research has found that people tend to consider those who like to enhance themselves but fail to show an expected performance actually as boastful and give them low evaluations (Schlenker & Leary, 1982 ), whereas undergraduates who choose to self-derogation when presenting themselves on SNSs receive increased positive feedback from their social network (Bareket-Bojmel et al., 2016 ).

As found, the deep and real self-disclosure on SNSs can gain more social support (Hampton & Lu, 2015 ; Ko & Kuo, 2009 ; Seo et al., 2016 ). When individuals present broader, deeper, and more authentic information on SNSs, they will get more online feedback from their friends (Yang, 2014 ). Only when individuals seek support via honest and sincere self-disclosure, can they receive it with a greater likelihood from others (Greene et al., 2006 ; Li et al., 2020 ), which could be beneficial to their SWB (Bij de Vaate et al., 2019 ; Luo & Hancock, 2019 ). While presenting a positive but untrue self, one can not receive helpful social support from their Facebook friends and thereby can not really feel happy (Oh et al., 2014 ).

On the basis of the above literature, we concluded that positive self-presentation would be negatively linked to positive online feedback but honest self-presentation would be positively associated with it.

In terms of the relationship between positive online feedback and life satisfaction, positive online feedback has been consistently found to be positively related to individuals’ social support (Lee et al., 2014 ; Liu & Brown, 2014 ; Wohn et al., 2016 ) and life satisfaction (Satici & Uysal, 2015 ; Wenninger et al., 2014 ). According to uncertainty reduction theory, interactive and verbal strategies are good ways for individuals to solve the relational uncertainty (Jin & Pena, 2010 ; Neuliep, 2012 ). Positive online feedback is very common during online interaction. It helps individuals understand how their friends see them by providing certain information for them (Brashers et al., 2004 ). Previous research has also found that the affirmation and recognition from others can effectively improve the attitude and evaluation towards oneself (Sung et al., 2016 ; Yang, 2014 ), which can improve their life satisfaction and happiness (Scissors et al., 2016 ). Given that social support is a vital source of happiness, and related to improved well-being (Haber et al., 2007 ), positive feedback, a more specific type of social support, provided by one’s online friends such as likes and comments may also positively contribute to the individual’s happiness and SWB (Kim & Lee, 2011 ; Zell & Moeller, 2018 ). In other words, the more social support from positive online feedback individuals perceived, the higher life satisfaction level they had (Nabi et al., 2013 ; Shahyad et al., 2011 ).

Accordingly, by incorporating previous literature, we predicted that positive online feedback would be a key mediator in the associations of different self-presentations with life satisfaction (H2). It has been shown to mediate the relationship between honest self-presentation and life satisfaction (Liu et al., 2016 ). It can mediate the association between self-disclosure on SNSs and bonding social capital as well (D. Liu & Brown, 2014 ). It also plays a mediating role in the link between online self-presentation and individuals’ self-esteem (Meeus et al., 2019 ). However, empirical evidence of its mediating role in the different relationships between different self-presentations and life satisfaction is scanty. Therefore, we aimed to narrow this gap in the current study and tested this hypothesis.

Self-esteem as a moderator

In addition to the mediating role of positive online feedback at an environmental level, self-esteem, one’s positive or negative attitudes towards the self (Rosenberg, 1965 ), may serve as a key moderator in this mediation model at an individual level.

According to previous experimental results, those high self-esteem individuals not only show more defense against negative information, but also accept less negative information (Zhou et al., 2018 ). By contrast, individuals with low self-esteem are not only more likely to perceive external rejection information (Zhou et al., 2018 ), but also more likely to perceive others’ behavior as rejection (Kashdan et al., 2014 ), thus perceiving less positive feedback. This is because low self-esteem individuals can not focus on their own qualities and do not have the ability to overcome negative or rejection information (Tazghini & Siedlecki, 2013 ). Therefore, for people with low self-esteem, in the case of receiving a large amount of feedback after self-presentation on SNSs, those negative rejection messages may attract their attention first. In the study of Cameron et al. ( 2009 ), individuals whose self-esteem is either high or low expressed failure information to their partners and received same positive feedback, but people with low self-esteem could not correctly perceive the positive feedback provided by their partners, and reported less positive feedback than their counterparts.

The “poor get poorer” Internet theory proposes that overuse of SNSs may destroy individuals’ well-being, and this negative effect is even worse particularly for people who lack adequate psychosocial support from others in daily life (Selfhout et al., 2009 ; Snodgrass et al., 2018 ). Thus, individuals with low self-esteem may perceive less positive feedback and life satisfaction than those high self-esteem ones when they present themselves on SNSs. Furthermore, according to the self-discrepancy theory (Higgins, 1987 ), when individuals present themselves positively on SNSs, the discrepancies between their actual self-state and ideal self-state will be greater for low self-esteem individuals than for high self-esteem ones, signifying more loss of positive outcomes and more dejection-related emotions. In addition, high self-esteem can operate as a buffer which can mitigate the negative effect of using positive self-presentation due to its low vulnerability to loss and strong resilience, in accordance with the buffer hypothesis (Arndt & Goldenberg, 2002 ), but people with low self-esteem would suffer a lot. Consequently, we deduced that positive self-presentation would be related to less positive feedback and life satisfaction, particularly for low self-esteem individuals.

In contrast, individuals with high self-esteem feel better about themselves and are more likely to believe themselves as attractive or popular than do their counterparts (Wood & Forest, 2016 ). They believe they are lovable, deserving of attention, and feel that if they are in trouble, others will respond to their needs and be ready to help them (Palermiti et al., 2017 ). As a result, they are able to feel more loved and accepted by others. In addition, they are more likely to perceive others’ supportive responses when they present themselves on SNSs (Greitemeyer et al., 2014 ; Sun et al., 2017 ). Thus, high self-esteem individuals would be more confident and easeful when showing the self honestly to online friends than low self-esteem ones. By disclosing true oneself on SNSs, they would also reveal more competence and thus perceived more positive online feedback and life satisfaction (Jang et al., 2018 ; Ryan & Deci, 2000 ).

Similarly, from the aspect of “rich get richer” theory, individuals who have good social skills and adequate social support will benefit more from the Internet use (Kraut et al., 2002 ; Reer & Krämer, 2017 ). There is agreement that more sociable people are more capable of making better use of the opportunities offered by SNS to strengthen their social network (Ross et al., 2009 ). For example, individuals not only can get all kinds of valuable support and help from their friends through using SNSs (Indian & Grieve, 2014 ; Wohn & Larose, 2014 ), but also can meet the needs of self-worth and self-integration and have relatively pleasant emotional experience (Wise et al., 2010 ). Therefore, SNSs use can improve their life satisfaction (Pang, 2018 ; Shahyad et al., 2011 ). Accordingly, those high self-esteem individuals may particularly perceive more positive feedback and life satisfaction when they present themselves honestly on SNSs than those low self-esteem individuals.

The possible moderation role of self-esteem, however, has not been fully explored in the previous research, we therefore examined it and hypothesized that positive self-presentation would be related to low sense of positive online feedback and life satisfaction, particularly for those low self-esteem individuals, whereas it would be weaker for those high self-esteem individuals (H3) and that honest self-presentation would be related to heightened sense of positive online feedback and life satisfaction, particularly for high self-esteem individuals, whereas it would be weaker for those low self-esteem individuals (H4).

In sum, as shown in Fig.  1 , we examined a moderated mediation model in which positive online feedback would differently mediate the different associations of different self-presentation with college students’ life satisfaction and self-esteem would differently moderate this mediating process as well.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 12144_2022_3444_Fig1_HTML.jpg

Hypothesized model

Participants

Participants included 460 Chinese college students from one normal university located in eastern China. A priori power analysis with the G*Power 3 software package (Faul et al., 2009 ) indicated that the sample size that would provide an adequate power (0.95) and a moderate effect size ( f 2  = 0.15) at a 0.05 significance level (α) using a hierarchical regression analysis with 4 tested predictors would be 129 participants, and thus justifying this sample size. Forty students were excluded due to missing data or inaccurately completing the measures. Ultimately, 420 students completed the survey with a response rate of 91%. The final sample consisted of 244 (58.10%) females and 176 (41.90%) males aged from 18–24 ( M  = 20.23, SD  = 4.59). Among these students, 145 (34.52%) were freshman, 106 (25.24%) were sophomore, 80 (19.05%) were junior, and 89 (21.19%) were senior; 276 (65.71%) were from rural areas and 144 (34.29%) were from urban areas.

Online self-presentation

Online self-presentation was assessed with nine items adapted from positive self-presentation and honest self-presentation scale (Kim & Lee, 2011 ) by Niu et al. ( 2015 ). The positive self-presentation subscale consists of 5 items, which is designed to evaluate how individuals selectively present positive aspects of themselves on SNSs (e.g., ‘‘I post photos that only show the happy side of me’’). The honest self-presentation subscale consists of 4 items that assesses the extent to which individuals honestly present their true selves on SNSs (e.g., ‘‘I don’t mind writing about bad things that happen to me when I update my status’’). All items were answered on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = ‘‘strongly disagree’’; 7 = ‘‘strongly agree’’). A higher averaged score indicated more positive/honest self-presentation on SNSs. The Chinese version of this scale has demonstrated good psychometric properties among Chinese college students (α = 0.82, 0.74; Niu et al., 2015 ). In the present study, the Cronbach’s αs for the two subscales were 0.85 and 0.79 respectively.

Positive online feedback

Positive online feedback was measured using the positive online feedback scale adapted from Liu and Brown ( 2014 ). The scale is composed of 5 items (e.g., “When I update my status on SNSs”; “When I post photos on SNSs”) assessing how often participants received positive feedback on SNSs. All items were answered on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “never”; 5 = “frequently”). The scale focuses on the overall frequency of positive online feedback rather than its level of positivity, as it is difficult for college students to determine the level of positive feedback (Liu & Brown, 2014 ). The higher the averaged score, the more frequently the participants were to receive positive feedback from friends while using SNSs. The Chinese version of this scale has demonstrated good psychometric properties among Chinese college students (α = 0.90; Jiang et al., 2019 ). The Cronbach’s α was 0.92 in the present study.

Self-esteem

Self-esteem was measured by a revised Chinese version of Rosenberg ( 1965 ) Self-Esteem scale. The scale is composed of 10 items (e.g., “I am able to do things as well as most other people.”). Considering that the 8 th item in the scale is not suitable for measuring Chinese self-esteem due to cultural difference (Tian, 2006 ), only the remaining 9 items were used in the present study. They were answered on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = ‘‘strongly disagree’’; 4 = ‘‘strongly agree’’). A higher averaged score indicated a higher self-esteem. The Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.90 in the current study.

Life satisfaction

Life satisfaction was measured with six items developed by Wang and Shi ( 2003 ). The items (e.g., “How satisfied are you with your current life?”) were answered on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “strongly disagree”; 7 = “strongly agree”) and then averaged to form an overall score of life satisfaction. The higher the averaged score, the higher of life satisfaction. In the current study the Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.78.

Before the survey began, informed consent was obtained from participants, and the study plan was approved by the Scientific Research Ethic Committee at our university. All participants completed a series of anonymous questionnaires at their classrooms administered by well-trained psychological graduate assistants. The authenticity, independence and completeness of their answers as well as the confidentiality of the information collected were emphasized to all participants. It took approximately 15 min to complete all of the measures.

Data analysis

In the current study, data analyses were conducted using SPSS 22.0 and PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013 ). SPSS 22.0 was used to investigate the correlations among the main variables, and PROCESS macro for SPSS 22.0 to estimate the models. Then, four models were constructed. Among them, two models examined the mediation role of online positive feedback in the relationship between different online self-presentation and life satisfaction. Following MacKinnon’s ( 2008 ) four-step procedure, Hayes’ ( 2013 ) PROCESS macro (model 4) was used to estimate the mediating effect. The other two models used Hayes’ ( 2013 ) PROCESS macro (model 8) to tested the moderation role of self-esteem in the two mediation models. Specifically, the current study assessed the effects of moderation of self-esteem on the association of online self-presentation with positive online feedback and on the association of online self-presentation with life satisfaction. In addition, values at two levels of self-esteem ( M  ± 1 SD ) were used to calculate the simple slopes. All the variables involved in the analysis were standardized.

As suggested by Preacher and Hayes ( 2008 ), the present study used a bootstrap approach to decide the significance of the mediation of positive online feedback. Specifically, 5,000 bootstrapped samples and 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs) were used in this approach. If the CI did not contain zero, the effect was seen as significant.

Descriptive statistics

Table ​ Table1 1 shows the means, standard deviations, and Pearson’s correlation coefficients for all study variables. As expected, the variables were all correlated with each other.

Descriptive statistics for study variables

N  = 420. * p  < 0.05, ** p  < 0.01, *** p  < 0.001. The same below

Testing the mediation model

Positive self-presentation as a predictor.

By the preliminary examining, all independent variables’ variance inflation factors were less than 2.00, therefore there was no multicollinearity.

The results (see Table ​ Table2) 2 ) revealed that positive self-presentation was negatively related to both positive online feedback and life satisfaction, while the latter two were positively associated with each other. Then, the mediation test showed that the path from positive self-presentation to life satisfaction through positive online feedback was significant, ab  = – 0.19, Boot SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [– 0.24, – 0.13]. It indicated that positive online feedback partially mediated the relationship between positive self-presentation and life satisfaction but in the opposite direction. The mediation effect was – 0.19, accounting for 42.61% of the total effect.

Testing the mediating role of positive online feedback in the relationship between positive self-presentation and life satisfaction

PS Positive self-presentation, POF Positive online feedback, LS Life satisfaction

Honest self-presentation as a predictor

To test another mediation model, the same procedure was used. As shown in Table ​ Table3, 3 , however, we found that honest self-presentation was positively related to both positive online feedback and life satisfaction, while the latter two were positively associated with each other. Then, the mediation test showed that the association of honest self-presentation with life satisfaction through positive online feedback was significant, ab  = 0.22, Boot SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [0.17, 0.28]. It indicated that positive online feedback also played a partial mediating role in the relationship between honest self-presentation and life satisfaction with a mediating effect of 0.22, which accounted for 44.02% of the total effect.

Testing the mediating role of positive online feedback in the relationship between honest self-presentation and life satisfaction

HS Honest self-presentation, POF Positive online feedback, LS Life satisfaction

Testing the moderated mediation models

As Table ​ Table4 4 illustrates, positive self-presentation was negatively correlated with positive online feedback, while self-esteem was positively linked with it, and the interaction between them was significant on positive online feedback as well, indicating a moderating role of self-esteem in the relationship between positive self-presentation and positive online feedback.

Testing the moderated mediation effects with positive self-presentation as a predictor

PS Positive self-presentation, POF POF, LS Life satisfaction

To better understand the moderation effect of self-esteem, Fig.  2 shows the plot of the association of positive self-presentation with positive online feedback at two levels of self-esteem ( M  ± 1 SD ). As shown in Fig.  2 , positive self-presentation was only significantly associated with positive online feedback for participants with high self-esteem ( β simple  = – 0.45, p  < 0.001), while not for those with low self-esteem ( β simple  = – 0.002, p  > 0.05). Although college students with high self-esteem received more positive feedback than those with low self-esteem ( t  = – 10.97, p  < 0.001), they would receive significantly decreased positive feedback when in high levels of positive self-presentation.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 12144_2022_3444_Fig2_HTML.jpg

Moderating role of self-esteem in relationship between positive self-presentation and positive online feedback

On the other hand, positive self-presentation was negatively linked to life satisfaction, and its interaction with self-esteem was significant on life satisfaction as well, indicating that self-esteem also moderated the relationship between positive self-presentation and life satisfaction.

As shown in Fig.  3 , a simple slope test found that positive self-presentation was only significantly related to life satisfaction among college students with low self-esteem ( β simple  = – 0.21, p  < 0.001) while not among their counterparts ( β simple  = – 0.04, p  > 0.05). It indicated that participants with low self-esteem not only perceived less life satisfaction than those with high self-esteem ( t  = – 13.18, p  < 0.001), but also further perceived significantly decreased life satisfaction when in high levels of positive self-presentation.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 12144_2022_3444_Fig3_HTML.jpg

Moderating role of self-esteem in relationship between positive self-presentation and life satisfaction

To test another conceptual model, the same procedure was used. The results (see Table ​ Table5) 5 ) showed that honest self-presentation and self-esteem were both positively associated with positive online feedback and their interaction was significant on positive online feedback as well.

Testing the moderated mediation effects with honest self-presentation as a predictor

HS Honest self-presentation, POF POF, LS Life satisfaction

As shown in Fig.  4 , a simple slope test found that honest self-presentation was positively linked with positive online feedback either for college students with low self-esteem ( β simple  = 0.19, p  < 0.001) or for those with high self-esteem, ( β simple  = 0.57, p  < 0.001), but the latter slope was obviously higher. It indicated that college students with high self-esteem would particularly benefit from high levels of honest self-presentation to positive online feedback.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 12144_2022_3444_Fig4_HTML.jpg

Moderating role of self-esteem in relationship between honest self-presentation and positive online feedback

On the other hand, honest self-presentation was positively linked with life satisfaction, and its interaction with self-esteem was significant on life satisfaction as well.

As shown in Fig.  5 , a simple slope test found that honest self-presentation was only significantly and positively related to life satisfaction among college students with low self-esteem ( β simple  = 0.17, p  < 0.001) while not among those with high self-esteem ( β simple  = 0.001, p  > 0.05). Although college students with low self-esteem perceived less life satisfaction than their counterparts ( t  = – 13.18, p  < 0.001), they would perceive significantly increased life satisfaction when in high levels of honest self-presentation.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 12144_2022_3444_Fig5_HTML.jpg

Moderating role of self-esteem in relationship between honest self-presentation and life satisfaction

Although the association between online self-presentation and individual life satisfaction has been studied in previous research, it is yet unclear whether and how different strategies of self-presentation are related to life satisfaction differently. The current study answered this question by revealing the direct and indirect relationships between different self-presentation and life satisfaction through positive online feedback and the moderating effect of self-esteem on them.

In accordance with H1, the results showed that positive self-presentation was negatively related to college students’ life satisfaction whereas honest self-presentation was positively linked with it.

Although SNSs offer college students a platform to connect to known and unknown online friends, present their own information, and look for others’ information (Griffiths et al., 2014 ; Kuss & Griffiths, 2017 ), the so-called friends on SNSs not only overlap with the social circle in real life, but also involve some strange net friends. Therefore, some college students will try to create a positive image by presenting positive information selectively. When individuals present themselves positively, they deliberately hide negative information and filter their cognition of themselves, real life and future negative aspects (Wright et al., 2018 ). Such cognitive filtering that cannot reflect the real situation will hinder their self-integration and self-acceptance (Carson & Langer, 2006 ). In line with the self-discrepancy theory (Higgins, 1987 ), this will lead to a greater self-discrepancy between the actual self and the virtual self, and then produce negative emotions (Grieve et al., 2020 ). Similarly, other studies have also indicated that a larger discrepancy between the real self and the ideal self will cause to greater psychological discomfort (Grieve et al., 2020 ; Heng et al., 2018 ), which further reduces their life satisfaction.

On the contrary, honest self-presentation seems to contribute to college students’ life satisfaction, as found in previous research (Kim & Lee, 2011 ). On the one side, it can be interpreted that individuals can present their real information and status on SNSs to carry out self-affirmation (Toma, 2013 ), thus obtaining social support (Ko & Kuo, 2009 ) and improving SWB (Bij de Vaate et al., 2019 ; Luo & Hancock, 2019 ). On the other side, according to the social penetration theory, honest self-presentation is beneficial for deepening interpersonal relationships, gaining interpersonal trust, and increasing social support (Lin & Utz, 2017 ; Sosik & Bazarova, 2014 ), which helps improve life satisfaction. As a result, when college students present themselves more authentically on SNSs, their life satisfaction is higher.

The mediating role of positive online feedback

Consistent with H2, we found that positive online feedback mediated the different relationships between different self-presentations and college students’ life satisfaction in different directions.

This is possible because when individuals present themselves positively rather than honestly on SNSs, other people may fail to form trust in them which will be unfavorable for their mental health and interpersonal relationship (Kim & Baek, 2014 ), whereas they will receive more positive feedback when they disclose themselves honestly even negatively (Bareket-Bojmel et al., 2016 ). While positive self-presentation may maintain a level of positive self-image, it hides the negative side of individuals, which may go against the development of deep intimate relationships and the acquisition of beneficial social support (Oh et al., 2014 ). Only when individuals seek support through disclosing themselves honestly, they can receive it with a great likelihood from online friends (Greene et al., 2006 ), and such support has been consistently shown to be positively linked to their SWB (Bij de Vaate et al., 2019 ; Luo & Hancock, 2019 ). While individuals hide behind a smiling online mask, it is difficult for them to acquire meaningful social support from online friends (Oh et al., 2014 ). Meanwhile, when individuals perceive less social support, their life satisfaction and overall SWB decrease, resulting in fewer positive emotions and more negative emotions (Kong & You, 2013 ). Therefore, when college students used more positive self-presentation on SNSs, they would perceive less positive online feedback, and thereby decrease their life satisfaction.

By contrast, because honest self-presentation is an individual’s real presentation on SNSs, which is sincere and open, it can help an individual reduce negative emotions or attitudes (Grieve & Watkinson, 2016 ), get more social support (Yang, 2014 ) and thereby improve life satisfaction (Chai et al., 2018 ) by showing one’s real side and sharing the current real situation. Although honest self-presentation may present negative information or attitudes about oneself, an individual’s unadorned and authentic presentation of information enables friends to perceive their current real situation, and thus provide their support and help more easily (Greene et al., 2006 ; Kim & Lee, 2011 ). As well, from the perspective of social penetration theory, honest self-presentation on SNSs can increase interpersonal trust and intimacy (Jian & Li, 2018 ), maintain interpersonal relationships and obtain social support (Ko & Kuo, 2009 ), and enable individuals to obtain emotional social support and social identification (Xie, 2014 ). Thus, honest self-presentation can enable college students to know themselves more objectively and clearly, perceive more positive online feedback, and then improve life satisfaction.

The moderating role of self-esteem

Partially consistent with H3, more positive self-presentation was found to be connected with less life satisfaction, only for those low self-esteem individuals, but associated with less positive online feedback only for those high self-esteem ones.

These findings partially supported the “poor get poorer” theory, which believes individuals with inadequate development resources in their real lives might erode their well-being through bad online experience (Selfhout et al., 2009 ; Snodgrass et al., 2018 ). They partially supported Higgins’s ( 1987 ) self-discrepancy theory as well, according to which people with low self-esteem will perceive larger differences between the real self and the ideal self when they present themselves positively but not honestly and thereby experience more dissatisfaction with themselves and their lives.

However, it is not the case for the results about positive online feedback. Several reasons can be considered. First, life satisfaction is a judgmental process that based on self-selected standards, which is greatly affected by the level of individual self-esteem. However, the frequency of positive online feedback is an objective component, which has little to do with individual experience/perception to some extent. Second, high self-esteem may be regarded as ostentation when individuals presenting excessive positive self, which will cause the audience’s disgust (Schlenker, 1980 ) and then lead to a significant decrease in positive feedback. By contrast, the audiences, particularly those who know the low self-esteem individuals, may relatively tolerate and encourage them when they present some positive information of themselves. Certainly, another possible explanation of the result about low self-esteem is that individuals with low self-esteem often look down on their own worth (Forest & Wood, 2012 ), and thus are not only more likely to perceive external rejection information (Zhou et al., 2018 ), but also more likely to perceive others’ behavior as rejection (Kashdan et al., 2014 ). They are also inclined to concentrate on concealing their perceived shortcomings and true feelings (Baumeister et al., 1989 ), which may hinder their social support seek and acquisition and thereby receive less positive feedback (Oh et al., 2014 ). Therefore, whether they have less or more positive self-presentation, the frequency of positive online feedback they received were always lower with no significant change, compared to their counterparts, as indicated in the present study. These results suggest that positive but not real self-presentation should be not good for everyone, but particularly for those low self-esteem individuals in terms of decreased life satisfaction and for those high self-esteem ones in terms of reduced positive feedback.

As well, H4 was partially supported. Honest self-presentation was found to be linked to high sense of positive online feedback despite the levels of self-esteem of participants, particularly for those high self-esteem individuals, but linked with more life satisfaction only for those low self-esteem ones.

These results seemed to partially support the hypothesis of “rich get richer”: because college students who have good social behavior are inclined to present more self-information on SNSs, and receive more positive feedback through honest self-presentation (Kraut et al., 2002 ; Reer & Krämer, 2017 ). Now it is widely believed that people who are more sociable make better use of the opportunities provided by SNSs to strengthen their social ties (Ross et al., 2009 ). In addition, individuals with low self-esteem usually have cognitive bias of rejecting information: they not only show less defense against negative information, but also accept more negative information (Zhou et al., 2018 ), which may discount the positive association of their honest self-presentation with perceived positive feedback to some degree. By contrast, people with high self-esteem play an active part in interpersonal communication (Sampthirao, 2016 ). Therefore, individuals with high self-esteem are more likely to get social feedback after their honest self-presentation on SNSs than their counterparts. Even so, more honest self-presentation on SNSs still brought significantly more positive feedback for those low self-esteem individuals.

However, the result about life satisfaction was not the case in that only low self-esteem college students benefited from their honest self-presentation while their counterparts did not. There may be several reasons. On the one side, according to the self-discrepancy theory (Higgins, 1987 ), when those low self-esteem individuals honestly present themselves, they will experience small discrepancies between their true self-states and ideal self-states and less negative emotions, which helps improve their life satisfaction. More than that, they can be more clear about their self-concepts regarding social anxiety (Orr & Moscovitch, 2015 ) and honest self-disclosure which enables them to express themselves and buffer negative feelings (Kim & Dindia, 2011 ). On the other side, low self-esteem may be seen as an act of sincerity when low self-esteem individuals presenting more honest self on SNSs, which will help them receive the audience’s likes (Schlenker, 1980 ), which will further result in a noticeable rise in life satisfaction. By contrast, people with high self-esteem are very clear about and believe in themselves and their selves are relatively free of influence of external social appraisal (Wood & Forest, 2016 ). Moreover, because self-esteem acts as a “buffer”, people with high self-esteem do not fluctuate greatly in terms of emotional adaptation (Arndt & Goldenberg, 2002 ). Thus, high self-esteem individuals always perceive more stable and higher life satisfaction than their counterparts, as found in the present study. It is still important to point out that, however, low self-esteem individuals seemed to benefit more from honest self-presentation on SNSs in terms of increased life satisfaction.

Limitations and future directions

There still are some limitations in the current study. First, in line with previous research (An et al., 2020 ; Liu & Brown, 2014 ), we focused on the overall frequency of positive feedback without distinguishing the roles of different specific positive feedback (e.g., likes, positive comments, or caring emojis). Future research could further explore the different associations of more specific types of positive online feedback with people’s well-being. Second, we did not sufficiently address the possible impact of SNS usage time and the number of SNS friends on the current results. Future studies should consider them as control variables to obtain more comprehensive and convincing findings. Third, we used a series of self-report questionnaires which may yield inaccurate measures because participants’ answers on some items are easily affected by social desirability. Therefore, future research should take other methods into account, such as evaluations by others and content analysis of SNSs accounts of participants to better understand the association of different strategies of self-presentation of college students’ life satisfaction and to improve the findings’ ecological validity. Fourth, we used a cross-sectional design which can not draw any causal conclusion. Future designs could benefit by implementing experimental manipulations that directly facilitate participants’ interactions on SNSs to test more causal models between self-presentation and life satisfaction. Finally, we used a small sample coming from only one university in China which limited its representativeness and the generalizability of the findings. Future studies should recruit a more diverse sample to provide new evidence.

Conclusions and implications

The current study provides valuable information by highlighting the positive role of honest self-presentation and the strength of positive online feedback for everyone on perceived life satisfaction, as well as the important moderating effect of self-esteem. We found that different strategies of self-presentation on SNSs was differently linked to college students’ life satisfaction and positive online feedback was a crucial mediator in such relationships. We also found that honest rather than positive self-presentation on SNSs was conducive to life satisfaction, particularly for those low self-esteem ones.

These findings have potential practical implications as well. First, the findings suggest that honest rather than positive self-presentation would be a better choice for anybody on SNSs to improve life satisfaction. They are particularly instructive for individuals in a special period, such as a home isolation period for the prevention of COVID-19, who reduce real-world social connections and turn to Internet for happiness. Second, they may be especially meaningful for individuals who are in low self-esteem because presenting more honest selves will particularly benefit their life satisfaction. Certainly, promoting self-esteem should have more fundamental benefits for happiness.

Authors' contributions

Lumei Tian constructed and designed this study, and substantially modified the manuscript. Pengyan Dai and Ruonan Zhai critically modified the manuscript as well. Jieling Cui collected the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors approved the final version to be published.

This research was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (No. 16BSH103).

Data availability

Code availability, declarations.

Approval was obtained from the ethics committee of Shandong Normal University. The procedures used in this study conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

The authors consented to the submission of the manuscript to the journal.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

  • Aljasir S, Bajnaid A, Elyas T, Alnawasrah M. Themes of Facebook status updates and levels of online disclosure: The case of university students. International Journal of Business Administration. 2017; 8 (7):80–97. doi: 10.5430/ijba.v8n7p80. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • An, R., Jiang, Y., & Bai, X. (2020). The relationship between adolescent social network use and loneliness: Multiple mediators of online positive feedback and positive emotions (in Chinese). Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology , 28 (4), 824–833. 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.04.036
  • Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2002). From threat to sweat: The role of physiological arousal in the motivation to maintain self-esteem. In A. Tesser & D. A. Stapel, & J. V. Wood (Eds.), Self and motivation : Emerging psychological perspectives (pp. 43–69) . American Psychological Association. 10.1037/10448-002
  • Bareket-Bojmel L, Moran S, Shahar G. Strategic self-presentation on Facebook: Personal motives and audience response to online behavior. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016; 55 :788–795. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.033. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baumeister RF, Tice DM, Hutton DG. Self-presentational motivations and personality differences in self-esteem. Journal of Personality. 1989; 57 (2):547–579. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb02384.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bazarova, N. N., Choi, Y. H., Schwanda Sosik, V., Cosley, D., & Whitlock, J. (2015). Social sharing of emotions on Facebook: Channel differences, satisfaction, and replies. In proceedings of the 18th ACM conference on computer supported cooperative work & social computing (pp. 154–164). ACM Press. 10.1145/2675133.267529
  • Bermack, B. (2014). Interpersonal relationships and life satisfaction among information technology professionals (Publication No.3644069) [Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global.
  • Bij de Vaate AJD, Veldhuis J, Konijn EA. How online self-presentation affects well-being and body image: A systematic review. Telematics and Informatics. 2019; 47 :101361. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2019.101316. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brailovskaia J, Margraf J. I present myself and have a lot of Facebook-friends-Am I a happy narcissist!? Personality and Individual Differences. 2019; 148 :11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.022. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brailovskaia J, Bierhoff HW, Rohmann E, Raeder F, Margraf J. The relationship between narcissism, intensity of Facebook use, Facebook flow and Facebook addiction. Addictive Behaviors Reports. 2020; 11 :1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100265. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brashers DE, Neidig JL, Goldsmith DJ. Social support and the management of uncertainty for people living with HIV or AIDS. Health Communication. 2004; 16 (3):305–331. doi: 10.1207/S15327027HC1603_3. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cameron JJ, Holmes JG, Vorauer JD. When self-disclosure goes awry: Negative consequences of revealing personal failures for lower self-esteem individuals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2009; 45 (1):217–222. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.009. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carson SH, Langer EJ. Mindfulness and self-acceptance. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy. 2006; 24 (1):29–43. doi: 10.1007/s10942-006-0022-5. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chai, H., Chu, X., Niu, G., Sun, X., Lian, S., & Yao, L. (2018). Self-disclosure on social networking sites and adolescents’ life satisfaction: A moderated mediation model (in Chinese). Journal of Psychological Science, 41 (5), 81–87. 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20180512
  • Chan, T. K. H. (2021). “Does self-disclosure on social networking sites enhance well-being? The role of social anxiety, online disinhibition, and psychological stress”, In Z. W. Y. Lee, T. K. H. Chan, & C. M. K. Cheung (Eds.), Information Technology in Organisations and Societies: Multidisciplinary Perspectives from AI to Technostress (pp. 175–202) . Emerald Publishing. 10.1108/978-1-83909-812-320211007
  • Chen L, Gong T, Kosinski M, Stillwell D, Davidson RL. Building a profile of subjective well-being for social media users. PLoS ONE. 2017; 12 (11):e0187278. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187278. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • D’agata MT, Holden RR. Self-concealment and perfectionistic self-presentation in concealment of psychache and suicide ideation. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018; 125 :56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.034. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diener E, Colvin CR, Pavot WG, Allman A. The psychic costs of intense positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1991; 61 (3):492–503. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.492. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diener E, Lucas RE, Oishi S. Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and life satisfaction. In: Snyder CR, Lopez SJ, editors. The handbook of positive psychology. Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 63–73. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duan W, He C, Tang X. Why do people browse and post on WeChat moments? Relationships among fear of missing out, strategic self-presentation, and online social anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2020; 23 (10):708–714. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0654. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods. 2009; 41 (4):1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forest AL, Wood JV. When social networking is not working: Individuals with low self-esteem recognize but do not reap the benefits of self-disclosure on Facebook. Psychological Science. 2012; 23 (3):295–302. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429709. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goffman E. The presentation of self in everyday life. Doubleday; 1959. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greene, K., Derlega, V. J., & Mathews, A. (2006). Self-Disclosure in Personal Relationships. In A. L. Vangelist & D. Perlman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships (pp. 409–428). Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/cbo9780511606632.023
  • Greitemeyer T, Mügge DO, Bollermann I. Having responsive Facebook friends affects the satisfaction of psychological needs more than having many Facebook friends. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 2014; 36 (3):252–258. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2014.900619. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grieve R, Watkinson J. The psychological benefits of being authentic on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016; 19 (7):420–425. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0010. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grieve R, March E, Watkinson J. Inauthentic self-presentation on Facebook as a function of vulnerable narcissism and lower self-esteem. Computers in Human Behavior. 2020; 102 :144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.08.020. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & Demetrovics, Z. (2014). Social networking addiction: An overview of preliminary findings. In K. P. Rosenberg & L. Curtiss Feder (Eds.), Behavioral addictions: Criteria, evidence, and treatment (pp. 119–141). Elsevier Academic Press. 10.1016/B978-0-12-407724-9.00006-9
  • Haber M, Cohen J, Lucas T, Baltes B. The relationship between self-reported received and perceived social support: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2007; 39 :133–144. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9100-9. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hampton KN, Lu W. Beyond the power of networks: Differentiating network structure from social media affordances for perceived social support. New Media & Society. 2015; 19 (6):861–879. doi: 10.1177/1461444815621514. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis : A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.
  • Heng S, Zhou Z, Lei Y, Niu G. The impact of actual-ideal self-discrepancies on adolescents' game addiction: The serial mediation of avatar identification and flow experience (in Chinese) Studies of Psychology and Behavior. 2018; 16 (2):111–118. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1672-0628.2018.02.016. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins ET. Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review. 1987; 94 (3):319–340. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.319. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Indian M, Grieve R. When Facebook is easier than face-to-face: Social support derived from Facebook in socially anxious individuals. Personality and Individual Differences. 2014; 59 :102–106. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.016. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jackson CA, Luchner AF. Self-presentation mediates the relationship between Self-criticism and emotional response to Instagram feedback. Personality and Individual Differences. 2017; 133 :1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.052. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jang W, Bucy EP, Cho J. Self-esteem moderates the influence of self-presentation style on Facebook users' sense of subjective well-being. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018; 85 :190–199. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.03.044. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jian RR, Li S. Seeking help from weak ties through mediated channels: Integrating self-presentation and norm violation to compliance. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018; 87 :121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.05.022. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jiang Y, Bai X, Qi S, Wu Y. The effect of adolescents’ social networking on social anxiety: The mediating role of online positive feedback and self-esteem (in Chinese) Chinese Journal of Special Education. 2019; 8 :76–81. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1007-3728.2019.08.013. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jin B, Pena JF. Mobile communication in romantic relationships: Mobile phone use, relational uncertainty, love, commitment, and attachment styles. Communication Reports. 2010; 23 (1):39–51. doi: 10.1080/08934211003598742. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kashdan TB, Dewall CN, Masten CL, Pond RS, Jr, Powell C, Combs D, Schurtz DR, Farmer AS. Who is most vulnerable to social rejection? The toxic combination of low self-esteem and lack of negative emotion differentiation on neural responses to rejection. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9 :e90651. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090651. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kereste G, Tulhofer A. Adolescents’ online social network use and life satisfaction: A latent growth curve modeling approach. Computers in Human Behavior. 2019; 104 :106–187. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.106187. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim Y, Baek YM. When is selective self-presentation effective? An investigation of the moderation effects of “self-esteem” and “social trust” Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2014; 17 (11):697–701. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0321. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim J, Dindia K. Online self-disclosure: A review of research. In: Wright KB, Webb LM, editors. Computer-mediated communication in personal relationships. Peter Lang Publishing; 2011. pp. 156–180. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim J, Lee JR. The Facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking. 2011; 14 (6):359–364. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0374. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ko HC, Kuo FY. Can blogging enhance subjective well-being through self-disclosure? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2009; 12 (1):75–79. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0163. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kong F, You X. Loneliness and self-esteem as mediators between social support and life satisfaction in late adolescence. Social Indicators Research. 2013; 110 :271–279. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9930-6. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kraut R, Kiesler S, Boneva B, Cummings J, Helgeson V, Crawford A. Internet paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues. 2002; 58 (1):49–74. doi: 10.1111/1540-4560.00248. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Social networking sites and addiction: Ten lessons learned. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017; 14 (3):311–328. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lebacq T, Dujeu M, Méroc E, Moreau N, Pedroni C, Godin I, Castetbon K. Perceived social support from teachers and classmates does not moderate the inverse association between body mass index and health-related quality of life in adolescents. Quality of Life Research. 2019; 28 (4):895–905. doi: 10.1007/s11136-018-2079-x. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee DKL, Borah P. Self-presentation on Instagram and friendship development among young adults: A moderated mediation model of media richness, perceived functionality, and openness. Computers in Human Behavior. 2020; 103 :57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.017. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee G, Lee J, Kwon S. Use of social-networking sites and subjective well-being: A study in South Korea. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking. 2011; 14 (3):151–155. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0382. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee E, Kim YJ, Ahn J. How do people use Facebook features to manage social capital? Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 36 :440–445. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.007. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Li X, Jiang Y, Zhang B. The influence of loneliness on problematic mobile social networks usage for adolescents: The role of interpersonal distress and positive self presentation (in Chinese) Journal of Psychological Science. 2018; 41 (5):1117–1123. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20180514. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Li S, Coduto KD, Song C. Comments vs. One-click reactions: Seeking and perceiving social support on social network sites. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media. 2020; 64 (5):777–793. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2020.1848181. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lin R, Utz S. Self-disclosure on SNS: Do disclosure intimacy and narrativity influence interpersonal closeness and social attraction? Computers in Human Behavior. 2017; 70 :426–436. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.012. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu D, Brown BB. Self-disclosure on social networking sites, positive feedback, and social capital among Chinese college students. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 38 :213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.003. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu Q, Sun X, Zhou Z, Niu G, Kong F, Lian S. The effect of honest self-presentation in online social network sites on life satisfaction: The chain mediating role of online positive feedback and general self-concept (in Chinese) Journal of Psychological Science. 2016; 39 (2):406–411. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20160223. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luo M, Hancock J. Self-disclosure and social media: Motivations, mechanisms and psychological well-being. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019; 31 :110–115. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.08.019. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis . Taylor & Francis Group.
  • Maddux JE. Subjective well-being and life satisfaction: An introduction to conceptions, theories, and measures. In: Maddux JE, editor. Subjective well-being and life satisfaction. Routledge; 2018. pp. 3–31. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Manago AM. Media and the development of identity. In: Scott R, Kosslyn S, editors. Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource. John Wiley & Sons; 2015. pp. 1–14. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meeus A, Beullens K, Eggermont S. Like me (please?): Connecting online self-presentation to pre- and early adolescents’ self-esteem. New Media & Society. 2019; 21 (3):1–18. doi: 10.1177/1461444819847447. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Metzler A, Scheithauer H. The long-term benefits of positive self-presentation via profile pictures, number of friends and the initiation of relationships on Facebook for adolescents’ self-esteem and the initiation of offline relationships. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017; 8 :1–15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01981. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Metzler A, Scheithauer H. Association of self-presentational strategies on Facebook and positive feedback in adolescence - A two-study approach. International Journal of Developmental Sciences. 2018; 12 (3):1–18. doi: 10.3233/DEV-180246. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michikyan M, Subrahmanyam K, Dennis J. Can you tell who I am? Neuroticism, extraversion, and online self-presentation among young adults. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 33 :179–183. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.010. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nabi RL, Prestin A, So J. Facebook friends with (health) benefits? Exploring social network site use and perceptions of social support, stress, and well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2013; 16 (10):721–727. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0521. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nadkarni A, Hofmann SG. Why do people use Facebook? Personality and Individual Differences. 2012; 52 (3):243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Neuliep JW. The relationship among intercultural communication apprehension, ethnocentrism, uncertainty reduction, and communication satisfaction during initial intercultural interaction: An extension of anxiety and uncertainty management (AUM) Theory. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research. 2012; 41 (1):1–16. doi: 10.1080/17475759.2011.623239. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Niu G, Bao N, Zhou Z, Fan C, Kong F, Sun X. The impact of self-presentation in online social network sites on life satisfaction: The effect of positive affect and social support (in Chinese) Psychological Development and Education. 2015; 31 (5):563–570. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2015.05.07. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oh HJ, Ozkaya E, LaRose R. How does online social networking enhance life satisfaction? The relationships among online supportive interaction, affect, perceived social support, sense of community, and life satisfaction. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 30 :69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.053. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ollier-Malaterre A, Rothbard NP, Berg JM. When worlds collide in cyberspace: How boundary work in online social networks impacts professional relationships. Academy of Management Review. 2013; 38 (4):645–669. doi: 10.5465/amr.2011.0235. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orr EMJ, Moscovitch DA. Blending in at the cost of losing oneself: Dishonest self-disclosure erodes self-concept clarity in social anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2015; 6 (3):278–296. doi: 10.5127/jep.044914. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Palermiti AL, Servidio R, Bartolo MG, Costabile A. Cyberbullying and self-esteem: An Italian study. Computers in Human Behavior. 2017; 69 :136–141. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.026. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pang H. Microblogging, friendship maintenance, and life satisfaction among university students: The mediatory role of online self-disclosure. Telematics and Informatics. 2018; 35 (8):2232–2241. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2018.08.009. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pew Research Center. (2018). Social media use in 2018 . https://www.pewinternet.org/2018/05/31/teens-social-media-technology-2018/
  • Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008; 40 :879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Proctor CL, Linley PA, Maltby J. Youth life satisfaction: A review of the literature. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2009; 10 (5):583–630. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reer F, Krämer NC. The connection between introversion/extroversion and social capital outcomes of playing world of warcraft. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking. 2017; 20 (2):97–103. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0439. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reinecke L, Trepte S. Authenticity and well-being on social network sites: A two-wave longitudinal study on the effects of online authenticity and the positivity bias in SNS communication. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 30 :95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.030. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton University Press; 1965. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ross C, Orr ES, Sisic M, Arseneault JM, Simmering MG, Orr RR. Personality and motivations associated with Facebook use. Computers in Human Behavior. 2009; 25 (2):578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.024. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist. 2000; 55 (1):68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sampthirao, P. (2016). Self-concept and interpersonal communication. The International Journal of Indian Psychology, 3 (3). 10.25215/0303.115
  • Satici SA, Uysal R. Well-being and problematic Facebook use. Computers in Human Behavior. 2015; 49 :185–190. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.005. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schlenker BR, Leary MR. Social anxiety and self-presentation: A conceptualization model. Psychological Bulletin. 1982; 92 (3):641–669. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.92.3.641. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept, social identity, and interpersonal relations . Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.
  • Schlosser AE. Self-disclosure versus self-presentation on social media. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2020; 31 :1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.025. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scissors, L., Burke, M., & Wengrovitz, S. (2016, February). What’s in a Like? Attitudes and behaviors around receiving likes on Facebook. In Proceedings of the 19th ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing—CSCW’16 (pp. 1499–1508). ACM Press. 10.1145/2818048.2820066
  • Selfhout MH, Branje SJ, Delsing M, Bogt TF, Meeus WH. Different types of Internet use, depression, and social anxiety: The role of perceived friendship quality. Journal of Adolescence. 2009; 32 (4):819–833. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.011. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seo M, Kim J, Yang H. Frequent interaction and fast feedback predict perceived social support: Using crawled and self-reported data of Facebook users. Journal of Computer Communication. 2016; 21 :282–297. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12160. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shahyad S, Besharat MA, Asadi M, Alipour AS, Miri M. The relation of attachment and perceived social support with life satisfaction: Structural equation model. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2011; 15 :952–956. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.03.219. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Snodgrass JG, Bagwell A, Patry JM, Dengah HJFI, Smarr-Foster C, Van Oostenburg M, Lacy MG. The partial truths of compensatory and poor-get-poorer internet use theories: More highly involved videogame players experience greater psychosocial benefits. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018; 78 :10–25. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.26701.13286. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sosik VS, Bazarova NN. Relational maintenance on social network sites: How Facebook communication predicts relational escalation. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 35 :124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.044. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sun X, Chai H, Niu H, Cui X, Lian S, Tian Y. The effect of self-disclosure on social networking site on adolescents’ loneliness: A moderated mediation model (in Chinese) Psychological Development and Education. 2017; 33 (4):477–486. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.04.12. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sung Y, Lee JA, Kim E, Choi SM. Why we post selfies: Understanding motivations for posting pictures of oneself. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016; 97 :260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.032. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor DA. The development of interpersonal relationships: Social penetration processes. The Journal of Social Psychology. 1968; 75 (1):79–90. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1968.9712476. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor DA, Altman I. Communication in interpersonal relationships: Social penetration processes. In: Roloff ME, Miller GR, editors. Interpersonal processes: New directions in communication research. Sage Publications Inc; 1987. pp. 257–277. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tazghini S, Siedlecki KL. A mixed method approach to examining Facebook use and its relationship to self-esteem. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013; 29 (3):827–832. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.010. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tian L. Shortcoming and merits of Chinese version of Rosenberg (1965) self-esteem scale (in Chinese) Psychological Exploration. 2006; 26 (2):88–91. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1003-5184.2006.02.020. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Toma CL. Feeling better but doing worse: Effects of Facebook self-presentation on implicit self-esteem and cognitive task performance. Media Psychology. 2013; 16 (2):199–220. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2012.762189. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Twomey C, O'Reilly G. Associations of self-presentation on Facebook with mental health and personality variables: A systematic review. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2017; 20 (10):587–595. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0247. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tyler JM, Adams KE, Kearns P. Self-presentation and subjective well-being. In: Maddux JE, editor. Subjective well-being and life satisfaction. Routledge; 2018. pp. 355–391. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang SS. “I share, therefore I am”: Personality traits, life satisfaction, and Facebook check-ins. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2013; 16 (12):870–877. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0395. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang Y, Shi S. Preparation for “life satisfaction scales applicable to college students (CSLSS)” (in Chinese) Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medical Science. 2003; 12 :199–201. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wenninger, H., Krasnova, H., & Buxmann, P. (2014, October). Activity matters: Investigating the influence of Facebook on life satisfaction of teenage users [Paper presentation]. ECIS 2014 Proceedings-22nd European Conference on Information Systems, Tel Aviv, Israel.
  • Wise K, Alhabash S, Park H. Emotional responses during social information seeking on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2010; 13 (5):555–562. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0365. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wohn DY, Larose R. Effects of loneliness and differential usage of Facebook on college adjustment of first-year students. Computers & Education. 2014; 76 (7):158–167. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.03.018. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wohn DY, Carr CT, Hayes RA. How affective is a "like"?: The effect of paralinguistic digital affordances on perceived social support. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016; 19 (9):562–566. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0162. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wood JV, Forest AL. Self-protective yet self-defeating: The paradox of low self-esteem people's self-disclosures. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 2016; 53 :131–188. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.10.001. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wright EJ, White KM, Obst PL. Facebook false self-presentation behaviors and negative mental health. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2018; 21 (1):40–49. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0647. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Xie W. Social network site use, mobile personal talk and social capital among teenagers. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014; 41 :228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.042. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang C, Brown BB. Online self-presentation on Facebook and self development during the college transition. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2016; 45 (2):402–416. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0385-y. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang Y, Liu Y, Jiang Z, Mo J, Lin L. Negative affect and life satisfaction mediate the association between negative life events and suicidal ideation in college students. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2020; 26 (6):692–700. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2020.1861637. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang, C. (2014). It makes me feel good: A longitudinal, mixed-methods study on college freshmen’s Facebook self-presentation and self development (Publication No. 3620548) [Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin–Madison]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global.
  • Zell AL, Moeller L. Are you happy for me… on Facebook? The potential importance of “likes” and comments. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018; 78 :26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.050. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhou J, Li X, Tian L, Scott HE. Longitudinal association between low self-esteem and depression in early adolescents: The role of rejection sensitivity and loneliness. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 2018; 93 :54–71. doi: 10.1111/papt.12207. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ziegele M, Reinecke L. No place for negative emotions? The effects of message valence, communication channel, and social distance on users’ willingness to respond to SNS status. Computers in Human Behavior. 2017; 75 :704–713. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.016. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

online self presentation

How to video record yourself presenting a PowerPoint

online self presentation

What type of content do you primarily create?

Recording yourself presenting a PowerPoint can be challenging, especially getting the technical aspects right. You need to capture your screen, webcam, and audio seamlessly while delivering your content naturally and avoiding awkward transitions between slides.

Finding a tool that integrates all these components can seem daunting. However, recording yourself presenting a PowerPoint doesn't have to be reserved for the tech-savvy or professional speakers.

Whether you're a teacher creating online lessons, a marketer producing a product demo, or someone looking to improve their presentation skills, this guide will walk you through user-friendly tools and strategies that will teach you how to video record yourself presenting a PowerPoint.

  • Recording yourself presenting a PowerPoint can be challenging, but it doesn't have to be reserved for experts.
  • Video recording offers unlimited reach, repurposing opportunities, and a more engaging experience.
  • Use PowerPoint's built-in recording feature or tools like Descript for a polished presentation.
  • Other tools like Zoom, Loom, OBS Studio, Clipchamp, and Quick Time Player can also be used for recording.
  • Tips include practicing your script, using slide notes, ensuring high-quality audio and lighting, and incorporating storytelling techniques.

Why video record yourself presenting a PowerPoint? 

Video recording your PowerPoint presentations offers several advantages:

  • Unlimited reach : Unlike in-person presentations confined to a specific time and location, recorded videos allow your message to reach audiences globally at their convenience.
  • Repurposing opportunities: Recorded presentations can be repurposed as evergreen content across multiple platforms, such as social media, online courses, webinars, and internal training materials, maximizing their value.
  • Personal and engaging : Recording yourself narrating a presentation adds a personal touch and dynamic element that static slides alone cannot replicate. Your viewers can see and hear you deliver the content, creating a more immersive and engaging experience.

How to record your presentation with the PowerPoint app

Built-in recording feature in powerpoint.

1. Open the PowerPoint slide you want to record. 

2. Click on the Record tab of the top toolbar.

3. Click on the Record button and choose from any of the following options:

  • From Current Slide
  • From Beginning

4. You'll see the PowerPoint screen recording window.

5. Select the microphone and camera from the icon on the top-right.

6. Click the Stop and Replay buttons to end or replay your recording.

7. To export your presentation's recording, click Export to Video within the Recording tab.

Benefits : Easy to use, no external tools needed

Limitations : Limited editing capabilities.

How to make a more polished recording of your presentation with Descript 

While PowerPoint's built-in recording features are helpful, they may not produce the refined result you want for your presentation. That's where Descript comes in handy. 

It’s a whole production studio at your fingertips, with video recording, transcription, and editing capabilities, ready to transform dull PowerPoint slides into an engaging experience for your audience. Descript also has a built-in screen recorder.

You can use Descript on Windows, Mac, or even straight from your web browser. 

Here's step-by-step guide to recording your PowerPoint presentation with Descript.

Set up your screen recording

1. Install and open Descript. At the top of the editor, click and open the Record panel. 

2. Select Screen.

3. Set Recorder settings:

Adjust the default recording settings to your liking, including the transcription language, the maximum recording resolution, and more. 

4. Set recording options. Choose which audio and video sources to record (e.g., microphone, computer audio, camera). Remember to turn the camera on to capture your talking head video alongside your PowerPoint presentation.

5. Click the Additional Settings icon for

  • Studio Sound (Optional): Enhance audio quality with noise reduction and background removal
  • Transcription (Optional): Enable automatic transcription for easier editing
  • Recording separate audio tracks

6. Add (optional) Speaker labels for each audio track (e.g., "Narrator," "Expert") by typing them in the text box next to your audio inputs. 

Capture your screen recording

1. Select the right recording option:

  • Recording into script : Choose this if you haven’t recorded anything yet.
  • Replace selection : Choose this if you already have a script and want to replace a specific section with a new recording.
  • Record new layer : Pick this for recording your video as a new layer on top of an existing recording.

2. Click Record at the bottom of the Record panel.

3. Drag over the area of your screen you wish to record. Click Start Recording. Press Space to start recording the full screen.

4. Click Stop to finish recording

Bonus: Descript creates separate tracks for camera, microphone, and computer audio. You can edit each track individually for more precise control.

  • Do a short test recording to ensure everything is set up correctly.
  • Set your recording to transibe for easier editing.

Editing and polishing your PowerPoint recording in Descript

Descript's AI video editing tool makes it a breeze to trim, cut, and splice your video footage, ensuring a seamless flow from start to finish. You can add smooth transitions and other visual effects to make your presentation pop.

Here’s a glimpse of how to edit and polish your PowerPoint recording in Descript:

Edit your recording like a doc

Did you make a mistake during a slide transition? No problem. Just delete those sections in the text transcript (created automatically by Descript) and the video will adjust to match.

Remove filler words

Sound smoother and more confident by easily removing filler words like "um" and "uh" from the transcript.

Fix voiceover mistakes by typing

Didn't nail your delivery on a specific slide? No need to start from scratch! Descript's Overdub feature lets you simply type in the correct narration and replace the audio on that slide with AI voice cloning.

Boost video quality and engagement : 

  • Crisp up audio quality by reducing background noise and room reverb with the AI-powered Studio Sound feature.
  • ‎Replace your background completely with the Green Screen Video Editor —look like you're presenting in a sleek studio or broadcasting from a beach.
  • Enable Eye Contact simulation with AI, so it appears you're making direct eye contact with viewers, even if you’re looking or reading something off-camera.

Other ways to video record yourself presenting a PowerPoint 

Use Zoom’s meeting recording feature to record yourself giving a PowerPoint presentation. Start a Zoom meeting with yourself, share your PowerPoint window, and enable recording

Loom is a handy screen recording tool that also offers a webcam overlay. Launch the Loom app or Chrome extension and set it to record your screen and webcam. Expect high-quality recordings but no advanced editing features. The free plan offers only five minutes of recording at a stretch.

3. OBS Studio

OBS Studio is a free, open-source screen recorder/streaming software with a steeper learning curve. It’s popular among advanced or tech-savvy users who need granular customization and control.

To record your presentation, add a "Screen Capture" source to capture your PowerPoint window and a "Video Capture Device" source for your webcam. 

4. Clipchamp: Recording natively in Windows

You can use Microsoft’s Clipchamp , the built-in screen recorder and video editor for Windows, to simultaneously capture your computer screen, webcam, and audio for recording your PowerPoint presentation. It’s also available as a browser-based app. You can record up to 30 minutes on screen and webcam recordings.

Users can also adjust the screen and webcam recordings in the editing timeline separately. 

5. Quick Time Player Recording natively in Mac

Use Mac’s Quick Time Player to record your PowerPoint presentation. It’s not as straightforward as the other options on this list, so here’s a quick guide to help you out:

  • Launch Quick Time Player.
  • Select File and go to New Movie Recording.
  • You'll be recording your entire screen in addition to recording your face, so adjust the size and location of the recording window so that it's in a nonintrusive corner of your PowerPoint.
  • Go to View and select Float on Top .
  • Next, go to File and select New Screen Recording.
  • On the menu that appears, select Record Selected Portion and drag a border around your PowerPoint and the webcam recording window.
  • Press Record and start presenting.
  • You can adjust the video quality and the audio source in the webcam recording window.

How to turn your PowerPoint presentation slides into a video 

Did you know PowerPoint lets you save your presentation as a video, too? 

Here's a breakdown of the two ways you can do so—keeping all the presentation elements (narration, animation, pointer movements, timings, and so on) intact in the presentation itself.  

Save your PowerPoint presentation as a video

This creates a separate video file (MP4 or WMV) that anyone can play, even without PowerPoint. 

Follow these steps:

  • Save your presentation
  • Go to File > Export > Create a Video
  • Ultra HD (4K): Best for large screens (if you have Windows 10+)
  • Full HD (1080p): Great for computers and TVs
  • HD (720p): Good for streaming online or on DVDs
  • Standard (480p): Smallest size, good for phones

4. Decide on narration

  • If you haven't recorded yourself talking, choose "Don't Use Recorded Timings and Narrations" from the dropdown menu
  • If you have a recording, choose "Use Recorded Timings and Narrations." You can also set how long each slide shows for (default is 5 seconds)

5. Click Create Video

6. Name your video, pick a folder to save it in, and choose a file type (MPEG-4 or Windows Media Video)

Creating the video might take a while, especially for longer presentations. You can even leave it running overnight. Once done, find your video in the chosen folder and double-click to play it.

Save your PowerPoint presentation as a slideshow

This saves your presentation as a special file (PPSX) that starts playing automatically on full screen when opened. It only works with PowerPoint.

Here’s how it works:

  • Make sure your presentation is saved (regular PowerPoint file)
  • Go to File > Save As .
  • Pick a folder to save the slideshow in.
  • Under Save as type, choose PowerPoint Show (*.ppsx) .
  • Click Save .

Now when someone opens the file, it will automatically play the slideshow.

Pro tips for making better video PowerPoint presentations 

Practice your script.

Recording a flawless PowerPoint video rarely happens in one take. The key is practice.

Record practice run-throughs and watch them back. Make notes on areas that need improvement or parts lacking clarity. The more you drill your script, the more confident and natural you'll sound in the final recording.

Take advantage of PowerPoint's built-in Speaker Coach . The feature shows you whether your pace is too fast or slow, your use of filler words like "um," and suggestions to improve your voice modulation.

Use slide notes for coherent delivery

Wouldn’t a personal teleprompter make presentations so much easier? That’s what slide notes are for.

Having your thoughts and talking points organized within your PowerPoint file lets you have all crucial information and prompts readily available when recording.

Go ahead and add detailed notes or even a full script for each slide. This will help you stay on track and provide a handy transcript reference if you need to do any editing or voiceover work in post-production.

Use high-quality audio equipment or turn on Studio Sound

Poor audio recording quality is a surefire way to make even the most visually polished video feel amateurish. Do your presentation justice by investing in a decent external or Bluetooth microphone and audio interface to capture clear voiceover audio.

If you must use a built-in mic, record in a quiet environment to minimize background noise pickup. Or better yet, pair up with an AI-powered audio enhancement tool like Descript's Studio Sound , which reduces background noise , reverb, and other artifacts.

Ensure a clean background and even lighting

Ditch cluttered backgrounds. You want people to focus on the content of your PowerPoint and not be distracted by the funny shapes and colors in your video. 

Your video lighting and framing matters, too. Position yourself facing a natural light source or bring supplemental video lighting to eliminate weird shadows and squinting-level glare.

Look directly into the camera or turn on Eye Contact

Looking directly into the camera is essential to create a sense of connection with the audience. Position your webcam or camera at eye level and imagine you're talking to a friend. If you're using Descript, enable the Eye Contact feature to auto-adjust your gaze towards the camera.

  • Dress professionally and maintain a good posture to project confidence and authority.
  • Use visuals, animations, and multimedia elements to enhance your presentation and keep your audience engaged.
  • Consider adding closed captions or subtitles to your video for better accessibility and engagement, especially for those watching without sound.
  • Speak clearly and at a moderate pace, using vocal variety to emphasize key points and maintain interest.
  • Incorporate storytelling techniques, real-life examples, or personal anecdotes to make your video presentation more relatable and memorable .

Take your PowerPoint presentations from amateur to amazing 

Recording yourself virtually presenting a PowerPoint allows you to distribute your message using the most engaging type of content: video. And there are many screen recording tools out there that can capture your screen and web camera footage with just a few clicks. 

But how do you stand out?

By focusing on the presentation's depth, your delivery, and the video's quality. The first two are the bare minimum to communicate your message effectively. 

But the right screen capture software and all-in-one video creation tool like Descript can help you with third. Use the tool to improve the quality of your recorded PPT presentations. 

With Descript, it's easier than ever to record your screen, polish up the audio, and create stunning visuals all in one place. So why wait? 

Sign up for Descript today and simplify your workflow while creating a PowerPoint presentation video that sticks with your audience in the long term.

How do you make a presentation video with your face?

To create a presentation video with your face, follow these steps:

  • Open your presentation in PowerPoint.
  • Go to the Slide Show tab.
  • Click on Record and choose From Current Slide or From Beginning .
  • Choose your microphone and camera by clicking on a sound icon on the top-right.
  • Start recording by clicking on the record button.
  • Speak into your microphone to narrate the presentation.
  • Your webcam will capture your face during the recording.
  • Click Stop when you finish recording.
  • Export your recorded presentation by clicking on File> Export>Create a video.

How do you present yourself in PowerPoint?

To present yourself effectively in PowerPoint, follow these steps:

  • Determine the key information to include in your presentation.
  • Design and prepare your PowerPoint slides with a cohesive theme.
  • Craft a compelling introduction to engage your audience.
  • Tailor your presentation to different time constraints for flexibility.
  • Capture the audience's attention before you begin speaking.

Related articles

online self presentation

Featured articles:

10 excellent Loom alternatives to record your screen

Looking for the best ways to record your screen without Loom? Find out 10 Loom alternatives, their features, and pricing.

online self presentation

How to record a YouTube video: A guide for all devices

If you want to record a YouTube video for personal use or fair-use repurposing, here are the steps to get started.

online self presentation

For Business

Social media video marketing: Everything you need to know

Learn how to win over potential customers (and the algorithms) with the right social media video marketing tools, strategy, and optimizations.

online self presentation

How to record a podcast on Zoom: A beginner’s guide

Follow this step-by-step guide on how to record a podcast on Zoom, then learn about why it may not be your best recording option.

online self presentation

How to record a Microsoft Teams meeting: 5 easy ways

Learn how to record a Microsoft Teams meeting on any device and get answers to common problems.

online self presentation

10 effective how-to videos to engage your audience

How-to videos are a great way to educate your audience. Whether you’re recording a software demo or training instructions, here’s how to create a how-to video.

online self presentation

Articles you might find interesting

online self presentation

10 proven ways to make money on TikTok in 2024

From built-in funds offered by the platform to other creative techniques, learn more about how to monetize TikTok and grow your following.

online self presentation

The 15 most popular podcasts right now

Explore the most popular podcasts of the moment. Discover trending topics and captivating hosts to find your next favorite podcast.

online self presentation

Product Updates

Descript now supports remote recording in 4K

Your podcast and video recordings are about to get stunningly, beautifully clear: SquadCast by Descript now supports 4K video recording. Paid Descript subscribers can now record in crystal-clear 4K quality.

online self presentation

Social media content creation: A 2024 guide

Want to build up your brand’s social media presence? Learn how to create a social media content creation strategy that will engage and entertain your audience.

New in Descript: Um detection, search, and more

Descript 3.1 is now available. Here’s what’s new:

online self presentation

42 million minutes: what millions of Descript creators did in 2023

What more than millions of Descript creators did in 2023.

Join millions of creators who already have a head start.

Get free recording and editing tips, and resources delivered to your inbox.

Related articles:

Share this article

How to use Google Slides, Google's free slideshow presentation maker

  • Google Slides is Google's slideshow presentation program that allows real time collaboration.
  • Google Slides is part of the Google Workspace suite, which also includes Google Docs and Gmail.
  • Google Slides differs from Microsoft PowerPoint in its simplicity and collaboration options.

Insider Today

Google Slides is a presentation program that's part of Google Workspace, a group of productivity apps that also includes Gmail, Google Sheets, Goole Docs, Google Meet , and more. Workspace has more than 3 billion users worldwide. 

With Google Slides, users can create, present, and collaborate via online presentations from various devices. You can present during Google Meet calls directly from Slides and embed charts from Google Sheets. You can also add YouTube videos to Slides presentations. 

Google recently announced plans to add artificial intelligence features like its Gemini AI tool to its Workspace programs, which include Slides. Users will be able to use Gemini to create images or written content for slides, or even reference other files in their Drives or emails in their Gmail accounts.

What is Google Slides? 

Google Slides is a cloud-based presentation program that's part of the Google Workspace. Google Slides can be used to create and deliver presentations online. 

Several different themes are available in Slides for designing presentations. Users can customize Slides presentations in a variety of colors and styles. You can add photos, videos from YouTube, charts from Google Sheets , and information from many other sources. Different members of a team can contribute and collaborate on the presentation in real time. 

There's no specific limit on how many slides you can add to your Google Slides presentation, but there is a 100 MB file size limit.

How to download Google Slides 

To access Google Slides, visit slides.google.com . 

Related stories

You can also open Slides while Gmail or Google Chrome is open by clicking on the Google Apps icon in the upper-right corner (shown as three rows of dots) and selecting Slides. 

Another option is to download the Google Slides app for your Apple or Android device. Search for Google Slides in the Apple App Store or Google Play Store.

What templates are available? 

Dozens of Google Slides templates are available, depending on your needs. For instance, there are general presentation templates, photography portfolios, pitch decks, case studies, science fair projects, and more. 

To browse the templates available, open Google Slides. Then, click Template Gallery in the upper-right corner. Scroll through the options, choose the one that meets your needs, and start creating a presentation. 

What's the difference between Google Slides and PowerPoint? 

Both Google Slides and PowerPoint are presentation programs. Google Slides is a program within Google Workspace, and PowerPoint is a Microsoft program. PowerPoint is an offline program, while Slides is online which allows for real time collaboration.

The programs share many features that allow for presentation creation and delivery, but PowerPoint may offer more advanced design features. 

You can convert Google Slides into PowerPoint presentations, and vice versa. From the top menu in Slides, click File, Download, and choose Microsoft PowerPoint. 

How to learn to use Google Slides 

Through Google Workspace, you can access several quick-start guides, cheat sheets, and troubleshooting resources to help you learn to use Google Slides. There are also many YouTube videos with tutorials for using Slides.

On February 28, Axel Springer, Business Insider's parent company, joined 31 other media groups and filed a $2.3 billion suit against Google in Dutch court, alleging losses suffered due to the company's advertising practices.

online self presentation

  • Main content

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

ONLINE SELF–PRESENTATION FROM THE CYBERPSYCHOLOGY PERSPECTIVE

Profile image of Elena  Stănculescu

2011, »eLearning and Software for Education« (eLSE)

The aim of this paper was to explore a new psychological concept: online self- presentation. There are differences between self – presentation in offline environments and online settings, because the last one could be revised before making it available in the cyberspace. Topics from social psychology (social identity theory, social cognition,), self – presentation and impression management approach, and personality differences (social skills and shyness) are discussed. In all settings (online and offline) individuals try to manage the impression made to other people, but in webspace, very little is done spontaneously because of overcontrolled self-presentation. Online self – presentation could contain self – serving bias due to the positive self-regard and cognitive consistency need. Tendency to create a very different self image in online settings than real self could represent a compensatory mechanism for low self-confidence, social skills, subjective well-being, shyness, and social anxiety.

Related Papers

Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking

Melanie Keep (nee Nguyen)

online self presentation

Journal of Internet Social Networking and Virtual Communities

Jamel-Eddine Gharbi

Self-Presentation on Facebook

Faryal Sohail

Self-presentation, is a crucial phenomenon for identity development. It leads people to think in a particular way and it is intended to maintain, manipulate and adjust an image of oneself. Drawn on the theory of self-presentation of Goffman (1959), the purpose of this study is to examine the relation between self-presentation, idealized version of self, judgment anxiety and supportive feedback. Researcher opted for quantitative approach. Survey of 50 respondents was conducted, from social sciences departments of Bahaddudin Zakariya University. Pearson’s correlation and regression analysis was applied to analyze the correlations between variables. The results indicated that there was a significant relation found between self-presentation and idealized version of self. Males are more involved in self-presentation. A significant correlation was found between self-presentation and other two variable of narcissism and judgment anxiety. On the other hand, no significant relation was found for supportive feedback, diminishing of communication barrier and self-presentation. In conclusion, results indicated that idealized version of self, judgment anxiety and narcissism are highly linked with self-presentation.

e-Journal of New Media

Osman Solmaz

This study aimed to lay out an up-to-date literature review on self-presentation and impression management (Goffman, 1959) in social networking sites (SNSs) through a descriptive analysis method. Following an introduction to the concepts, and the significance of self-presentation research, the current state of the discourse has been discussed under four themes: the debate of actual versus idealized selves in SNSs, resources for self-presentation in SNSs, online self-presentation typology and strategies, and determinants of online self-presentation. The review indicated that impression management typology was found to be a useful analytical framework for future research. However, it was reported that users employed various self-presentation tactics to create a favorable impression on others as well. It was also revealed that personality traits, technical features of SNSs, audience size and diversity, culture, and other-provided information were among the determinants of self-presentation. Finally, it was shown that self-presentation in SNSs merits focused attention as more research is needed to gain a solid understanding of to what extent actual selves are presented online. The study concluded with a call for further research in the investigation of the presentation of self in educational settings including second language teaching and learning contexts.

Dayanara Dawn Bustillos

IJAR Indexing

This study investigates self-presentation strategies among Face book Participants, exploring how participants manage their online presentation of self in order to accomplish the goal of presenting the impression they desire. Thirty-four individuals participated in semi-structured interviews about their Face book experiences and perceptions of each other profile and were asked to describe the impression being projected and give a adjective to it. This helped in capturing the impression being ?given? and ?impression being received? by others. Qualitative data analysis suggests that participants attended to small cues online, mediated the tension between impression management pressures and the desire to present an authentic sense of self through tactics such as creating a profile that reflected their \"ideal self,\" and attempted to establish the veracity of their identity claims. This study provides empirical support for Social Information Processing theory in a naturalistic context.

Computers in Human Behavior

Tom Buchanan

Symposium on Cocking and van den Hoven’s "Evil Online"

Carissa Véliz

Kim Hua Tan , Husnita Habsah

Facebook is the most visited social network site in Malaysia and is popularly used by Malaysian tertiary level students. Its increasing popularity has led to the rise of issues highlighting the effects of social media on today's youths. Inappropriate use of the site could negatively affect youths, society, and the nation as a whole. Context collapse, undefined audience, and limited physical cues within " nonymous 1 " environment pose challenges to Facebook users. Despite the applications afforded by Facebook such as photos, videos, and audios, interpretation of self-presentation by the audience mostly relies on the textual content. Just as in face-to-face communication where individuals are being judged by their physical attributes and speech, Facebook users are being assessed through their writing and language. Thus, this research seeks to investigate how tertiary level students present themselves online by emphasising on language as a tool for self-presentation and impression management (SPIM). The question of how language is employed as a tool of SPIM was taken up through content analysis of 441 Facebook status updates supported by a linguistic software programme – LIWC. The analyses revealed linguistic aspects of which interpretations of SPIM are manifested in the messages. Expressive and assertive speech acts followed by positive emotion words were primarily used, suggesting that students strategically used emotions as a means of SPIM. The findings demonstrate how youths express themselves in social media which could address implications for online self-presentation. The findings also add an important dimension to the existing theory and research in language and computer-mediated communication. Keywords 1 the opposite of " anonymous "

Catherine Podgornaya , Bondareva Evgeniya , Chistyakova Galina

The aim of the research is gender analysis of communication strategies and tactics by which the Internet users of online dating sites create free-text (without multiple choice option) self-presentations. The data for the study are the profiles made up by Russian and English-speaking men and women aged 18 to 60. The data are analyzed by means of three methods: descriptive, comparative and quantitative. The analysis results show five universal speech strategies: “Self-characterization”, “Addressee’s characterization”, “Purpose of searching”, “Representing an ideal situation”, “Orientation to communication”, which are verbalized in the self-presentations of both Russian- and English-speaking men and women in online dating sites. Each speech strategy has the certain set of communication and speech tactics. The article also presents the percentage of each speech strategy in all the factual units collected. Furthermore, the authors point out two commonly used speech strategies: “Self-characterization” and “Addressee’s characterization” Also the authors conclude that there are no fundamental differences in choosing men’s or women’s specific speech strategies, while gender differences in choosing communication and speech tactics are found.

RELATED PAPERS

Pan American Journal of Medical Thermology

Cezar Schadeck

Biophysical Journal

Ajay Mahalka

Encontros Teológicos

Felipe Sérgio Koller

Skin Appendage Disorders

Sonia Chavez Alvarez

Gastroenterology

Tabata Souza

Rui Ligeiro

Aquatic Toxicology

Bruno Carrijo Carneiro

Nature genetics

Jianfeng Xu

1比1定做uq学位证书 昆士兰大学毕业证电子版学位证书毕业证认证原版一模一样

Zenodo (CERN European Organization for Nuclear Research)

Yazachew Genet Ejigu

Mahsa Tashakor

Jacques Mudry

Henri Capron

INTEGRAR E INOVAR SABERES PARA A DEMOCRATIZAÇÃO DO CONHECIMENTO

Isaiane Rocha Bezerra

The journal of physical chemistry letters

Aditya Pandey

The American Journal of Pathology

Sử Học Nguyễn

Journal of Psychodidactics

Josu Perales

Verhandlungen

Martin Thoms

Spanish journal of palaeontology

Bienvenido Martínez-Navarro

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024
  • Open access
  • Published: 21 May 2024

Effectively teaching cultural competence in a pre-professional healthcare curriculum

  • Karen R. Bottenfield 1 ,
  • Maura A. Kelley 2 ,
  • Shelby Ferebee 3 ,
  • Andrew N. Best 1 ,
  • David Flynn 2 &
  • Theresa A. Davies 1 , 2  

BMC Medical Education volume  24 , Article number:  553 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

221 Accesses

Metrics details

There has been research documenting the rising numbers of racial and ethnic minority groups in the United States. With this rise, there is increasing concern over the health disparities that often affect these populations. Attention has turned to how clinicians can improve health outcomes and how the need exists to educate healthcare professionals on the practice of cultural competence. Here we present one successful approach for teaching cultural competence in the healthcare curriculum with the development of an educational session on cultural competence consisting of case-based, role-play exercises, class group discussions, online discussion boards, and a lecture PowerPoint presentation.

Cultural competence sessions were delivered in a pre-dental master’s program to 178 students between 2017 and 2020. From 2017 to 2019, the sessions were implemented as in-person, case-based, role-play exercises. In 2020, due to in-person limitations caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, students were asked to read the role-play cases and provide a reflection response using the online Blackboard Learn discussion board platform. Evaluation of each session was performed using post-session survey data.

Self-reported results from 2017 to 2020 revealed that the role-play exercises improved participant’s understanding of components of cultural competence such as communication in patient encounters (95%), building rapport with patients (94%), improving patient interview skills (95%), and recognition of students own cultural biases when working with patients (93%).

Conclusions

Students were able to expand their cultural awareness and humility after completion of both iterations of the course session from 2017 to 2019 and 2020. This session can be an effective method for training healthcare professionals on cultural competence.

Peer Review reports

It is projected that by the year 2050, racial and ethnic minority groups will make up over 50% of the United States population [ 1 ]. With a more multicultural society, growing concern has emerged over how to address the health disparities that effect these populations and the ways in which healthcare professionals can increase positive health outcomes. Continuing evidence suggests that many patients from racial and ethnic minority groups are not satisfied with the current state of healthcare which has been attributed to implicit bias on the part of physicians and current challenges faced by practitioners who feel underprepared to address these issues due to differences in language, financial status, and healthcare practice [ 2 , 3 , 4 ].

To contend with health disparities and the challenges faced by practitioners working with a more diverse population, healthcare educators have begun to emphasize the importance of educating healthcare workforce on the practice of cultural competence and developing a skilled-based set of behaviors, attitudes and policies that effectively provides care in the wake of cross-cultural situations and differences [ 4 , 5 , 6 ]. There are several curricular mandates from both medical and dental accreditation bodies to address this issue [ 7 , 8 , 9 ], and large amounts of resources, ideas, and frameworks that exist for implementing and training future and current healthcare providers on the inadequacies of the healthcare system and cultural competence [ 10 , 11 , 12 ]. These current institutional guidelines for accreditation and the numerous amounts of resources for training cultural competence, continue to evolve with work documenting the need for blended curriculum that is continuous throughout student education, starting early as we have done here with pre-dental students, including in-person didactic or online sessions, a service learning component, community engagement and a reflective component [ 4 , 5 , 13 , 14 ].

This study investigates teaching cultural competence in a healthcare curriculum. We hypothesized that early educational exposure to cultural competence through role playing case studies, can serve as an effective mechanism for training early pre-doctoral students the practice of cultural competence. Utilizing student self-reported survey data conducted in a predental master’s curriculum, in which two iterations of role-playing case studies were used to teach components of cultural competence, this study aims to evaluate and support research that suggests role-playing case studies as effective means for educating future clinical professionals on the practice of cultural competence.

This study was determined to be exempt by the Institutional Review Board of Boston University Medical Campus, Protocol # H-37,232. Informed consent was received from all subjects.

Data collection

The role-playing, case-based simulated patient encounter exercises were developed and administered at Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine to predental students in the Master of Science in Oral Health Sciences Program (see Table  1 ). From 2017 to 2020, we administered patient encounter cases [see Additional File 1 ] to students ( n  = 178) in the program as a portion of a case-based, role-playing exercise to teach the importance of cultural competence and cultural awareness during patient encounters. During years 2017–2019, real actors portrayed the patient and physician. In 2020, the session was conducted online via a discussion board through a Blackboard Course Site. The original case was published as part of a master’s students thesis work in 2021 [ 15 ].

Description of patient encounter cases 1 and 2

Patient Encounter Case 1 [see Additional file 1 ] is composed of two subsections, scenario 1 A and scenario 1B, and is centered around a patient/physician interaction in which a patient who is pregnant presents with pain upon urination. The physician in 1 A is short and terse with the patient, immediately looking at a urine sample, prescribing medication for a urinary tract infection, and telling the patient to return for a follow-up in 2 weeks. In scenario 1B, a similar situation ensues; however, in this scenario the physician takes more time with the patient providing similar care as the physician in 1 A, but asking for more information about the patients personal and medical history. At the conclusion of the scenario, the patient is offered resources for an obstetrician and a dentist based on the information that is provided about the patient’s background. The patient is then sent on their way and asked to follow-up in 2 weeks. The patient does not return.

Patient Encounter Case 2 [see Additional file 1 ] follows a similar format to the Patient Encounter Case 1. In scenario 2 A, the same patient from Case 1 returns with tooth pain after giving birth. The physician in 2 A, like 1 A, is short with the patient and quickly refers the patient to a dentist. In 2B, the physician again takes more time with the patient to receive background information on the patient, make a connection, and provides an antibiotic and dental referral.

Each Patient Encounter Case explored topics such as the importance of building a trusting physician/patient relationship, the importance of asking a patient for patient history, making a connection, and the importance of a physician taking all facets of a patient’s circumstances into consideration [ 15 ].

Session outline

The sessions conducted between 2017 and 2019 were composed of three parts: (1) enactment of an abridged patient encounter facilitated by session administrators, (2) group discussion and reflection during which time students were asked to critically reflect and discuss the theme and key take-aways from the role play exercise, and (3) a PowerPoint presentation emphasizing take-away points from the role-play exercise. At the conclusion of the cultural competence training sessions, students participated in a post-session Qualtrics generated survey administered electronically to assess each student’s feelings about the session [see Additional file 3 ].

Role-play enactment

Facilitators dressed-up in clothing to mimic both the physician and patient for all case scenarios in Patient Encounter Case 1 and Case 2. At the conclusion of the role play portion of each of the cases, the facilitators paused to lead students in a real-time class group discussion. After Case 1, students were asked questions such as: What did you think ? Were the patient’s needs met? Did you expect the patient to return? Following Case 2, similar questions were asked by the facilitators, including: What did you think ? Were the patient’s needs met? Did you expect the patient to accept help?

At the conclusion of this portion of the session, the facilitators led a larger general discussion about both cases and how they related to one another. Finally, the course session concluded with a PowerPoint presentation that reinforced the take-home points from the session [see Additional file 2 ] [ 15 ].

Change in session modality due to COVID-19 pandemic

In Fall 2020, due to the COVID-19 pandemic, the course modality moved to an online platform and consisted of three parts on a Blackboard Discussion Board (Blackboard, Inc.). Students were required to: (1) read each of the Patient Encounter Cases and add a brief reflection comparing the scenarios, (2) then comment on at least two peer’s posts in the discussion forum and (3) attend class to hear a PowerPoint presentation by a course session facilitator on the key take-aways from each scenario [ 15 ].

Student surveys

At the conclusion of the cultural competence training sessions, students participated in a post-session Qualtrics ( https://www.qualtrics.com ) generated survey administered electronically to assess each student’s feelings about the sessions [see Additional file 3 ]. The format of the survey included 5 questions with the following Likert scale response options: strongly agree, agree, disagree, strongly disagree. These post-session surveys were not required but rather optional [ 15 ].

A total of 178 students completed the cultural competence sessions between 2017 and 2020. Of these participants, 112 voluntarily completed a post-session survey on the effectiveness of the course in teaching cultural competence and cultural awareness during patient encounters. Between 2017 and 2019, 99 students completed post-session surveys following sessions with role play exercises. In 2020, 13 students completed post-session surveys following discussion board sessions.

Role-play exercises enhanced cultural competence

In responding to post-session survey questions following cultural competence sessions that included role-play exercises (2017–2019), 71% of students surveyed strongly agreed and 24% agreed that the role-play exercises helped them to identify the importance of communication in patient encounters. In asking participants if the role-play exercises made them more aware of different strategies to improve their patient interview skills, 72% strongly agreed and 23% agreed. Also, 68% of the students strongly agreed and 26% agreed that the exercises helped them to better identify the importance of building rapport and trust during patient encounters. When asked if the exercises helped the students to better understand their own bias and/or cultural awareness when working with patients, the results of the survey showed that 62% of students strongly agreed and 31% agreed with this statement. In addition, most students found the role-play exercises to be enjoyable (72% strongly agreed and 22% agreed). See results shown in Fig.  1 .

figure 1

Cultural Competence Session Survey Data from the Year 2017–2019. Survey data from students at Boston University’s Oral Health Sciences Program for the years 2017–2019. Data is presented as percent of respondents ( n  = 99)

Discussion boards and reflections enhanced cultural competence

Cultural competence sessions held during 2020 did not include role-play exercises due to the Covid-19 pandemic. Instead, students participated in discussion boards and reflections on Blackboard. In response to the post-session survey question asking if the discussion board exercises were helpful in identifying the importance of communication during patient encounters, 67% of students strongly agreed and 25% agreed with this statement. Also, 75% of students strongly agreed and 17% agreed that the discussion board exercises helped them identify the importance of building rapport and trust during patient contact. When asked if the exercises helped the students to better understand their own bias and/or cultural awareness when working with patients, the results of the survey showed that 67% of students strongly agreed and 25% agreed with this statement. In addition, most students found the discussion board exercises to be enjoyable (67% strongly agreed and 22% agreed). See results shown in Fig.  2 .

figure 2

Cultural competence session survey data from the Year 2020. Survey data from students at Boston University’s Oral Health Sciences Program for the year 2020. Data is presented as percent of respondents ( n  = 13)

Student responses to the reflection portion of the online cultural competency sessions were recorded and categorized. Five themes were selected and 441 reflection responses were coded using NVivo (Version 12). The results showed that 29% of reflections demonstrated student’s ability to understand a holistic approach to clinical care, 24.3% understood the importance of collecting a patient history, 6.8% recognized the socioeconomic factors during a patient encounter, 27.9% reflected on the importance of the patient clinical relationship, and 12% on the effects on improving health outcomes (Table  1 ). Representative student responses to these themes are shown in Table  1 .

There exists a need to develop novel and effective means for teaching and training the next generation of healthcare professionals the practice of cultural competence. Thus, two iterations of a course session using case-based patient centered encounters were developed to teach these skills to pre-professional dentals students. Overall, the results of this study demonstrated that participation in the course, subsequent group discussion sessions, and take-away PowerPoint sessions significantly improved the participant’s understanding of the importance of communication skills and understanding of socioeconomic, environmental, and cultural disparities that can affect a patient’s health outcome.

According to results from the course session implemented in-person from 2017 to 2019, the role-playing exercise significantly improved participants understanding of important components that can be used to improve health outcomes that may be affected due to health disparities. Students were strongly able to identify the importance of communication in patient encounters, to understand strategies such as communication and compassionate care in patient encounters, identify the importance of building a patient-physician relationship with patients, and were able to recognize their own cultural biases. Similarly, in 2020, even with a change in course modality to on-line learning due to COVID-19, students were able to understand the same key take-aways from the course session as demonstrated by reflections using the discussion board regarding the need for a holistic approach to care, importance of the patient clinician relationship, and importance of taking a patient history. Despite promising implications of both iterations of the session, students completing the session online did not find the same success in “understanding my own bias/and or cultural awareness when working with patients.” This decrease may be attributed to change in course modality and the strengths of the role-play enactment of the patient encounter. It is important to recognize that additional learning components, including video recordings of the role-play enactment, may be necessary if the discussion board is used as the primary learning method in the future.

In contrast to previous studies that attempted to determine the effectiveness of cultural competence training methods, this session had many unique characteristics. The simulated role-playing exercise enabled student participants to see first-hand an interactive patient scenario that could be used as an example for when students begin working with patients or communicating with patients who are culturally diverse. Additionally, the nature of the cases created for the course session which were divided into a part A in which the patient physician was more straightforward when diagnosing and treating the patient and a part B with a more comprehensive and nurturing approach to care, allowed the students to compare the scenarios and make their own assumptions and comments on the effectiveness of each portion of the case. Another strength of this training, was the faculty with cultural competence training were uniquely involved in case creation and facilitation of the course session. According to previous studies with similar aims, it was noted that direct observation and feedback from a faculty member who had cultural competence training and direct contact with patients can provide students with a more memorable and useful experience when educating students [ 12 ]. The facilitators of this session were able to emphasize from their own personal experiences how to work with culturally diverse populations.

An important aspect of the 2020 iteration of the course session in which a discussion board format was used, was that it allowed students who may feel uncomfortable with sharing their thoughts on a case and their own biases, the opportunity to share in a space that may feel safer than in person [ 4 ]. Previous studies have mentioned challenges with online discussion boards [ 4 ] but here we had robust participation, albeit required. Students often contributed more than the required number of comments and they were often lengthy and engaging when responding to peers. Finally, in contrast to previous studies, this course session took place in a pre-professional master’s program, the M.S. in Oral Health Sciences Program at Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine. This program, in which students are given the opportunity to enhance their credentials for professional school, provided students with early exposure to cultural competence training. Students that completed this session in their early pre-professional curriculum should be better prepared than peers who did not receive any cultural competence training until they entered their designated professional school. This session is part of an Evidence Based Dentistry course, which incorporates a larger component of personal reflection that serves to engage students in critical thinking as they begin to develop the skills to be future clinicians. Students that understand different cultures, society and themselves through self-assessments will grow and be best suited in time to treat future patients [ 4 , 16 , 17 ].

One limitation of the present study was the number of survey participants that competed the post-session surveys, as survey completion was not required. Thus, the number of student participants declined over the years, reaching its lowest number of participants in 2020 when the discussion board course session was implemented, and students may have been over surveyed due to the pandemic. Another limitation to this study, was the lack of both a pre and post survey that could be used to determine how student’s understanding of cultural competence had evolved from their entry into the course to the conclusion of the course as well as individual bias and self-reporting measures.

In the future, the course should implement both a role-playing format and subsequent discussion board reflections within the same course session. Studies have shown that alternatives ways of drawing students to reflect whether role play, personal narratives, etc. can be extremely advantageous in developing personal reflection and awareness building competency [ 4 , 16 , 17 , 18 ]. It is noted that role-playing exercises that allow students to provide feedback with student colleagues can provide students with more insight into their own behaviors. It has also been shown in previous studies that student writing and reflection activities can also facilitate student’s reflections on their own beliefs and biases [ 4 , 11 ]. Reflective writing skills are an important and effective means for students to continue to gauge their cultural competence throughout the remainder of their academic training and as future clinicians [ 4 , 17 , 19 ]. Further, students may experience emotional responses through the process of reflective writing as they recognize personal bias or stereotypes, creating a profound and impactful response resulting in enhanced understanding of cultural differences and beliefs [ 4 ]. By combining both learning techniques, students would be able to understand their own bias and their classmates and create a dialogue that could be more beneficial than just one learning method alone. Furthermore, by implementing the discussion board into the role-playing session, as stated previously, students that are more cautious about sharing their point of view or about their own implicit bias in a traditional classroom setting would be able to express their opinions and facilitate a more comprehensive discussion more thoroughly.

Here we show an effective means to utilize role-play of a multi-scenario case-based patient encounter to teach pre-professional healthcare student’s components of cultural competence, emphasizing the importance of provider-patient interactions, holistic patient care, and patient history and socioeconomic factors in provider care. This study contributes to the larger body of work that seeks to address this important aspect of education as it relates to enhancing patient health care outcomes.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Albino JEN, Inglehart MR, Tedesco LA. Dental education and changing oral health care needs: disparities and demands. J Dent Educ. 2012;76(1):75–88.

Article   Google Scholar  

Constantinou CS, Papageorgiou A, Samoutis G, McCrorie P. Acquire, apply, and activate knowledge: a pyramid model for teaching and integrating cultural competence in medical curricula. Patient Educ Couns. 2018;101(6):1147–51.

DallaPiazza M, Padilla-Register M, Dwarakanath M, Obamedo E, Hill J, Soto-Greene ML. Exploring racism and health: an intensive interactive session for medical students. MedEdPORTAL. 2018;14:10783.

Forsyth CJ, Irving MJ, Tennant M, Short SD, Gilroy JA. Teaching Cultural competence in Dental Education: a systematic review and exploration of implications for indigenous populations in Australia. J Dent Educ. 2017;81(8):956–68.

Betancourt JR. Cultural competence and medical education: many names, many perspectives, one goal. Acad Med. 2006;81(6):499–501.

Jernigan VBB, Hearod JB, Tran K, Norris KC, Buchwald D. An examination of cultural competence training in US medical education guided by the tool for assessing cultural competence training. J Health Disparities Res Pract. 2016;9(3):150–67.

Google Scholar  

Behar-Horenstein LS, Warren RC, Dodd VJ, Catalanotto FA. Addressing oral Health disparities Via Educational Foci on Cultural competence. Am J Public Health. 2017;107(S1):S18–23.

Lie D, Boker J, Cleveland E. Using the tool for assessing cultural competence training (TACCT) to measure faculty and medical student perceptions of cultural competence instruction in the first three years of the curriculum. Acad Med. 2006;81(6):557–64.

Holyfield LJ, Miller BH. A tool for assessing cultural competence training in dental education. J Dent Educ. 2013;77(8):990–7.

Vasquez Guzman CE, Sussman AL, Kano M, Getrich CM, Williams RL. A comparative case study analysis of cultural competence training at 15 U.S. medical schools. Acad Med. 2021;96(6):894–9.

Jernigan VB, Hearod JB, Tran K, Norris KC, Buchwald D. An examination of cultural competence training in US medical education guided by the tool for assessing cultural competence training. J Health Dispar Res Pract. 2016;9(3):150–67.

Kripalani S, Bussey-Jones J, Katz MG, Genao I. A prescription for cultural competence in medical education. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21(10):1116–20.

Mariño R, Satur J, Tuncer E, Tran M, Milford E, Tran VMTH, Tran PQ, Tsai RP. Cultural competence of Australian dental students. BMC Med Educ. 2021;21(1):155.

Beagan BL. Teaching social and cultural awareness to medical students: it’s all very nice to talk about it in theory, but ultimately it makes no difference. Acad Med. 2003;78(6):605–14.

Ferrebee S, Boston University School of Medicine Master’s Thesis. (2021). Effectively Teaching Cultural Competence in Healthcare Education. Available at Boston University Libraries: Open BU: https://open.bu.edu/handle/2144/43838 .

Crosson JC, Deng W, Brazeau C, Boyd L, Soto-Greene M. Evaluating the effect of cultural competency training on medical student attitudes. Fam Med. 2004;36(3):199–203.

Cathryn F, Michelle I, Short S, Tennant M, Gilroy J. Strengthening indigenous cultural competence in dentistry and oral health education: academic perspectives. Eur J Dent Educ. 2019;23(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/eje.12398

DasGupta S, Meyer D, Calero-Breckheimer A, Costley AW, Guillen S. Teaching cultural competency through narrative medicine: intersections of classroom and community. Teach Learn Med. 2006;18(1):14–7.

Woldt JL, Nenad MW. Reflective writing in dental education to improve critical thinking and learning: A systematic review. J Dent Educ. 2021;85(6):778–785. https://doi.org/10.1002/jdd.12561 . Epub 2021 Feb 11. PMID: 33576055.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge Boston University’s Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine’s Graduate Medical Science students and study participants.

No funding was used for the completion of this study.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Graduate Medical Sciences, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, 72 East Concord Street, L317, R-1017, Boston, MA, 02118, USA

Karen R. Bottenfield, Andrew N. Best & Theresa A. Davies

Department of Medical Sciences & Education, Boston University Chobanian & Avedisian School of Medicine, 72 East Concord Street, Boston, MA, 02118, USA

Maura A. Kelley, David Flynn & Theresa A. Davies

University of Maryland School of Dentistry, 650 W Baltimore Street, Baltimore, MD, 21201, USA

Shelby Ferebee

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

TAD designed the original study concept, taught the classes (roleplay), conducted the surveys, and collected data; MAK designed the original case and PowerPoint, and performed roleplay; DBF and SF evaluated data and drafted original figures; ANB assisted in drafting the manuscript; KRB finalized figures and the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Theresa A. Davies .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

This study was determined to be EXEMPT by the Institutional Review Board of Boston University Medical Campus, Protocol # H-37232.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Informed consent

Informed consent was received from all subjects.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Supplementary material 2, supplementary material 3, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Bottenfield, K.R., Kelley, M.A., Ferebee, S. et al. Effectively teaching cultural competence in a pre-professional healthcare curriculum. BMC Med Educ 24 , 553 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-024-05507-x

Download citation

Received : 27 October 2023

Accepted : 02 May 2024

Published : 21 May 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-024-05507-x

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Communication

BMC Medical Education

ISSN: 1472-6920

online self presentation

Online Self-Presentation: Balancing Privacy Concerns and Impression Construction on Social Networking Sites

  • First Online: 01 January 2011

Cite this chapter

online self presentation

  • Nicole C. Krämer 3 &
  • Nina Haferkamp 4  

24 Citations

Reaching the milestone figure of 500 million members in July 2010, the growth of the social networking site Facebook has rapidly accelerated. Currently, its membership figures would make it the third largest country in the world, suggesting that participation in online social networks has become more than a cursory phenomenon. Members of Facebook are required to create an individualized online profile that provides information about themselves, their physical appearance, individual tastes, and preferences (see Liu 2007; Liu et al. 2006), and that highlights certain aspects of their own personality. By means of these features, users inevitably construct and manage impressions of their self. Research has already shown that such a personal webpage even allows a more detailed self-presentation than a casual face-to-face interaction and that people indeed make use of it in order to emphasize certain aspects of their “true” self (Bargh et al. 2002; Haferkamp and Krämer 2010). Additionally, empirical findings indicate that social networking sites (SNSs) are not only a potential means for self-presentation but that people are indeed highly motivated to use this new arena for presenting themselves (Haferkamp and Krämer 2010). In doing this, they even adopt profile elements that have originally been provided for other purposes (e.g., people become a member of a group in order to display their attitudes and interests instead of in order to communicate with others, Haferkamp and Krämer 2009). This tendency might be due to the fundamental motive of every human being to present him/herself in a positive way and, in doing so, gain positive reactions from those forming an impression (Leary 1995; Leary and Kowalski 1990).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Archer JL (1980) Self-disclosure. In: Wegner D, Vallacher R (eds) The self in social psychology. Oxford University, London, pp 183–204

Google Scholar  

Bargh JA, McKenna KYA, Fitzsimons GM (2002) Can you see the real me? Activation and expression of the “true self” on the internet. J Soc Issues 58(1):33–48

Article   Google Scholar  

Barnes SB (2006) A privacy paradox: social networking in the United States. First Monday 11. www.firstmonday.org/issues/-issue11_9/barnes/index.html . Accessed 8 Aug 2008

Baumeister RF, Jones EE (1978) When self-presentation is constrained by the target’s prior knowledge: consistency and compensation. J Personal Soc Psychol 36:608–618

boyd dm, Ellison NB (2007) Social network sites: definition, history, and scholarship. J Comput Mediat Commun 13(1):210–230

Burgoon JK (1982) Privacy and communication. In: Burgoon M (ed) Communication yearbook 6. Sage, Beverly Hills, pp 206–249

Ellison N, Heino R, Gibbs J (2006) Managing impressions online: self-presentation processes in the online dating environment. J Comput Mediat Commun 11(2):415–441

Goffman E (1959) The presentation of self in everyday life. Double Day, Garden City

Gollwitzer PM (1986) Striving for specific identities: the social reality of self-symbolizing. In: Baumeister R (ed) Public self and private self. Springer, New York, pp 143–159

Gosling SD, Gaddis S, Vazire S (2007) Personality impressions based on facebook profiles. Paper presented at the international conference on weblogs and social media 2007, Boulder. http://www.icwsm.-org/papers/3-Gosling-Gaddis-Vazire.pdf . Accessed 2 Mar 2008

Haferkamp N, Krämer NC (2009) When I was your age, Pluto was a planet”: Impression Management and Need to belong as motives for joining groups on social networking sites. Paper presented at the annual meeting of ICA 2009 (International Communication Association), Chicago

Haferkamp N, Krämer NC (2010) Creating a digital self: impression management and impression formation on social networking sites. In: Drotner K, Schrøder KC (eds) Digital content creation: creativity, competence, critique. Peter Lang, New York, pp 129–149

Joinson AN (2001) Self-disclosure in computer-mediated communication: the role of self-awareness and visual anonymity. Eur J Soc Psychol 31:177–192

Jones EE (1964) Ingratiation. Appleton, New York

Jones EE, Pittman TS (1982) Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. In: Suls J (ed) Psychological perspectives on the self. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 231–262

Leary MR (1995) Self presentation: impression management and interpersonal behavior. Brown & Benchmark, Madison

Leary MR, Kowalski RM (1990) Impression management: a literature review and two-component model. Psychol Bull 107:34–47

Lenhart A, Madden M (2007) Teens, privacy & online social networks. Pew internet & American life project 2007. www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Teens_Privacy_SNS_Report_Final.pdf . Accessed 8 Aug 2008

Lewis K, Kaufman J, Christakis N (2008) The taste for privacy: an analysis of college student privacy settings in an online social network. J Comput Mediat Commun 14:79–100

Liu H (2007) Social network profiles as taste performances. J Comput-Mediat Commun 13(1). http://jcmc.indi-ana.edu/vol13/issue1/liu.html . Accessed 10 May 2007

Liu H, Maes PM, Davenport G (2006) Unraveling the taste fabric of social networks. Int J Semantic Web Inf Syst 2(1):42–71

Livingstone S (2008) Taking risky opportunities in youthful content creation: teenagers’ use of social networking sites for intimacy, privacy and self-expression. New Media Soc 10:339–411

Markus H, Nurius P (1986) Possible selves. Am Psychol 41:954–969

Mehdizadeh S (2010) Self-presentation 2.0: narcissism and self-esteem on Facebook. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 13(4):357–364

Reinecke L, Trepte S (2008) Privatsphäre 2.0: Konzepte von Privatheit, Intimsphäre und Werten im Umgang mit “user-generated-content” (Concepts of privacy, intimacy, and values with regard to “user-generated-content”). In: Zerfaß A, Welker M, Schmidt J (eds) Kommunikation, Partizipation und Wirkungen im Social Web. (Communication, participation, and effects in the social web) Band 1: Grundlagen und Methoden: Von der Gesellschaft zum Individuum. Halem Verlag, Köln, pp 205–228

Reingold H (1993) The virtual community. Addison-Wesley, New York

Schlenker BR (1980) Impression management: the self-concept, social identity, and interpersonal relations. Brooks/Cole, Monterey

Schneider DJ (1981) Tactical self-presentations: toward a broader conception. In: Tedeschi JT (ed) Impression management theory and social psychological research. Academic, New York, pp 23–40

Tidwell LS, Walther JB (2002) Computer-mediated communication effects on disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations. Getting to know one another a bit at a time. Hum Commun Res 28(3):317–348

Toma CL, Hancock J (2009) Self-presentation in online dating profiles: the role of physical attractiveness. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the international communication association, Chicago

Utz S, Krämer NC (2009) The privacy paradox on social network sites revisited: the role of individual characteristics and group norms. J Psychosoc Res Cyberspace. http://www.cyberpsychology.eu/view-.php?cisloclanku=2009111001 . Accessed 3 Mar 2010

Walther JB (1996) Computer-mediated communication: impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Commun Res 23:3–43

Walther JB, Van der Heide B, Kim SY, Westerman D, Tong ST (2008) The role of friends’ appearance and behavior and evaluations of individuals on Facebook: are we known by the company we keep? Hum Commun Res 34:28–49

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany

Nicole C. Krämer

Technical University of Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Nina Haferkamp

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicole C. Krämer .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

, Fachbereich Psychologie, Universität Hamburg, Von-Melle-Park 5, Hamburg, 20146, Germany

Sabine Trepte

Leonard Reinecke

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Krämer, N.C., Haferkamp, N. (2011). Online Self-Presentation: Balancing Privacy Concerns and Impression Construction on Social Networking Sites. In: Trepte, S., Reinecke, L. (eds) Privacy Online. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-21521-6_10

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-21521-6_10

Published : 19 June 2011

Publisher Name : Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

Print ISBN : 978-3-642-21520-9

Online ISBN : 978-3-642-21521-6

eBook Packages : Computer Science Computer Science (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

online self presentation

IMAGES

  1. Free Self-Presentation Template for PowerPoint and Google Slides

    online self presentation

  2. 5 Tools to Give an Online Presentation From Anywhere

    online self presentation

  3. Digital Self: Healthy Self Presentation and Positive Online Interaction

    online self presentation

  4. Online Self Presentation

    online self presentation

  5. Self Introduction PowerPoint Template

    online self presentation

  6. Self Introduction PowerPoint Presentation Template

    online self presentation

VIDEO

  1. Online Self Presentation

  2. How to give self-presentation?

  3. Self Presentation (First Impressions) (Bad)

  4. Pavlen Avramov Presentation Video

  5. Natalia Aleksandrova Presentation Video

  6. Merlin Hasanova Presentation Video

COMMENTS

  1. Self-Presentation in the Digital World

    Both self-presentation 1, and BAS and BIS 6, have been noted to show gender differences. In the real world, women have shown higher levels of BIS than men (at least, to this point in time ...

  2. Online self-presentation: Psychological predictors and outcomes

    Online self-presentation style has also been linked indirectly linked to cyberbullying behaviours. Using an ingenuine online self-presentation style is known to correlate with cyberbullying behaviours , and cyberbullies are known to use multiple online personas as a bullying strategy [8].

  3. The self presentation theory and how to present your best self

    Ask a trusted friend or mentor to share what you can improve. Asking for feedback about specific experiences, like a recent project or presentation, will make their suggestions more relevant and easier to implement. 2. Study people who have been successful in your role. Look at how they interact with other people.

  4. The Relationship Between Self-Concept and Online Self-Presentation in

    Self-presentation is the process by which we portray ourselves to others to make a desired impression. Traditionally, self-presentation has been studied in face-to-face interaction. However, social media platforms have shifted how people exchange information. For example, personal content can be edited before posting online, giving the individual more control over their presentation. With this ...

  5. How different online self-presentations relate to life satisfaction

    In the Internet age, some online factors, such as online self-presentation, related to life satisfaction have received much attention. However, it is unclear whether and how different strategies of online self-presentation are linked to an individual's life satisfaction differently. Accordingly, the present study examined the possible different relationships between different online self ...

  6. Frontiers

    Introduction. Self-presentation is the core concept of American sociologist Irving Goffman's Dramaturgy.As an individual's role-playing behavior of self-expression in interpersonal interaction, self-presentation provides an impetus for self-promotion in real life (Goffman, 1959).Western social psychologists have tested and revised the Goffman's theory (Jones and Nisbett, 1971), and the ...

  7. Managing Impressions Online: Self-Presentation Processes in the Online

    Self-Presentation and Self-Disclosure in Online and Offline Contexts. Self-presentation and self-disclosure processes are important aspects of relational development in offline settings (Taylor & Altman, 1987), especially in early stages. Goffman's work on self-presentation explicates the ways in which an individual may engage in strategic ...

  8. Online Self-presentation From the Cyber Psychology Perspective

    In general, online-self presentation via social media profiles, blog posts, etc., is much more controlled than self-presentation in offline environments, since the former can be edited and revised ...

  9. How online self-presentation affects well-being and body image: A

    Both online self-presentation and lurking can either be beneficial or harmful for well-being. Even tough results are mixed, in general, active self-presentation online seems to benefit (rather than diminish) all types of well-being, or leaves it unaffected. In contrast, lurking generally seems to decrease personal well-being.

  10. Full article: Self-transformation online through alternative

    An online self-presentation is the presentation of oneself via some digital, mediated platform; we refer to offline experiences as those that take place face-to-face (or in anticipation of face-to-face encounters) without electronic mediation. Because online platforms offer various degrees of plasticity, online self-presentations can take many ...

  11. The Presentation of Self Online

    This suits well my ideas about online self-presentation, confirming the complex interplay between technological affordances, individual actions, and the place of both in a cultural and social context. Giddens argues that self-identity is an aggregation of a person's experiences, an ongoing account, and a continuous integration of events.

  12. Filtering the I From the Ideal: Examining Preadolescents' Online Self

    More recently, scholars have started to draw attention to the potential transformative effects of online self-presentation, and the subsequent feedback to these efforts, on the beliefs individuals hold toward the self (Carr & Hayes, 2019; Valkenburg, 2017; Vogel & Rose, 2016).As such, it is argued that the personal content users choose to post online can be incorporated into the self-concept ...

  13. Adaption and psychometric evaluation of the Presentation of Online Self

    The Presentation of Online Self Scale (POSS) (Fullwood et al., 2016) is a self-report measure with 21 items divided into four factors: Item 1-9: Ideal Self (IS), Item 10-14: Multiple Selves (MS), Item 15-18: Consistent Self (CS), and Item 19-21: Online Presentation Preference (OPP). Each factor is scored on a 5-point Likert scale from ...

  14. The Online Self

    Abstract. This chapter considers how people create their online selves. It outlines how an online "self" could be an extension or replication of the offline self, but equally how an individual may create different versions of self online. It considers the online world as a heterogeneous environment that requires the involuntary creation of ...

  15. Online Self-Presentation on Facebook and Self Development During the

    Self-presentation, a central element of young people's identity development, now extends from face-to-face contexts to social networking sites. Online self-presentation may change when youth transition to college, faced with the need to reclaim or redefine themselves in the new environment. Drawing on theories of self-presentation and self development, this study explores changes in youth ...

  16. Online Self-Presentation in Adolescence

    Adolescence is the most prevalent age group that uses communication technologies, such as texting and social media. Adolescents are drawn to online communication because of its usefulness for addressing their developmental dilemmas. With communication technologies being ubiquitous in the lives of teens, self-presentation has become complex. The ...

  17. Online Self

    Online self - presentation could contain self - serving bias due to the positive self - regard and cognitive consistency need, and represent a compensatory mechanism for low self - confidence, social skills, subjective well - being, shyness, and social anxiety. The aim of this paper was to explore a new psychological concept: online self - presentation.

  18. PDF How different online self-presentations relate to life satisfaction

    Online self‑presentation and life satisfaction In terms of relationship management, the strategies of online self-presentation can be divided into two contrasting cat-egories (Kim & Lee, 2011): positive self-presentation and honest self-presentation. The former refers to selectively revealing or highlighting one's positive aspects in order to

  19. The Evolution of Online Self-Presentation

    This chapter examines factors influencing online self-presentation as it evolved from early personal webpages requiring considerable computer programming skills and often web hosting fees, to contemporary, ostensibly free to use, social media platforms which can be updated from almost any connected device in two keystrokes or a voice command.

  20. How different online self-presentations relate to life satisfaction

    Online self-presentation and life satisfaction. In terms of relationship management, the strategies of online self-presentation can be divided into two contrasting categories (Kim & Lee, 2011): positive self-presentation and honest self-presentation.The former refers to selectively revealing or highlighting one's positive aspects in order to create a good impression on SNSs.

  21. How to Video Record Yourself Presenting a PowerPoint: 7 Ways

    Record new layer: Pick this for recording your video as a new layer on top of an existing recording. 2. Click Record at the bottom of the Record panel. 3. Drag over the area of your screen you wish to record. Click Start Recording. Press Space to start recording the full screen. 4. Click Stop to finish recording.

  22. JTAER

    Previous studies have mainly focused on the impact of social media self-presentation on individuals' happiness and mental health, whereas few have attended to its potential influence on consumer behavior. A mixed-method empirical study with an online survey (N = 408) and an experimental study (N = 160) investigated the effects of social media self-presentation on individuals' conspicuous ...

  23. Women activists' strategies of online self-presentation

    Activists present themselves on- and offline using a diverse range of tools, discursive strategies, and means of self-presentation, all conducive to making both themselves and their cause well known. To that end, of paramount importance is their ability to make audiences and readers empathize, and a key factor all strategies have in common is the repetitive nature of their multiplatform ...

  24. How to use Google Slides, Google's free slideshow presentation maker

    To browse the templates available, open Google Slides. Then, click Template Gallery in the upper-right corner. Scroll through the options, choose the one that meets your needs, and start creating ...

  25. How to Start a Presentation: 12 Ways to Keep Your Audience Hooked

    1 Make a provocative statement. "I want to discuss with you this afternoonwhy you're going to fail to have a great career." One surefire way to get your audience's attention is to make a provocative statement that creates interest and a keen desire to know more about what you have to say. The presentation above, for example, does just that by ...

  26. Online Self Presentation From the Cyberpsychology Perspective

    Zarghooni (2007) introduced a new concept - detached self - presentation - that is a cognitive division of self - concept caused by the difference between the offline self and online self. The author has chosen these name to suggest that the self - presentation is realized in a detached or free from emotion state.

  27. Free Map Maker & Generator

    Free online map maker. Make a custom map. Easy to create and customize. Professionally designed and formatted. Millions of photos, icons and illustrations. Easily download or share. Design stunning custom maps with ease. No art background or design software necessary. Use Canva's free online map maker and create your maps you can add to ...

  28. Effectively teaching cultural competence in a pre-professional

    Self-reported results from 2017 to 2020 revealed that the role-play exercises improved participant's understanding of components of cultural competence such as communication in patient encounters (95%), building rapport with patients (94%), improving patient interview skills (95%), and recognition of students own cultural biases when working ...

  29. Online Self-Presentation: Balancing Privacy Concerns and ...

    Consequently, online self-presentation is constrained by technological boundaries allowing only limited flexibility with regard to online self-presentation. People can compose different forms of online self-presentation based on the quantity of information (more or less categories are visible) but they cannot change the quality of information ...

  30. Microsoft Build 2024: Create custom copilots from SharePoint

    Don't miss us this week at Microsoft Build in Seattle and online, May 21-23, 2024, to learn more and chat about creating copilots from SharePoint and other AI innovations we're bringing to our maker community. • KEY01: Opening keynote with Satya Nadella, Kevin Scott, Rajesh Jha, and me. • BRK144: Integrating your bots and Copilot ...